Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-14T06:02:16.795Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Thomas Gilovich
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Dale Griffin
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Daniel Kahneman
Affiliation:
Princeton University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Heuristics and Biases
The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment
, pp. 763 - 854
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abarbanell, J. S., & Bernard, V. L. (1992). Tests of analysts overreaction/underreaction to earnings information as an explanation for anomalous stock price behavior. Journal of Finance, 47, 1181–1207. [38] Google Scholar
Abell, G. O. (1981). Astrology. In Science and the paranormal: Probing the existence of the supernatural (pp. 70–94), G. O. Abell & B. Singer (Eds.). New York: Charles Scribner & Sons. [34] Google Scholar
Adams, E. W. (1975). The logic of conditionals. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D. Reidel. [3] Google Scholar
Adelmann, P., & Zajonc, R. (1989). Facial efference and the experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 249–280. [5] Google ScholarPubMed
Adler, J. E. (1984). Abstraction is uncooperative. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 14, 165–181. [24] Google Scholar
Adler, J. E. (1991). An optimist's pessimism: Conversation and conjunctions. In Probability and rationality: Studies on L. Jonathan Cohen's philosophy of science (pp. 251–282), E. Eells & T. Maruszewski (Eds.). Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi. [24] Google Scholar
Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1996). Construing benefits from adversity: Adaptational significance and dispositional underpinnings. Journal of Personality, 64, 899–922. [16] Google ScholarPubMed
Agnoli, F. (1991). Development of judgmental heuristics and logical reasoning: Training counteracts the representativeness heuristic. Cognitive Development, 6, 195–217. [2] Google Scholar
Agnoli, F., & Krantz, D. H. (1989). Suppressing natural heuristicsbyformal instruction: The case of the conjunction fallacy. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 515–550. [2] Google Scholar
Ahlers, D., & Lakonishok, J. (1983). A study of economists' consensus forecasts. Management Science, 29, 1113–1125. [38] Google Scholar
Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [16] Google Scholar
Ajzen, I. (1977). Intuitive theories of events and the effects of base rate information on prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 303–314. [1, 24] Google Scholar
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. [14] Google Scholar
Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453–474. [14] Google Scholar
Alhakami, A. S., & Slovic, P. (1994). A psychological study of the inverse relationship between perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis, 14 (6), 1085–1096. [23] Google Scholar
Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., & Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 804–825. [27,36] Google Scholar
Alker, H., & Hermann, M. (1971). Are Bayesian decisions artificially intelligent? The effect of task and personality on conservatism in information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 31–41. [24] Google Scholar
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3–19. [22] Google Scholar
Allison, S. T., & Beggan, J. K. (1994). Estimating popular support for group decision outcomes: An anchoring and adjustment model. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 9, 617–638. [6] Google Scholar
Allison, S. T., & Messick, D. M. (1990). Social decision heuristics and the use of shared resources. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 3, 195–204. [30] Google Scholar
Alpert, M., & Raiffa, H. (1982). A progress report on the training of probability assessors. In Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 294–305), D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [13,15] Google Scholar
Amir, E., & Ganzach, Y. (1998). Overreaction and underreaction in analysts' forecasts. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 37, 333–347. [38] Google Scholar
Andersen, S. M., & Ross, L. (1984). Self knowledge and social inference: I. The impact of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 280–293. [36] Google Scholar
Andersen, S. M. (1984). Self knowledge and social inference: II. The diagnosticity of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 294–307. [36] Google Scholar
Anderson, C. A. (1983a). Abstract and concrete data in the perseverance of social theories: When weak data lead to unshakeable beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 93–108. [20] Google Scholar
Anderson, C. A. (1983b). Imagination and expectation: The effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 293–305. [4,17,19] Google Scholar
Anderson, C. A., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. (1980). Perseverance of social theories: The role of explanation in the persistence of discredited information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1037–1049. [4] Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295. [20] Google Scholar
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Personalility and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279. [18] Google ScholarPubMed
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press. [20, 23] Google Scholar
Anderson, N. H. (1991). Contributions to Information Integration Theory. Vol. I: Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [2] Google Scholar
Anderson, N. H. (1996). A functional theory of cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [2] Google Scholar
Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1991). Measures of subjective well-being. InMeasures of personality and social psychological attitudes (Vol. 1, pp. 61–114), J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), San Diego: Academic Press. [16] Google Scholar
Angier, N. (1999). Woman: An intimate geography. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. [30] Google Scholar
Appadurai, A. (1981). Gastro-politics in Hindu South Asia. American Ethnologist, 8, 494–511. [11] Google Scholar
Appley, M. H. (Ed.). (1971). Adaptation-level theory: A symposium. New York: Academic Press. [20] Google Scholar
Ariely, D. (1998). Combining experiences over time: The effects of duration, intensity changes and on-line measurements on retrospective pain evaluations. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11, 19–45. [2] Google Scholar
Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties. Psychological Science, 12 (2), 157–162. [2] Google ScholarPubMed
Ariely, D., Kahneman, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2000). Joint comment on “When does duration matter in judgment and decision making?” (Ariely & Loewenstein, 2000). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 524–529. [2] Google Scholar
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2000). When does duration matter in judgment and decision making? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 508–523. [2] Google ScholarPubMed
Arkes, H. R. (1991). Costs and benefits of judgment errors: Implications for debiasing. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 486–498. [6] Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R. (2001). Overconfidence in judgmental forecasting. In Principles of forecasting handbook (pp. 495–515), J. S. Armstrong (Ed.). Boston: Kluwer. [41] Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 125–140. [32] Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Dawson, N. V., Speroff, T., Harrell, F. E. Jr., Alzola, C., Phillips, R., Desbiens, N., Oye, R. K., Knaus, W., Connors, A. F. Jr., & the SUPPORT Investigators (1995). The covariance decomposition of the probability score and its use in evaluating prognostic estimates. Medical Decision Making, 15, 120–131. [39] Google ScholarPubMed
Arkes, H. R., Faust, D., Guilmette, T. J., & Hart, K. (1988). Eliminating the hindsight bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 305–307. [6, 14] Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R., Wortmann, R. L., Saville, P. D., & Harkness, A. R. (1981). Hindsight bias among physicians weighing the likelihood of diagnoses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 252–254. [40] Google ScholarPubMed
Arkin, R. A., & Duval, S. (1975). Effects of focus of attention on the causal attributions of actors and observers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 427–438. [20] Google Scholar
Armor, D. A. (1998). The illusion of objectivity: A bias in the perception of freedom from bias. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles. [36] Google Scholar
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 309- 379), M. P. Zanna (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [14] Google Scholar
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2000). Mindset, prediction, and performance: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University. [19] Google Scholar
Armstrong, J. S. (Ed.). (2001). Principles of forecasting handbook. Boston: Kluwer. [41] Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34), L. Berkowitz (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [36] Google Scholar
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 178–186. [19] Google ScholarPubMed
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 177–181. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Turken, A. U., & Waldron, E. M. (1998). A neuro-psychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review, 105, 442–481. [22] Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. [29] Google ScholarPubMed
Ashton, R. H. (1992). Effects of justification and a mechanical aid on judgment performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 292–306. [32] Google Scholar
Aspinwall, L. G., Kemeny, M. E., Taylor, S. E., Schneider, S. G., & Dudley, J. P. (1991). Psychological predictors in gay men's AIDS risk-reduction behavior. Health Psychology, 10, 432–444. [19] Google Scholar
Aspinwall, L. G., & Richter, L. (1999). Optimism and self-mastery predict more rapid disengagement from unsolvable tasks in the presence of alternatives. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 221–245. [19] Google Scholar
Astington, J., Harris, P., & Olson, D. (Eds.). (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. [8] Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. [19, 35] Google Scholar
Au, T. K. (1993). Developing an intuitive understanding of conservation and contamination. Developmental Psychology, 29, 286–299. [11] Google Scholar
Ayton, P. (1992). On the competence and incompetence of experts. In Expertise and decision support (pp. 77–105), G. Wright & F. Bolger (Eds.). Plenum Press: New York. [39] Google Scholar
Ayton, P. (1997). How to be incoherent and seductive: Bookmakers' odds and support theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 99–115. [27] Google Scholar
Ayton, P. (1998). How bad is human judgment? In Forecasting with judgment, G. Wright & P. Goodwin (Eds.). West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons. [2] Google Scholar
Ayton, P., & Hardman, D. (1997). Are two rationalities better than one? Current Psychology of Cognition, 16, 39–51. [24] Google Scholar
