Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-swr86 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T16:14:51.914Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The Last Barriers in High-Level L2 Speech

Morphosyntax in Focus

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 June 2018

Kenneth Hyltenstam
Affiliation:
Stockholms Universitet
Inge Bartning
Affiliation:
Stockholms Universitet
Lars Fant
Affiliation:
Stockholms Universitet
Get access

Summary

This chapter addresses the mastery of morphosyntax in L2 French in relation to the speakers’ command of formulaic sequences, information structure, low-frequent vocabulary and fluency. The studies underlying this chapter were all based on the InterFra corpus of oral production by highly proficient L2 speakers (university students and ‘cultural migrants’, and by native controls). Possibilities of nativelikeness according to a ‘pass-as-a-native’ perception test are presented, as well as a proposal for establishing differentiated high-level proficiency stages. A major finding is that highly proficient non-native speakers’ mastery of morphosyntax clearly differs from that of native speakers in cognitively challenging contexts. In addition, linguistic criteria are provided that discriminate between different highly proficient levels of L2 mastery and between L2 mastery and L1 acquisition. These criteria are: non-targetlike morphosyntactic forms, formulaic sequences and low-frequent vocabulary in contrast to fluency and information structure, which become nativelike.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsson, N. 2012. Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. High-level L2 acquisition, learning and use. Thematic issue of Studies in Second Language Acquisition 34/2: 187214.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. 2009. Age of L2 acquisition and degree of nativelikeness – listener perception vs linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning 58/3: 249306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ågren, M. 2008. A la Recherche de la Morphologie Silencieuse. Sur le Développement en Français L2 Écrit. Études Romanes de Lund 84. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Ågren, M. 2016. Apprendre l’orthographe profonde du français langue étrangère. Revue Française de Linguistique Appliquée XXI/2: 95108.Google Scholar
Bardel, C. & Gudmundson, A. 2018 (this volume, Chapter 5). Developing lexical complexity in oral production: limitations and possibilities of the advanced L2 learner, in Hyltenstam, K., Bartning, I. & Fant, L. (eds.), High-Level Language Proficiency in Second Language and Multilingual Contexts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. & Bofman, T. 1989. Attainment of syntactic and morphological accuracy by advanced language learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 11: 1734.Google Scholar
Bartning, I. 1997. L’apprenant dit avancé et son acquisition d’une langue étrangère. Tour d’horizon et esquisse d’une caractérisation de la variété avancée. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, (AILE) 9: 950.Google Scholar
Bartning, I. 2009. The advanced learner variety: 10 years later, in Labeau, E. & Myles, F. (eds.), The Advanced Learner Variety: The Case of French. Bern: Peter Lang. 1141.Google Scholar
Bartning, I. 2012. High-level proficiency in second language use: morphosyntax and discourse, in Watorek, M., Benazzo, S. & Hickman, M. (eds.), Comparative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. A Tribute to Clive Perdue. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 170188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartning, I. 2016. Morphosyntax and discourse at high levels of second language acquisition, in Hyltenstam, K. (ed.), Advanced Proficiency and Exceptional Ability in Second Languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 4371.Google Scholar
Bartning, I., Forsberg, F. & Hancock, V. 2009. Resources and obstacles in very advanced L2 French. Formulaic language, information structure and morphosyntax. EUROSLA Yearbook 9. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 185211.Google Scholar
Bartning, I., Forsberg Lundell, F. & Hancock, V. 2012. On the role of linguistic contextual factors for morphosyntactic stabilisation in high-level L2 French. High-level L2 acquisition, learning and use. Thematic issue of Studies in Second Language Acquisition 34/2: 243267.Google Scholar
Bartning, I. & Schlyter, S. 2004. Itinéraires acquisitionnels et stades de développement en français L2. Journal of French Language Studies 14/3: 281299.Google Scholar
Beeching, K. & Detges, U. (eds.) 2014. Discourse Functions at the Left and Right Periphery. Cross-Linguistic Investigations of Language Use and Language Change. Leiden/Boston: Brill.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. 1999. Introduction: whys and whys not of the critical period hypothesis for second language acquisition, in Birdsong, D. (ed.), Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. Mawhaw, NJ: L. Erlbaum. 122.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. 2003. Authenticité de prononciation en français L2 chez des apprenants tardifs anglophones: analyses segmentales et globales. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, (AILE) 18: 1736.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. 2005. Nativelikeness and non-nativelikeness in L2A research. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL) 43: 319328.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. 2007. Nativelike pronounciation among late learners of French as a second language, in Bohn, S.-O. & Munro, M. (eds.), Second Language Speech Learning: The Role of Language Experience in Speech Perception and Production. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 99116.Google Scholar
