Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-wtssw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-14T15:22:01.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Introduction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Herbert L. Colston
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Parkside
Get access

Summary

Figurative language generally refers to speech where speakers mean something other than what they literally say. Consider a situation in which Harriet says to a friend, “My marriage is an icebox.” Most people recognize that Harriet intends to communicate something negative about her marriage, such as it is not emotionally affectionate or sexually passionate. How listeners arrive at these metaphorical meanings is a topic of considerable debate within both the humanities and cognitive science. One widely held belief is that metaphorical language must be more difficult to understand than plain, literal speech, precisely because metaphors, like all kinds of figurative language, are ornamental and deviate from literal thought. A standard way to assess this idea is to measure the amount of time it takes listeners, or readers, to comprehend figurative (in this case metaphorical) utterances compared to roughly equivalent literal speech.

The results of dozens of studies present a complex picture on this issue. Although, when seen in isolation, metaphorical utterances generally take longer to understand than literal ones, figurative speech can often be understood as quickly as literal speech when encountered in realistic discourse contexts (Gibbs, 1994, 2011 for reviews). This conclusion is especially true for more familiar, conventional figurative language, such as idioms (e.g., “kick the bucket”), stock metaphors (e.g., “John is a tiger”), conventional ironies (e.g., “A fine friend you are”), and certain indirect speech acts (e.g., “Can you pass the salt?”). Some scholars argue, nonetheless, that novel figurative language, such as creative metaphors, will always require additional cognitive effort to understand compared to that needed to process nonfigurative speech. The extra time needed to comprehend figurative language is often assumed to reflect initial processing of a word's (or an entire phrase's) literal or semantic meaning, with context having a guiding role later in the interpretation process.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Introduction
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz, Herbert L. Colston, University of Wisconsin, Parkside
  • Book: Interpreting Figurative Meaning
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168779.001
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Introduction
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz, Herbert L. Colston, University of Wisconsin, Parkside
  • Book: Interpreting Figurative Meaning
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168779.001
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Introduction
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz, Herbert L. Colston, University of Wisconsin, Parkside
  • Book: Interpreting Figurative Meaning
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168779.001
Available formats
×