Ayton, P., & Onkal, D. (1996). Effects of expertise on forecasts and confidence in forecast. Paper presented at the International Symposium on Forecasting, Istanbul, Turkey. [39] Google Scholar
Baker, L. A., & Emery, R. E. (1993). When every relationship is above average: Perceptions and expectations of divorce at the time of marriage. Law and Human Behavior, 17, 439–450. [19] Google Scholar
Baker, N. (1991). Room temperature. New York: Vintage. [35] Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. [19, 36] Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company. [19] Google Scholar
Bankhart, C. P., & Elliot, R. (1974). Heart rate and skin conductance in anticipation of shocks with varying probability of occurrence. Psychophysiology, 11, 160–174. [30] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1973). On the subjective probability of compound events. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 9, 396–406. [1] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. Acta Psychologica, 44, 211–233. [24] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1983). The base rate fallacy controversy. In Decision making under uncertainty (pp. 39–61), R. W. Scholz (Ed.). Amsterdam: North-Holland. [13] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1990). Back to base rates. In Insights into decision making: A tribute to Hillel J. Einhorn (pp. 200–216), R. M. Hogarth (Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [24] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M., & Fischhoff, B. (1981). When do base-rates affect predictions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 671–680. [2] Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M., & Neter, E. (1993). How alike is it versus how likely is it: A disjunction fallacy in probability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1119–1131. [Introduction, 2, 25, 26] Google Scholar
Baranski, J. V., & Petrusic, W. M. (1994). The calibration and resolution of confidence in perceptual judgments. Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 412–428. [39] Google ScholarPubMed
Barefield, R. M., & Comiskey, E. E. (1975). The accuracy of analysts' forecasts of earnings per share. Journal of Business Research, 3, 241–252. [19] Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1982). Attention and automaticity in the processing of self-relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 425–436. [9] Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In The automaticity of everyday life. R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.). In The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Introduction, 2, 30] Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Bond, R. N., Lombardi, W. J., & Tota, M. E. (1986). The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 (5), 869–878. [2] Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Baron, J. (1992). The effect of normative beliefs on anticipated emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 320–330. [16] Google ScholarPubMed
Baron, J. (1993). Morality and rational choice. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. [24] Google Scholar
Baron, J. (1994a). Nonconsequentialist decisions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 1–42. [24] Google Scholar
Baron, J. (1994b). Thinking and Deciding, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. [6] Google Scholar
Baron, J. (1997). Confusion of relative and absolute risk in valuation. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14, 301–309. [23] Google Scholar
Baron, J. (1998). Judgment misguided: Intuition and error in public decision making. New York: Oxford University Press. [24,39] Google Scholar
Baron, J., & Brown, R. (Eds.). (1991) Teaching decision making to adolescents. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [41] Google Scholar
Barone, D. F., Maddux, J. E., & Snyder, C. R. (1997). Social cognitive psychology: History and current domains, New York: Plenum Press. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Barrett, S. (1987). Homeopathy: Is it medicine? Skeptical Inquirer, 12, 56–62. [34] Google Scholar
Barron, F. J. (1953). An ego-strength scale which predicts response to psychotherapy. Journal Of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 17, 327–333. [40] Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629–654. [20] Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In Concepts reconsidered: The ecological and intellectual bases of categories, U. Neisser (Ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. [20] Google Scholar
Barth, M. E., Elliot, J. A., & Finn, M. W. (1999). Market rewards associated with patterns of increasing earnings. Journal of Accounting Research, 37, 387–413. [38] Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [41] Google Scholar
Basil, R. (1989). Graphology and personality, “Let the buyer beware.” Skeptical Inquirer, 13, 241–243. [34] Google Scholar
Bastardi, A., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). False polarization and the gender gap: Actual vs. assumed discrepancies in belief. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University. [36] Google Scholar
Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer, C, & Vohs, K. D. (in press). Bad is stronger than good. (Review of General Psychology.) [11] Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F, & Leary, M. F. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motive. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. [32] Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Steinhilbm, A. (1984). Paradoxical effects of supportive audiences on performance under pressure: The home field disadvantage in sports championships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 85–93. [35] Google Scholar
Baumhart, R. (1968). An honest profit. New York: Prentice-Hall. [18] Google Scholar
Bazerman, M. H., & Neale, M. A. (1982). Improving negotiation effectiveness under final offer arbitration: The role ofselection and training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 543–548. [39] Google Scholar
Bazerman, M. H., & Neale, M. A. (1992). Negotiating rationally. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [41] Google Scholar
Beach, L. R. (1990). Image theory: Decision making in personal and organizational contexts. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons. Ltd. [30] Google Scholar
Beach, L. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1987). Image theory: Principles, goals and plans in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 66, 201–220. [30] Google Scholar
Beattie, J., & Baron, J. (1988). Confirmation and matching biases in hypothesis testing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 269–297. [6] Google Scholar
Bechera, A., Damasio, H., Tramel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295. [29] Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaum, J. K. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychology, 4, 561–571. [14] Google ScholarPubMed
Becker, G. (1996). The economic way of looking at behavior: The Nobel lecture. Stanford, CA: Stanford University, Hoover Institution on War, Revolution, and PeaceGoogle Scholar
Begg, I., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 446–458. [2] Google Scholar
Begg, I., & Armour, V. (1991). Repetition and the ring of truth: Biasing comments. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 23, 195–213. [2] Google Scholar
Bell, D. E. (1982). Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 30, 961–981. [21] Google Scholar
Bell, D. E. (1983). Risk premiums for decision regret. Management Science, 29, 1156–1166. [21] Google Scholar
Bell, D. E. (1985). Disappointment in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 33, 1–27. [21] Google Scholar
Bellhouse, D. R., & Franklin, J. (1997). The language of chance. International Statistical Review, 65 (1), 73–85. [39] Google Scholar
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200. [9] Google ScholarPubMed
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62), L. Berkowitz (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [Introduction, 9, 10, 36] Google Scholar
Benartzi, S., & Thaler, R. H. (2001). Naive diversification strategies in defined contribution saving plans. American Economic Review, 91 (1), 79–98. [30] Google Scholar
Benthin, A., Slovic, P., Moran, P., Severson, H., Mertz, C. K., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Adolescent health-threatening and health-enhancing behaviors: A study of word association and imagery. Journal of Adolescent Health, 17, 143–152. [23] Google ScholarPubMed
Berkeley, D., & Humphreys, P. (1982). Structuring decision problems and the “bias heuristic.” Acta Psychologica, 50, 201–252. [24] Google Scholar
Beyth-Marom, R. (1981). The subjective probability of conjunctions (Decision Research Report No. 81–12). Eugene, OR: Decision Research. [1] Google Scholar
Beyth-Marom, R. (1982). How probable is probable? Numerical translation of verbal probability expressions. Journal of Forecasting, 1, 257–269. [41] Google Scholar
Beyth-Marom, R., Dekel, S. K., Gombo, R., & Shaked, M. (1985). An elementary approach to thinking under uncertainty. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [41] Google Scholar
Beyth-Marom, R., Fischhoff, B., Quadrel, M. J., & Furby L. (1991). Teaching adolescents decision making. In Teaching decision making to adolescents (pp. 19–60), J. Baron & R. Brown (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [41] Google Scholar
Bierce, A. (1914). The devil's dictionary. New York: World Publishing Company. [14] Google Scholar
Biller, B., Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (1992, April). Die Leichtigkeit der Erinnerung als Information in der Urteilsbildung: der EinfluBder Fragenreihenfolge (Ease of recall as information: The impact of question order). Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen, Osnabrück, FRG. [5] Google Scholar
Birge, R. T. (1929). Probable Values of the General Physical Constants. Reviews of Modern Physics, 1, 1–73. [37] Google Scholar
Birge, R. T. (1932). The Calculation of Errors by the Method of Least Squares. Physical Review, 40, 207–227. [37] Google Scholar
Birge, R. T. (1941). The General Physical Constants: As of August 1941 with Details on the Velocity of Light Only. Reports on Progress in Physics, 8, 90–134. [37] Google Scholar
Birnbaum, M. H. (1983). Base rates in Bayesian inference: Signal detection analysis of the cab problem. American Journal of Psychology, 96, 85–94. [24] Google Scholar
Birnbaum, M. H. (1999). How to show that 9 221: Collect judgments in a between-subjects design. Psychological Methods, 4 (3), 243–249. [2] Google Scholar
Birnbaum, M. H., & Mellers, B. A. (1983). Bayesian inference: Combining base rates with opinions of sources who vary in credibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 792–804. [2] Google Scholar
Biswas, A., & Burton, S. (1993). Consumer perceptions of tensile price claims in advertisements: An assessment of claim types across different discount levels. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 21, 217–229. [6] Google Scholar
Bjorkman, M. (1994). Internal cue theory: Calibration and resolution of confidence in general knowledge. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 386–405. [39] Google Scholar