Bongaerts, T. 1999. Ultimate attainment in L2 pronunciation. The case of very advanced late L2 learners, in Birdsong, D. (ed.), Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. Mawhaw, NJ: L. Erlbaum. 133159.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. & Hopper, P. J. (eds.) 2001. Frequency and Emergence of Linguistic Structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. 2005. Input and SLA: adults’ sensitivity to different sorts of cues to French gender, in De Keyser, R. (ed.), Grammatical Development in Language Learning. The Best of Language Learning Series. Oxford: Blackwell. 79138.Google Scholar
Conway, Å. 2005. Le Paragraphe Oral en Français L1, Suédois L1 et Français L2. Études Romanes de Lund73. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Coppieters, R. 1987. Competence differences between native and non-native speakers. Language 63: 544573.Google Scholar
Dechert, H. & Raupach, M. (eds.) 1980. Temporal Variables in Speech. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Keyser, R. (ed.) 2005. What makes learning second language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Grammatical Development in Language Learning. The Best of Language Learning Series. Oxford: Blackwell. 125.Google Scholar
Donaldson, B. 2011. Left dislocation in near-native French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33: 399432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, B. 2017. Negation in near-native French. Variation and sociolinguistic competence. Language Learning 67/1: 141170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. 2015. Cognitive and social aspects of learning from usage, in Cadierno, T. and Eskildsen, S. W. (eds.), Usage-Based Perspectives on Second Language Learning. Berlin: Walter De Gruyter. 4974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. 2008. The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Engel, H. 2010. Dislocation et référence aux entités en français L2. Développement, interaction, variation. Cahiers de la recherche. 43. PhD Diss. Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Erman, B., Forsberg Lundell, F. & Lewis, M. 2018 (this volume, Chapter 4). Formulaic language in advanced long-residency L2 speakers, in Hyltenstam, K., Bartning, I. & Fant, L. (eds.), High-Level Language Proficiency in Second Language and Multilingual Contexts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Forsberg, F. 2008. Le Langage Préfabriqué – Formes, Fonctions et Fréquences en Français Parlé L2 et L1. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Forsberg, F. 2010. Using conventional sequences in L2 French. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL) 48: 2550.Google Scholar
Forsberg Lundell, F. & Bartning, I. (eds.) 2015a. Cultural Migrants and Optimal Language Acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsberg Lundell, F. & Bartning, I. 2015b. Successful profiles in high-level L2 French – ‘c’est un choix de vie’, in Forsberg Lundell, F. & Bartning, I. (eds.), Cultural Migrants and Optimal Language Acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 5982.Google Scholar
Forsberg Lundell, F., Bartning, I., Engel, H., Gudmundson, A., Hancock, V. & Lindqvist, C. 2014. Beyond advanced stages in high-level spoken French L2. Journal of French Language Studies 24: 126.Google Scholar
Freed, B. F., Segalovitz, N.P. & Dewey, D. 2004. Context of learning and second language fluency in French: comparing regular classroom, study abroad and intensive domestic immersion programs. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26/2: 275301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granfeldt, J. 2003. L’acquisition des catégories fonctionnelles. Étude comparative du développement du DP français chez des enfants et des apprenants adultes. Études Romanes de Lund 67. Lund University.Google Scholar
Hancock, V. 2007. Quelques éléments modaux dissociés dans le paragraphe oral dans des interviews en français L2 et L1. Journal of French Language Studies 17: 2347.Google Scholar
Herschensohn, J. 2006. Review article Français langue seconde: from functional categories to functionalist variation. Second Language Research 22 /1: 95113.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. 2010. Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua 120: 901931.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. 2012a. The development of L2 morphology. Introduction. Second Language Research 29/ 1: 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. 2012b. Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research 29/1: 3356.Google Scholar
Housen, A., Kemps, N. & Pierrard, M. 2009. The use of verb morphology of advanced L2 learners and native speakers of French, in Labeau, E. & Myles, F. (eds.), The Advanced Learner Variety: The Case of French. Bern: Peter Lang. 4061.Google Scholar
Howard, M. 2005. The emergence and use of the plus-que-parfait in advanced French interlanguage, in Dewaele, J.-M. (ed.), Focus on French as a Foreign Language, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. 6387.Google Scholar