Bjorkman, M., Juslin, P., & Winman, A. (1993). Realism of confidence in sensory discrimination: The underconfidence phenomenon. Perception and Psychophysics, 54, 75–81. [39] Google ScholarPubMed
Blalock, S. J., DeVellis, B. M., & Afifi, R. A. (1990). Risk perceptions and participation in colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychology, 9, 792–806. [17] Google ScholarPubMed
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246. [29] Google ScholarPubMed
Bless, H. (1997). Stimmung und Denken. (Mood and reasoning.) Bern, Switzerland: Huber. [29] Google Scholar
Bless, H. (2001). The consequences of mood on the processing of social information. In Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes (pp. 391–421), A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell. [29] Google Scholar
Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 331–345. [29] Google Scholar
Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., Golisano, V, Rabe, C, & Wolk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665–679. [2, 29] Google ScholarPubMed
Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Sufficient and necessary conditions in dual process models: The case of mood and information processing. In Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 423–440), S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.). New York: Guilford. [29] Google Scholar
Bless, H., Schwarz, N., & Kemmelmeier, M. (1996). Mood and stereotyping: The impact of moods on the use of general knowledge structures. In M. Hewstone & W. Stroebe (Eds.). European Review of Social Psychology, 7, 63–93. [29] Google Scholar
Bless, H., Schwarz, N., & Wieland, R. (1996). Mood and stereotyping: The impact of category membership and individuating information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 935–959. [29] Google Scholar
Block, R. A., & Harper, D. R. (1991). Overconfidence in estimation: Testing the anchoring-and-adjustment hypothesis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 49, 188–207. [6] Google Scholar
Bobbio, M., Deorsola, A., Pistis, G., Brusca, A., & Diamond, G. A. (1988). Physician perception of exercise electrocardiography as a prognostic test after acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Cardiology, 62, 675–678. [39] Google ScholarPubMed
Bobbio, M., Detrano, R., Shandling, A. H., Ellestad, M. H., Clark, J., Brezden, O., Abecia, A., & Martinezcaro, D. (1992). Clinical assessment of the probability of coronary-artery disease - Judgmental bias from personal knowledge. Medical Decision Making, 12, 197–203. [39] Google ScholarPubMed
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1 (5), 319–322. [Introduction, 2] Google Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (1999). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias: The case of anchoring. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern University. [29] Google Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P, & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 621–632. [29] Google Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S. (1987). Social cognition and social reality: Information acquisition and use in the laboratory and the real world. In Social information processing and survey methodology (pp. 6–41), H. J. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.). New York: Springer Verlag. [5] Google Scholar
Bollnow, O. F. (1956). Das Wesen der Stimmungen. (The nature of moods.) Frankfurt: Klostermann. [29] Google Scholar
Bond, M. H., & Cheung, T. S. (1983). The spontaneous self-concept of college students in Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 153–171. [15] Google Scholar
Boneau, C. A. (1990). Short-term recognition memory under rehearsal instructions and imaging instructions. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 297–299. [17] Google Scholar
Borg, I. (1987). The effect of mood on different well-being judgments. Archiv fur Psychologie, 139, 181–188. [29] Google ScholarPubMed
Borgida, E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1977). The differential impact of abstract vs. concrete information on decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 7, 258–271. [28] Google Scholar
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–289. [23] Google Scholar
Bostrom, A., Fischhoff, B., & Morgan, G. M. (1992). Characterizing mental models of hazardous processes: A methodology with an application to radon. Journal of Social Issues, 48, 85–100 [41] Google Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148. [29] Google Scholar
Bower, G. H., & Mayer, J. D. (1985). Failure to replicate mood congruent retrieval. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 39–42. [29] Google Scholar
Brainard, R. W., Irby, T. S., Fitts, P. M., & Alluisi, E. (1962). Some variables influencing the rate of gain of information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 105–110. [12] Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1990). The “natural logic” approach to reasoning. In Reasoning, necessity, and logic: Developmental Perspectives (pp. 133–157), W. F. Overton (Ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [22] Google Scholar
Brake, G. L., Doherty, M. E., & Kleiter, G. D. (1997). A Brunswikian approach to calibration of subjective probabilities. Unpublished manuscript, Bowling Green State University. [39] Google Scholar
Brase, G. L., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1998). Individuation, counting, and statistical inference: The role of frequency and whole-object representations in judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 3–21. [24] Google Scholar
Braun, P. A., & Yaniv, I. (1992). A case study of expert judgment: Economists probabilities versus base-rate model forecasts. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5, 217–231. [39] Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press. [36] Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley. [36] Google Scholar
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: Atheory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press. [36] Google Scholar
Breiman, L., Friedman, J. H., Olshen, R. A., & Stone, C. J. (1984). Classification and regression trees. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth International Group. [31] Google Scholar
Brenner, L. (2000). Should observed overconfidence be dismissed as a statistical artifact? Critique of Erev, Wallsten, and Budescu (1994). Psychological Review, 107, 943–946. [27, 39] Google Scholar
Brenner, L. A. (1995). A stochastic model of the calibration of subjective probabilities. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University. [27, 39] Google Scholar
Brenner, L., & Koehler, D. J. (1999). Subjective probability of disjunctive hypotheses: Local-weight models for decomposition of evidential support. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 16–47. [27] Google Scholar
Brenner, L. A., Koehler, D. J., Liberman, V., & Tversky, A. (1996). Overconfidence in probability and frequency judgments: A critical examination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 212–219. [Introduction, 39] Google Scholar
Brenner, L., & Rottenstreich, Y. (1999). Focus, repacking, and the judgment of grouped hypotheses. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 141–148. [27] Google Scholar
Brenner, L., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2000). Asymmetric support theory: Focus-dependence and context independence in likelihood judgment. Unpublished manuscript. [27] Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36), T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [29] Google Scholar
Brickman, P., Coates, D., Janoff-Bulman, R. J. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917- 927. [16] Google Scholar
Brier, G. W. (1950). Verification of forecasts expressed in terms of probability. Monthly Weather Review, 78 (1), 1–3. [15] Google Scholar
Briggs, L. K., & Krantz, D. H. (1992). Judging the strength of designated evidence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5, 77–106. [25, 26, 27] Google Scholar
Brockner, J., Rubin, J. Z., & Lang, E. (1981). Face-saving and entrapment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 68–79. [32] Google Scholar
Brody, N. (1997). Intelligence, schooling, and society. American Psychologist, 52, 1046- 1050. [24] Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–215), E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [20] Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R. (1987). Decentralized control of categorization: The role of prior processing episodes. In Concepts reconsidered: The ecological and intellectual bases of categories U. Neisser (Ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. [20] Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278–287. [22] Google Scholar
Broome, J. (1990). Should a rational agent maximize expected utility? In The limits of rationality (pp. 132–145), K. S. Cook & M. Levi (Eds.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [24] Google Scholar
Brown & Williamson Tobacco Company (1999). [23] Google Scholar
Brown, P., Foster, G., & Noreen, E. (1985). Security analyst multi-year earnings forecasts and the capital market. Sarasota: American Accounting Association. [38] Google Scholar
Browne, T. (1642/1963). Riligio. Medici. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [16] Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [24] Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [24] Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. In Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 41–69), H. Gruber, K. R. Hammond, & R. Jesser (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [36] Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1966). On the conservation of liquids. In Studies in cognitive growth, J. S. Bruner, R. R. Olver, & P. M. Greenfield, et al. (Eds.), New York: Wiley. [1] Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1943). Organismic achievement and environmental probability. Psychological Review, 50, 255–272. [2, 39] Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychological Review, 2, 193–217. [Introduction, 39] Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1964). Scope and aspects of the cognitive problem. In Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 5–31), J. S. Bruner et al. (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [31] Google Scholar
Buckley, T., & Sniezek, J. (1992). Passion, preference, and predictability in judgmental forecasting. Psychological Reports, 70, 1022. [19] Google Scholar
Budescu, D. V., Erev, I., & Wallsten, T. S. (1997). On the importance of random error in the study of probability judgment. Part I: New theoretical developments. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 157–171. [39] Google Scholar
Budescu, D. V., Wallsten, T. S., & Au, W. T. (1997). On the importance of random error in the study of probability judgment. Part II: Applying the stochastic judgment model to detect systematic trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 173–188. [39] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., & Griffin, D. (1996, August). Getting things done: The impact of predictions on task completion. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. [14, 19] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., & Griffin, D. (2000a). Dispositional, contextual, and cognitive underpinnings of the planning fallacy. Unpublished manuscript, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario. [14] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., & Griffin, D. (2000b). Motivated prediction for self and others. Unpublished manuscript, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario. [14] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & MacDonald, H. (1997). The role of motivated reasoning in optimistic time predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 238–247. [14, 19] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., Otsubo, Y., Lehman, D., & Heine, S. (2000). A cross-cultural comparison of the planning fallacy. Unpublished manuscript, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario. [14] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (in press). Intensity bias in affective forecasts: The role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. [14] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the “planning fallacy”: Why people underestimate their task completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 366–381. [14, 19, 36] Google Scholar
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1995). It's about time: Optimistic predictions in work and love. In European Review of Social Psychology (Vol.6, pp. 1–32), W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.). Chichester, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons. [14] Google Scholar
Bunker, J. P., Barnes, B. A., & Mosteller, F. (Eds.). (1977). Costs, risks, and benefits of surgery. New York: Oxford University Press. [41] Google Scholar
Bunn, D. W., & Salo, A. A. (1993). Forecasting with scenarios. European Journal of Operational Research, 68, 291–303. [14] Google Scholar
Burgstahler, D., & Dichev, I. (1997). Earnings management to avoid earnings decreases and losses. Journal of Accounting and Economics, 24, 99–126. [38] Google Scholar
Burros, O. K. (1978). Mental measurement yearbook, 8th ed. Highland Park, NJ: Gryphon Press. [34] Google Scholar
Busemeyer, J., Hastie, R., & Medin, D. L. (Eds.) (1995). Decision making from a cognitive perspective, San Diego: Academic Press. [23] Google Scholar
Busemeyer, J. R., & Goldstein, W. M. (1992). Linking together different measures of preference: a dynamic model of matching derived from decision field theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 370–396. [6] Google Scholar
Busemeyer, J. R., & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Decisionfield theory: Adynamic - cognitive approachto decision making inan uncertain environment. Psychological Review, 100, 432–459. [6] Google Scholar
Byram, S. J. (1997). Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time prediction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 216–219. [14] Google Scholar
Byrne, J. A. (1986). Business fads: What's in - and out: Executives latch on to any management idea that looks like a quick fix. Business Week, January 20, 52–61. [41] Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Overton, W. F. (1986). Reasoning about certainty and uncertainty in concrete, causal, and propositional contexts. Developmental Psychology, 22, 793–799. [24] Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., & Tabossi, P. (Eds.) (1993). Idioms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [8] Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17. [2, 7, 29] Google Scholar
Cadsby, T. (2000). The ten biggest investment mistakes Canadians make and how to avoid them. Toronto: Stoddart. [39] Google Scholar
Calderon, T. G. (1993). Predictive properties of analysts' forecasts of corporate earnings. The Mid-Atlantic Journal of Business, 29, 41–58. [19] Google Scholar
Camerer, C. (1981). General conditions for the success of bootstrapping models. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 27, 411–422. [41] Google Scholar
Camerer, C. (1990). Do markets correct biases in probability judgment? Evidence from market experiments. In Advances in behavioral economics (Vol. 2, pp. 125–172), L. Green & J. H. Kagel (Eds.). Northwood, NJ: Ablex. [13] Google Scholar
Camerer, C., & Hogarth, R. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 7–42. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Camerer, C., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental approach. The American Economic Review, 89, 306–318. [19] Google Scholar
Campbell, J. D., & Fairey, P. J. (1985). Effects of self-esteem, hypothetical explanations, and verbalization of expectancies on future performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1097–1111. [19] Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. (1987). The role of associative interference in learning and retrieving arithmetic facts. In Cognitive processes in mathematics, J. A. Sloboda & D. Rogers (Eds.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [22] Google Scholar
Canadian Standards Association. (1997). Risk management (CSA-850). Ottawa. [41] Google Scholar
Capute, A. J., Neidermeyer, E. F. L., & Richardson, F. (1968). Electroencephalogram in children with minimal cerebral dysfunction. Pediatrics, 41, 1104–1114. [40] Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [22] Google Scholar
Carleton, W. T., Chen, C. R., & Steiner, T. L. (1998). Optimism biases among brokerage and nonbrokerage equity recommendations: Agency costs in the investment industry. Financial Management, 27, 17–30. [38] Google Scholar
Carlson, B. W. (1990). Anchoring and adjustment in judgments under risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 665–676. [6, 7] Google Scholar
Carlson, S. (1985). A double-blind test of astrology. Nature, 318, 419–425. [34] Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1975). Sentence comprehension: A psycholoinguistic processing model of verification. Psychological Review, 82, 45–73. [9] Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [24] Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1997). Psychometrics, intelligence, and public perception. Intelligence, 24, 25–52. [24] Google Scholar
Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effect of imagining an event on expectations for the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 88–96. [4, 17, 21] Google Scholar
Carroll, J. S., Wiener, R. L., Coates, D., Galegher, J., & Alibrio, J. J. (1982). Evaluation, diagnosis, and prediction in parole decision making. Law and Society Review, 17, 199–228. [40] Google Scholar
Carson, T. P., & Adams, H. E. (1980). Acticity valence as a function of mood change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 368–377. [29] Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer Verlag. [19] Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. [19] Google Scholar
Casman, E., Fischhoff, B., Palmgren, C., Small, M., & Wu, F. (2000). Integrated assessment of Cryptosporidium risks. Risk Analysis, 20, 493–509. [41] Google Scholar
Casscells, W., Schoenberger, A., & Graboys, T. (1978). Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 999–1001. [24] Google Scholar
Caverni, J.-P., & Pris, J. L. (1990). The anchoring-adjustment heuristic in an “information rich, real world setting”: Knowledge assessment by experts. In Cognitive biases, J.-P. Caverni, J.-M. Fabre, & M. Gonzalez (Eds.). New York: North-Holland. [6] Google Scholar
Centor, R. M., Dalton, H. P., & Yates, J. F. (1984). Are physicians' probability estimates better or worse than regression model estimates? Paper presented at the Sixth Annual Meeting of the Society for Medical Decision Making, Bethesda, MD. [39] Google Scholar
Cervone, D., & Peake, P. K. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and action: the influence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 492–501. [6, 7] Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, andpoint of view. In Subject and topic, C. N. Li (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [8] Google Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cuesinpersuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766. [32] Google Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In Social influence: the Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39), M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [9] Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In Unintended thought (pp. 212–252), J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.). New York: Guilford Press. [Introduction, 22] Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. [2, 5, 22, 23] Google Scholar
Chapman, G. B., & Bornstein, B. H. (1996). The more you ask for the more you get: Anchoring in personal injury verdicts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 519–540. [6] Google Scholar
Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (1994). The limits of anchoring. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 223–242. [6, 7] Google Scholar
Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (1999). Anchoring, activation and the construction of value. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79, 115–153. [6] Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1967). Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodi-agnostic observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193–204. [1, 40] Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1969). Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 271–280. [40] Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perceptioninchess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81. [39] Google Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416. [22] Google Scholar
Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). On the natural selection of reasoning theories. Cognition, 33, 285–313. [24] Google Scholar
Cherniak, C. (1986). Minimal rationality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [24] Google Scholar
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., & Beach, L. R. (1982). Experience and the base-rate fallacy. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 270–278. [39] Google Scholar
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., & Bushyhead, J. B. (1981). Physicians' use of probabilistic information in a real clinical setting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 928–935. [39] Google Scholar
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., & Willham, C. F. (1991). The hindsight bias: A metaanal-ysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 48, 147–173. [41] Google Scholar
Chwe, M. (1998). Culture, circles, and commercials: Publicity, common knowledge, and social coordination. Rationality & Society, 10, 47–75. [8] Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Levy, A., Herman, C. P., Kozlowski, I. T., & Petty, R. E. (1976). Elastic shifts of opinion: Determinants of direction and durability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 663–672. [32] Google Scholar
Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation in clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 87–95. [29] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1979). Responding to indirect speech acts. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 430- 474. [20] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [8] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Carlson, T. B. (1981). Context for comprehension. In Attention and Performance IX (pp. 313–330), J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [8] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517. [9] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1974). Perceptual coding strategies in the formation and verification of descriptions. Memory and Cognition, 2, 101–111. [9] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Haviland, S. E. (1977). Comprehension and the given new contract. In Discourse production and comprehension (pp. 1–40), R. O. Freedle (Ed.). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. [20] Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Elements of discourse understanding (pp. 10–63), A. H. Joshe, B. Webber, & I. A. Sag (Eds.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [8] Google Scholar
Clayman, M. R., & Schwartz, R. A. (1994). Falling in love again: Analysts' estimates and reality. Financial Analysts Journal, 50, 66–68. [38] Google Scholar
Clemen, R. (1991). Making hard decisions. Boston: PWS-Kent. [41] Google Scholar
Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In The construction of social judgments (pp. 133–163), L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [5] Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Byrne, D. (1974). A reinforcement affect model of attraction. In Foundations of interpersonal attraction (pp. 173–170), T. L. Huston (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [29] Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed.; Vol. 1, pp. 323–418), R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [29] Google Scholar
Cohen, B., & Lee, I. S. (1979). A catalog of risks. Health Physics, 36, 707–722. [41] Google Scholar
Cohen, E. R., Crowe, K. M., & Dumond, J. W. M. (1957). Fundamental constants of Physics. New York: Interscience. [37] Google Scholar
Cohen, E. R., & Dumond, J. W. M. (1965). Our knowledge of the fundamental constants of physics and chemistry in 1965. Review of Modern Physics, 37, 537–594. [37] Google Scholar
Cohen, E. R., Dumond, J. W. M., Layton, T. W., & Rollett, J. S. (1955). Analysis of variance of the 1952 data on the atomic constants and a new adjustment, 1955. Review of Modern Physics, 27, 363–380. [37] Google Scholar
Cohen, E. R., & Taylor, B. N. (1973). The 1973 Least-Squares adjustment of the fundamental constants. Journal of Physical and Chemical Reference Data, 2 (4), 663–734. [37] Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. San Diego: Academic Press. [2] Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112 (1), 155–159. [2] Google Scholar
Cohen, J., & Hansel, C. M. (1957). The nature of decision in gambling: Equivalence of single and compound subjective probabilities. Acta Psychologica, 13, 357–370. [1] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1977). The probable and the provable. Oxford, U.K.: Clarendon Press. [1] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1979). On the psychology of prediction: Whose is the fallacy? Cognition, 7, 385–407. [24, 28] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1981). Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 317–370. [Introduction, 24, 28] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1982). Are people programmed to commit fallacies? Further thoughts about the interpretation of experimental data on probability judgment. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 12, 251–274. [24] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1983). The controversy about irrationality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 510–517. [24] Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (1986). The dialogue of reason. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. [24] Google Scholar
Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. College Board (1976–1977). Student descriptive questionnaire. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. [18] Google Scholar
Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Skokan, L. A. (1990). A better world or a shattered vision? Changes in life perspectives following victimization. Social Cognition, 8, 263–285. [16] Google Scholar
Combs, B., & Slovic, P. (1979). Newspaper coverage of causes of death. Journalism Quarterly, 56, 832–849. [41] Google Scholar
Conway, M., & Ross, M. (1984). Getting what you want by revising what you had. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 738–748. [19] Google Scholar
Cooke, R. M. (1991). Experts in Uncertainty. New York: Oxford University Press. Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribner's. [36] Google Scholar
Cooper, A., Woo, C., & Dunkelberg, W. (1988). Entrepreneurs' perceived chances for success. Journal of Business Venturing, 3, 97–108. [13, 39] Google Scholar
Cooper, A. C., & Artz, K. W. (1995). Determinants of satisfaction for entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing, 10, 439–457. [19] Google Scholar
Cooper, R., DeJong, D., Forsythe, B., & Ross, T. (1992). Forward induction in coordination games. Economics Letters, 40, 167–172. [8] Google Scholar
Cooper, R., DeJong, D., Forsythe, B., & Ross, T. (1994). Alternative institutions for resolving coordination problems: Experimental evidence on forward induction and preplay communication. In Problems of coordination in economic activity (pp. 129–146), J. Friedman (Ed.). Norwell, MA: Kluwer. [8] Google Scholar
Corneille, O., Leyens, J., Yzerbyt, V. T., & Walter, E. (1999). Judgeability concerns: The interplay of information, applicability, and accountability in the overattribution bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 377–387. [32] Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276. [24] Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an evolutionarily rigorous cognitive science. Cognition, 50, 41–77. [24] Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment and uncertainty. Cognition, 58 (1), 1–73. [Introduction, 2, 14, 24] Google Scholar
Cowley, G., King, P., Hager, M., & Rosenberg, D. (1995, June 26). Going mainstream. Newsweek, 56–57. [34] Google Scholar
Crocker, J. (1982). Biased questions in judgment of covariation studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 214–220. [7] Google Scholar
Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83, 85–113. [35] Google Scholar
Crosby, F. J., & Cordova, D. I. (1996). Words of wisdom: Toward an understanding of affirmative action. Journal of Social Issues, 52, 33–49. [10] Google Scholar
Cross, H., Holcomb, A., & Matter, C. G. (1967). Imprinting or exposure learning in rats given early auditory stimulation. Psychonomic Science, 7, 233–234. [30] Google Scholar
Cross, P. (1977). Not can but will college teaching be improved. New Directions for Higher Education, 17, 1–15. [18] Google Scholar
Cummings, K. M., Becker, M. H., & Maile, M. C. (1980). Bringing the models together: An empirical approach to combining variables used to explain health actions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 3,123–145. [17] Google Scholar
Cummins, D. D. (1996). Evidence for the innateness of deontic reasoning. Mind & Language, 11, 160–190. [24] Google Scholar
Curley S. P., Yates, J. F., & Abrams, R. A. (1986). Psychological sources of ambiguity avoidance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 230–256Google Scholar
Cvetkovich, G. (1978). Cognitive accommodation, language, and social responsibility. Social Psychology, 2, 149–155. [32] Google Scholar
Czaczkes, B., & Ganzach, Y. (1996). The natural selection of prediction heuristics: anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 125–139. [6] Google Scholar
Czerlinski, J. (1997). Algorithm calculation costs measured by EIPs. Manuscript, Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich. [31] Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descarte's error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam [23, 29] Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. C. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 41, 81–94. [23] Google Scholar
David, F. N. (1949). Probability theory for statistical methods. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [33] Google Scholar
Davidson, W. P. (1983). The third person effect in communication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 1–15. [36] Google Scholar
Davies, T. (Ed.), (1996). Comparing environmental risks. Washington, DC: Resources for the Future. [41] Google Scholar
Davis, H. L., Hoch, S. J., & Ragsdale, E. E. (1986). An anchoring and adjustment model of spousal predictions. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 25–37. [6] Google Scholar
Davis, J. A. (1966). The campus as a frog pond: An application of the theory of relative deprivation to career decisions of college men. American Journal of Sociology, 72, 17–31. [20] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. (1998). Judgment and choice. In Handbook of social psychology, D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.) (pp. 497–548). New York: McGraw Hill. [42] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1971). A case study of graduate admissions: Application of three principles of human decision making. American Psychologist, 26, 180–188. [40] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making. American Psychologist, 34, 571–582. [13,31,40] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1980). You can't systemize human judgment: Dyslexia. New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science, 4, 67–78. [28] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1988). Rational choice in an uncertain world, New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [Introduction, 13,14] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1989). Statistical criteria for establishing a truly false consensus effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–17. [24] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1990). The potential nonfalsity of the false consensus effect. In Insights into decision making (pp. 179–199), R. M. Hogarth (Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [24] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1993). Equating the inverse probabilities in implicit personality judgments. Psychological Science, 6, 396–400. [41] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built on myth. New York: Free Press. [34] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M., & Corrigan, B. (1974). Linear models in decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 95–106. [31,40,41] Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M., Faust, D., & Meehl, P. E. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science, 243, 1668–1674. [13] Google Scholar
Dawson, S. (1993). Decision-making and leadership: Heller, F Journal of Management Studies, 30 (3), 479–481. [39] Google Scholar
De Bondt, W. F M., & Thaler, R. H. (1985). Does the stock market overreact? Journal of Finance, 40, 793–805. [38] Google Scholar
De Bondt, W. F M., & Thaler, R. H. (1987). Further evidence on investor overreaction and stock market seasonality. Journal of Finance, 42, 557–581. [38] Google Scholar
de Finetti, B. (1970). Theory of probability (Vol. 1). New York: John Wiley and Sons (republished 1990). [Introduction, 24] Google Scholar