Howard, M. 2009. Short versus long term effects of naturalistic exposure on the advanced instructed learner’s L2 development: a case study, in Labeau, E. & Myles, F. (eds.), The Advanced Learner Variety. The Case of French. Bern: Peter Lang. 93124.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. 2015a. Language Proficiency in Native and Non-Natives Speakers. Theory and Research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. 2015b. Explaining phenomena of first and second language acquisition with constructs of implicit and explicit learning: the virtues and pitfalls of a two-system view, in Rebuschat, P. (ed.), Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2546.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K. 1992. Non-native features of near-native speakers: on the ultimate attainment of childhood L2 learners, in Harris, R. J. (ed.), Cognitive Processing in Bilinguals. Amsterdam: North-Holland. 351368.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K. (ed.) 2016. Advanced Proficiency and Exceptional Ability in Second Languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K. & Abrahamsson, N. 2003. Maturational constraints in SLA, in Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. 539588.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., Bartning, I. & Fant, L. 2005. High-Level Proficiency in Second Language Use. Grant application. Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., Bartning, I. & Fant, L. (eds.) 2018 (this volume). High-Level Language Proficiency in Second Language and Multilingual Contexts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinsella, C. & Singleton, D. 2014. Much more than age. Applied Linguistics 2014, 123.Google Scholar
Klein, W. & Perdue., C. 1997. The basic variety or: couldn’t natural languages be much simpler? Second Language Research 13/4, 301347.Google Scholar
Labeau, E. & Myles, F. (eds.) 2009. The Advanced Learner Varieties: The Case of French. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lahmann, C. 2015. Beyond Barriers: Complexity, Accuracy and Fluency in Long-Term L2 Speakers’ Speech. PhD thesis. University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. 2007. Ultimate Attainment in Second Language Acquisition: A Case Study. Mahwaw, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Laufer, B. & Nation, P. 1995. Vocabulary size and use: lexical sophistication in L2 written production. Applied Linguistics 16/3: 307322.Google Scholar
Lindqvist, C. 2010. La richesse lexicale dans la production orale de l’apprenant avancé de français. Canadian Modern Language Review 66/3: 393420.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. 1990. Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 12: 251285.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. 2003. Stabilization and fossilization in interlanguage development, in Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. 487535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñoz, C. & Singleton, D. 2011. A critical review of age-related research on L2 ultimate attainment. Language Teaching 44/1: 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. 2004. The Acquisition of Spanish. Morphosyntactic Development in Monolingual and Bilingual L1 Acquisition and in Adult L2 Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. & Slabakova, R. 2003. Competence similarities between native and near-native speakers: An investigation of the Preterite/Imperfect contrast in Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25: 351398.Google Scholar
Morel, M.-A. & Danon-Boileau, L. 1998. La Grammaire de l’Intonation. L’Exemple du Français. Paris: Ophrys.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. 1999. Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology: the critical factors of age, motivation and instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 21: 81108.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. 2009. Declarative and Procedural Determinants of Second Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. 1998. Language Processing and Second Language Development. Processability Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Piller, I. 2002. Passing for a native speaker: Identity and success in second language learning. Journal of Sociolinguistics 6: 179206.Google Scholar
Seliger, H.W. 1978. Implications of a multiple critical periods hypothesis for second language learning, in Ritchie, W. C. (ed.), Second Language Acquisition Research: Issues and Implications. New York, NY: Academic Press. 1119.Google Scholar
Sharwood-Smith, M. & Truscott, J. 2005. Stages or continua in SLA: a Mogul solution. Applied Linguistics 26/2: 219240.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. 2009. Modelling second language performance: integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency and lexis. Applied Linguistics 30/4: 510532.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. 2014. The bottleneck of second language acquisition. Foreign Language Teaching and Research 46/4: 543559.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. 2003. Near-nativeness, in Doughty, C. & Long, L. (eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. 130151.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. & Serratrice, L. 2009. Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism 13: 195210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Stutterheim, C. 2003. Linguistic structure and information organisation. EUROSLA Yearbook 3:183206.Google Scholar
Towell, R., Hawkins, R. & Bazergui, N. 1996. The development of fluency in advanced learners of French. Applied Linguistics 17: 84119.Google Scholar
Vermeer, A. 2001. Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency input. Applied Psycholinguistics 22/ 2: 217234.Google Scholar
Véronis, J. 2011. Fréquence des mots en français parlé. https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01388193.Google Scholar
Véronique, D., Carlo, C., Granget, C., Kim, J-O. & Prodeau, M. 2009. L’Acquisition de la Grammaire du Français, Langue Étrangère. Paris: Didier.Google Scholar
Wray, A. 2002. Formulaic Language and the Lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×