Dean, G. (1987). Does astrology need to be true? Part II: The answer is no. Skeptical Inquirer, Spring, 257–273. [34] Google Scholar
Degeorge, F., Patel, J., & Zeckhauser, R. (1999). Earnings management to exceed thresholds. Journal of Business, 72, 1–33. [38] Google Scholar
DeKay M. L., & McClelland, G. H. (1995). Probability and utility components of endangered species preservation programs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 60–83. [2] Google Scholar
Dellarosa, D., & Bourne, L. E. (1984). Decisions and memory: Differential retrievabil-ity of consistent and contradictory evidence. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 669–682. [6] Google Scholar
DeMarco, T. (1982). Controlling software projects: Management, measurement, and estimation. New York: Yourdon. [14] Google Scholar
Dempster, A. P. (1967). Upper and lower probabilities induced by a multivalued mapping. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 38, 325–339. [1, 25] Google Scholar
Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 819–829. [23] Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1981). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press. [16, 28] Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1981). Three kinds of intentional psychology. In Reduction, time and reality, R. Healey (Ed.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [28] Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1983). True believers: The intentional strategy and why it works. Unpublished manuscript, Tufts University. [28] Google Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Stone, J. L., & Lassiter, G. D. (1985). Deceiving and detecting deceit. In The self in social life (pp. 323–370), B. R. Schlenker (Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. [9] Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1984). Principles of philosophy. In The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 1, pp. 193–291), J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch, (Eds. and Trans.)Google Scholar
Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1644.) [9] Google Scholar
DeSmet A. A., Fryback D. G., & Thornbury J. R. (1979). A second look at the utility of radiographic skull examination for trauma. American Journal of Roentgenology, 132 (1), 95–99. [39] Google Scholar
Desvouges, W. H., Johnson, F., Dunford, R., Hudson, S., Wilson, K., & Boyle, K. (1993). Measuring resource damages with contingent valuation: Tests of validity and reliability. In Contingent valuation: A critical assessment. Amsterdam: North Holland. [2] Google Scholar
Deutsch, R. M. (1977). The new nuts among the berries: How nutrition nonsense captured America. Palo Alto, CA: Ball Publishing. [34] Google Scholar
Diamond, (1996). Testing the internal consistency of contingent valuation surveys. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 28, 155–173. [2] Google Scholar
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575. [16] Google Scholar
Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. [16] Google Scholar
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. [16] Google Scholar
DiFranza, J. R., Rigotti, N. A., McNeill, A. D., Ockene, J. K., Savageau, J. A., St Cyr, D., & Coleman, M. (2000). Initial symptoms of nicotine dependence in adolescents. Tobacco Control, 9, 313–319. [23] Google Scholar
DiMatteo, M. R., & DiNicola, D. D. (1982). Achieving patient compliance. New York: Pergamon Press. [4] Google Scholar
Doleys, E. J., & Renzaglia, G. A. (1963). Accuracy of student prediction of college grades. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 41, 528–530. [19] Google Scholar
Dominowski, R. L. (1995). Content effects in Wason's selection task. In Perspectives on thinking and reasoning (pp. 41–65), S. E. Newstead & J. S. B. T. Evans (Eds.). Hove, England: Erlbaum. [24] Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children's minds. London: Fontana Paperbacks. [24] Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1993). Human minds: An exploration. New York: Viking Penguin. [24] Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., & Johnson, B. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510–540. [10] Google Scholar
Dowen, R. J. (1996). Analyst reaction to negative earnings for large well-known firms. Journal of Portfolio Management, 23, 49–55. [38] Google Scholar
Dowie, J. (1983). Risk. Buckinghamshire, England: Open University. [41] Google Scholar
Downs, J. S., & Shafir, E. (1999). Why some are perceived as more confident and more insecure, more reckless and more cautious, more trusting and more suspicious, than others: Enriched and impoverished options in social judgment. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 598–610. [27] Google Scholar
Dreman, D. (1979). Contrarian investment strategy. New York: Random House. [41] Google Scholar
Dreman, D. (1999). Contrarian investment strategies: The next generation. New York: Simon & Schuster. [41] Google Scholar
Dreman, D. N., & Berry, M. A. (1995). Analyst forecasting errors and their implications for security analysis. Financial Analysts Journal, 51, 30–41. [38] Google Scholar
Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and prevention. New York: Oxford University Press. [41] Google Scholar
Dube-Rioux, L., & Russo, J. E. (1988). An availability bias in professional judgment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 1, 223–237. [25] Google Scholar
Dubois, D., & Prade, H. (1988). Modelling uncertainty and inductive inference: A survey of recent non-additive probability systems. Acta Psychologica, 68, 53–78. [25] Google Scholar
Dulany, D. E., & Hilton, D. J. (1991). Conversational implicature, conscious representation, and the conjunction fallacy. Social Cognition, 9, 85–110. [2, 24] Google Scholar
Dumond, J. W. M., & Cohen, E. R. (1951). Least-Squares adjusted values of the atomic constants as of December, 1950. Physical Review, 82, 555–556. [37] Google Scholar
Dumond, J. W. M., & Cohen, E. R. (1952). Least-Squares adjustment of the atomic constants. Reviews of Modern Physics, 25, 691–708. [37] Google Scholar
Dun & Bradstreet. (1967). Patterns of success in managing a business. New York: Dun & Bradstreet. [13] Google Scholar
Dunham H. W., & Meltzer, B. M. (1946). Predicting length of hospitalization of mental patients. American Journal of Sociology, 52, 123–131. [40] Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Griffin, D. W., Milojkovic, J., & Ross, L. (1990). The overconfidence effect in social prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 568–581. [13, 14, 16] Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082–1090. [19, 27] Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Perie, A., & Story, A. L. (1991). Self-serving prototypes of social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 957–968. [36] Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1976). The elementary forms of the religious life (2nd ed.). London: Allen and Unwin. (Original work published 1925). [32] Google Scholar
Dutton, D. G, & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 30, 510- 517. [10] Google Scholar
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press. [20] Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. [36] Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [14, 22, 29] Google Scholar
Eddy, D. M. (1982). Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: Problems and opportunities. In Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 249–267), D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [39, 41] Google Scholar
Edmonson, F. K. (1934). Velocity of Light. Nature, 133, 759–760. [37] Google Scholar
Edwards, A. L. (1957). Techniques of attitude scale construction. New York: Appleton-Century-Croft. [23] Google Scholar
Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 5–24. [36] Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380–417. [31] Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1961). Behavioral decision theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 12, 473- 498. [31] Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1968). Conservatism in human information processing. In Formal representation of human judgment (pp. 17–52), B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.). New York: Wiley. [Introduction, 13] Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1970). Comment. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 70, 291- 293. [1] Google Scholar
Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. J. (1963). Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research. Psychological Review, 70, 193–242. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Edwards, W., & von Winterfeldt, D. (1986). Cognitive illusions and their implications for the law. Southern California Law Review, 59, 225–276. [39] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J. (1972). Expert measurement and mechanical combination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 7, 86–106. [40] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1975). Unit weighting schemes for decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 13, 171–192. [40] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1978). Confidence in judgment: Persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 85, 395–416. [28, 40] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1981). Behavioral decision theory: Processes of judgment and choice. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 53–88. [1, 24, 28] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1982). A theory of diagnostic inference: Imagination and the psychophysics of evidence. Center for Decision Research, Graduate School of Business, University of Chicago. [20] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1985). Ambiguity and uncertainty in probabilistic inference. Psychological Review, 92, 433–461. [6] Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1986). Probable cause: Adecision making framework. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 3–19. [20] Google Scholar
Eisenhardt, C. (1968). Expression of uncertainties of final results. Science, 160, 1201- 1204. [37] Google Scholar
Elliott, R. (1975). Heart ratein anticipation of shocks which have different probabilities of occurrences. Psychological Reports, 36, 923–931. [30] Google Scholar
Ellis, H. D. (1986). Face recall: A psychological perspective. Human Learning. Journal of Practical Research and Applications, 5, 189–196. [17] Google Scholar
Elster, J. (1977). Ulysses and the sirens. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [16] Google Scholar
Elster, J. (1983). Sour grapes: Studies in the subversion of rationality. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [24] Google Scholar
Elster, J., & Loewenstein, G. (1992). Utility from memory and anticipation. In Choice over time (pp. 213–234), J. Elster & G. Loewenstein (Eds.). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [14] Google Scholar
Elton, E. J., Gruber, M. J., & Gultekin, M. N. (1984). Professional expectations: Accuracy and diagnosis of errors. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 19, 351–365. [38] Google Scholar
Englich, B., & Mussweiler, T. (in press). Legal judgment under uncertainty: Anchoring effects in the court room. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. [6] Google Scholar
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861–875. [14, 36] Google Scholar
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2000a). Anchoring and effortful adjustment from self-generated anchors. Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University. [7] Google Scholar
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2000b). Close but not quite: insufficient adjustment from self-generated anchors. Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University. [7] Google Scholar
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back in the anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic: Self-generated versus experimenter provided anchors. Psychological Science, 12, 391–396. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49 (8), 709–724. [2, 23, 24] Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C., & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 328–339. [22] Google Scholar
Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., & Therriault, N. (1996). On being cool and collected: Mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 757–766. [16] Google Scholar
Erber, R., & Tesser, A. (1992). Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 339–359. [16] Google Scholar
Erev, I., Wallsten, T. S., & Budescu, D. V. (1994). Simultaneous over- and underconfi-dence: The role of error in judgment processes. Psychological Review, 101, 519–527. [15, 25, 26, 39] Google Scholar
Ericsson, A., & Simon, H. (1994). Verbal reports as data (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [41] Google Scholar
Erwin, P. G., & Calev, A. (1984). The influence of Christian name stereotypes on the marking of children's essays. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 223–227. [23] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1982). The psychology of deductive reasoning. London: Routledge. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 451–468. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1989). Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. London: Erlbaum Associates. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1996). Deciding before you think: Relevance and reasoning in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 223–240. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Barston, J., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11, 295–306. [22, 24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Handley, S. H., Perham, N., Over, D. E., & Thompson, V. A. (2000). Frequency versus probability formats in statistical word problems. Cognition, 77, 197–213. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Lynch, J. S. (1973). Matching bias in the selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 391–397. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993). Human reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Hove, England: Erlbaum. [24] Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, England: Psychology Press. [Introduction, 22, 24] Google Scholar
Evans, J., Handley, S. J., Perham, N., Over, D. E., & Thompson, V. A. (2000). Frequency versus probability formats in statistical word problems. Cognition, 77, 197–213. [2] Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the Azande. Oxford: Clarendon. [34] Google Scholar
Falk, R. (1981). The perception of randomness. In Proceedings, Fifth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Grenoble, France. [33] Google Scholar
Falk, R. (1992). A closer look at the probabilities of the notorious three prisoners. Cognition, 43, 197–223. [22] Google Scholar
Farber, H. S., & Bazerman, M. H. (1986). The general basis of arbitrator behavior: An empirical analysis of conventional and final offer arbitration. Econometrica, 54, 1503–1528. [39] Google Scholar
Farber, H. S., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Divergent expectations as a cause of disagreement in bargaining: Evidence from a comparison of arbitration schemes. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 104, 99–120. [39] Google Scholar
Faust, D. (1984). The limits of scientific reasoning. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. [40] Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. In Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 247–282), R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [23] Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. [10] Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1967). Valence of outcome and expectation of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 372–386. [35] Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1969). Attribution of responsibility and valence of success and failure in relation to initial confidence and task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 129–144. [35] Google Scholar
Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McGown, W. G. (1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Plenum Press. [14] Google Scholar
Ferrell, W. R. (1994). Discrete subjective probabilities and decision analysis: Elicitation, calibration, and combination. In Subjective probability (pp. 411–451), G. Wright & P. Ayton (Eds.). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley. [39] Google Scholar
Ferrell, W. R., & McGoey, P. J. (1980). Amodel of calibration for subjective probabilities. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26, 32–53. [13, 27, 39] Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140. [35] Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [16, 36] Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (Ed.). (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [42] Google Scholar
Festinger, L., & Maccoby, N. (1964). On resistance to persuasive communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 359–366. [9] Google Scholar
Fetherstonhaugh, D., Slovic, P., Johnson, S. M., & Friedrich, J. (1997). Insensitivity to the value of human life: A study of psychophysical numbing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14 (3), 282–300. [23] Google Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1988). The dependence of the conjunction fallacy on subtle linguistic factors. Psychological Research, 50, 123–129. [24] Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., & Armbruster, T. (1994). Two halfs may be more than one whole. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 633–645. [25] Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., Pampe, H., & Scherf, U. (1986). Mood an memory for tightly organized social information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 149–164. [29] Google Scholar
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 1–17. [2, 23, 30] Google Scholar
Fischer, G. W., & Hawkins, S. A. (1988). Preference reversals in multiattribute decision making: Scale compatibility, strategy compatibility, and the prominence effect. Carnegie-Mellon University. Typescript. [12] Google Scholar
Fischer, G. W., Morgan, G. M., Fischhoff, B., Nair, I., & Lave, L. B. (1991). What risks are people concerned about? Risk Analysis, 11, 303–314. [41] Google Scholar
Fischer, P. M. (1991). Brand logo recognition by children ages 3 to 6 years: Mickey Mouse and Old Joe the Camel. Journal of the American Medical Association, 266, 3145. [23] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic. P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1981). Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk. In Progress in resource management and environmental planning (Vol. 3, pp. 161–202), T. O'Riordan & R. K. Turner (Eds.). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley. [33] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight is not equal to foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299. [5, 6, 8,40,42] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1980). For those condemned to study the past. In New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science (pp. 79–93), R. A. Schweder & D. W. Fiske (Eds.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. [40] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1982a). Debiasing. In Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 422–444), D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [5,10,13,14, 41, 42] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1982b). For those condemned to study the past: Heuristics and biases in hindsight. In Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208), D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [5,19] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1984). Setting standards: A systematic approach to managing public health and safety risks. Management Science, 30, 823–843. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1988). Judgment and decision making. In The psychology of human thought (pp. 153–187), R. Sternberg & E. E. Smith (Eds.). New York: Cambridge University Press. [39] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1989). Eliciting knowledge for analytical representation. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, 13, 448–461. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1992). Giving advice: Decision theory perspectives on sexual assault. American Psychologist, 47, 577–588. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1994). What forecasts (seem to) mean. International Journal of Forecasting, 10, 387–403. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1995). Risk perception and communication unplugged: Twenty years of process. Risk Analysis, 15, 137–145. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1996). The real world: What good is it? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 232–248. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1998). Communicate unto others…. Reliability Engineering and System Safety, 59, 63–72. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1999). Why (cancer) risk communication can be hard. Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 7–13. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (2000a). Informed consent in eliciting environmental values. Environmental Science and Technology, 38, 1439–1444 [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (2000b). Need to know: Analytical and psychological criteria. Roger Williams University Law Review. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., & Bar-Hillel, M. (1984). Focusing techniques: A shortcut to improving probability judgments? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 34, 175–194. [13] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., & Beyth, R. (1975). “I knew it would happen” - remembered probabilities of once-future things. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 1–16. [7,8,19, 40] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., & Beyth-Marom, R. (1983). Hypothesis evaluation from a Bayesian perspective. Psychological Review, 90, 239–260. [13, 41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1978). Fault trees: Sensitivity of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 330–344. [14, 25] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Derby, S. L., & Keeney R. L. (1981). Acceptable risk. New York: Cambridge University Press. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., & MacGregor D. (1982). Subjective confidence in forecasts. Journal of Forecasting, 1, 155–172. [13, 39] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Nadaï, A., & Fischhoff, I. (2001). Investing in Frankenfirms. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 2, 100–111. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Parker, A., Bruine de Bruin, W., Downs, J., Palmgren, C, Dawes, R. M., & Manski, C. (2000). Teen expectations for significant life events. Public Opinion Quarterly, 64, 189–205. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Riley, D., Kovacs, D., & Small, M. (1998). What information belongs in a warning? A mental models approach. Psychology and Marketing, 15, 663–686. [41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 552–564. [15] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Reid, S., & Coombs, B. (1978). How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sciences, 9, 127–152. [23, 41] Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B., & Whipple, C. (1981). Risk Assessment: Evaluating error in subjective estimates. Environmental Professional, 3, 277–291. [37] Google Scholar
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley [14] Google Scholar
Fishburn, P. (1983). Nontransitive measurable utility. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 26, 31–67. [21] Google Scholar
Fiske, A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1997). Taboo trade-offs: Reactions to transactions that transgrees spheres of justice. Political Psychology, 18, 255–297. [32] Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., Kenny, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1982). Structural models for the mediation of salience effects on attribution. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 105–127. [20] Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74), M. P. Zanna (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [Introduction, 29] Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition, 2nd Ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210. [12] Google Scholar
Flavell, J. (1986). The development of children's knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. American Psychologist, 41, 418–425. [11] Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. New York: Van Nostrand. [27, 36] Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [36] Google Scholar
Fleming, J., & Arrowood, A. J. (1979). Information processing and the perseverance of discredited self-perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5, 201–205. [20] Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2000). The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71. [22] Google Scholar
Fong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1983). Improving inductive reasoning through statistical training. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan. [28] Google Scholar
Fong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 253–292. [24] Google Scholar
Fordyce, M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures: A sixty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators Research, 20, 355–381. [16] Google Scholar
Forer, B. (1949). The fallacy of personal validation: A classroom demonstration of gullibility. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44, 118–123. [13] Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Emotion in social judgments: Review and a new affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66. [29] Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 53–60. [29] Google Scholar
Forster, J., & Strack, F (1996). Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–430. [7] Google Scholar
Forster, J., & Strack, F. (1997). Motor actions in retrieval of valenced information: a motor congruence effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 1419–1427. [7] Google Scholar
Fox, C. R. (1999). Strength of evidence, judged probability, and choice under uncertainty. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 167–189. [27] Google Scholar
Fox, C. R., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Correlations, causes, and heuristics in surveys of life satisfaction. Social Indicators Research, 27, 221–234. [27] Google Scholar
Fox, C. R., Rogers, B. A., & Tversky A. (1996). Options traders exhibit subadditive decision weights. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 13, 5–17. [25, 26, 27] Google Scholar
Fox, C. R., & Tversky, A. (1995). Ambiguity Aversion and comparative ignorance. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 585–603. [27] Google Scholar
Fox, C. R., & Tversky, A. (1998). A belief-based account of decision under uncertainty. Management Science, 44, 879–895. [26, 27] Google Scholar
Frame, D. M. (transl.). (1943). The complete essays of Montaigne. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [8] Google Scholar
Francis, J., & Philbrick, D. R. (1993). Analysts' decisions as products of multi-task environment. Journal pf Accounting Research, 31, 216–230Google Scholar
Frank, M. G., & Gilovich, T. (1988). The dark side of self- and social perception: Black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 74–85. [35] Google Scholar
Frank, R. H., & Cook, P. (1995). The winner-take-all society. New York: Free Press. [35] Google Scholar
Franklin, A. D. (1981). Millikan's Published and Unpublished Data on Oil Drops. Historical Studies in Physical Science, 11, 185–201. [37] Google Scholar
Franklin, A. D. (1984). Forging, cooking, trimming, and riding on the bandwagon. American Journal of Physics, 52, 786–793. [37] Google Scholar
Frazer, J. G. (1890/1959). The golden bough: a study in magic and religion. New York: Macmillan. (Reprint of 1922 abridged edition, edited by T. H. Gaster; original work published 1890) [11] Google Scholar
Frederick, S. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1998). Scope (in)sensitivity in elicited valuations. Risk, Decision, and Policy, 3, 109–124. [2,30] Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 45–55. [2,16] Google Scholar
Freud, A. (1936). Ego and the mechanisms of defense (Rev. ed., 1966, trans., C. Baines). New York: International Universities Press. [16] Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1913). The interpretation of dreams. London: George Allen & Unwin. [22] Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1950). Totem and taboo: some points of agreement between the mental lives of savages and neurotics. (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton & Co. (Original work published 1913) [11] Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1966). The Dream Work. In Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis (Lecture Ⅺ, pp. 170–183), J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton & Co. (Original work published 1920) [11] Google Scholar
Friedman, N., & Goldszmit, L. (1996). A software for learning Bayesian networks. (Not released for public use.) [31] Google Scholar
Friedman, R. S., & Forster, J. (2000). The effects of approach and avoidance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–492. [29] Google Scholar
Friedrich, J. (1996). On seeing oneself as less self serving than others: The ultimate self serving bias? Teaching of Psychology, 23, 107–109. [36] Google Scholar
Friedrich, J. Barnes, P., Chapin, K., Dawson, I., Garst, V., & Kerr, D. (1999). Psychophysical numbing: When lives are valued less as the lives at risk increase. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8(3), 277–299. [23] Google Scholar
Fries, J. F, Bloch, D. A., Sharp, J. T., McShane, D. J., Spitz, P., Bluhm, G. B., Forrester, D., Genant, H., Gofton, P., Richman, S., et al. (1986). Assessment of radiologic progression in rheumatoid arthritis. A randomized, controlled trial. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 29, 1–9. [40] Google Scholar
Frisch, D. (1993). Reasons for froming effects. Organizational Behavior Human Decision Processes, 54, 399–429. [24] Google Scholar
Froome, & Essen. (1973). The Velocity of Light and Radio Waves. London: Academic. [37] Google Scholar
Froot, K. A., & Frankel, J. A. (1989). Forward discount bias: Is it an exchange risk premium? Quarterly Journal of Economics, 104, 139–161. [38] Google Scholar
Funder, D. C. (1982). On the accuracy of dispositional vs. situational attributions. Social Cognition, 1, 205–222. [20] Google Scholar
Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90. [24] Google Scholar
Funtowicz, S., & Ravetz, J. (1990). Uncertainty and quality in science for policy. London: Kluwer. [41] Google Scholar
Furnham, A. F (1988). Lay theories. London: Pergamon Press. [41] Google Scholar
Fussell, S. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1989). The effects of intended audience on message production and comprehension: reference in a common ground framework. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 203–219. [8] Google Scholar
Gabrielcik, A., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). Priming and frequency estimation: A strict test of the availability heuristic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 85- 89. [5] Google Scholar
Gaeth, G. J., & Shanteau, J. (1984). Reducing the influence of irrelevant information on experienced decision makers. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 33, 263. [37] Google Scholar
Gallagher, D., & Clore, G. L. (1985, May). Effects of fear and anger on judgments of risk and blame. Paper presented at the meetings of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. [29] Google Scholar
Ganzach, Y. (1996). Preference reversals in equal-probability gambles: A case for anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 92, 95–109. [6] Google Scholar
Ganzach, Y. (2000). Judging risk and return of financial assets. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83, 353–370. [23] Google Scholar
Ganzach, Y., & Krantz, D. H. (1990). The psychology of moderate prediction. I. Experience with multiple determination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47, 177–204. [2] Google Scholar
Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. New York: Wiley. [20] Google Scholar
Garner, W. R. (1970). Good patterns have few alternatives. American Scientist, 58, 34–42. [20] Google Scholar
Gati, I., & Tversky A. (1982). Representations of qualitative and quantitative dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 325–340. [1] Google Scholar
Gerard, H. B., & Mathewson, G. C. (1966). The effects of severity of initiation on liking for a group: A replication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 278–287. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., & Warner, T. D. (1991). Effects of reviewing risk-relevant behavior on perceived vulnerability among women Marines. Health Psychology, 10, 173–179. [17] Google Scholar
Gerrard, M., & Luus, C. A. E. (1995). Judgments of vulnerability to pregnancy: The role of risk factors and individual differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 160–171. [19] Google Scholar
Gettys, C. E., Mehle, T., & Fisher, S. (1986). Plausibility assessments in hypothesis generation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 14–33. [25] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1991a). How to make cognitive illusions disappear: Beyond “heuristics and biases.” In European Review of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 83–115), W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley. [2,14] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1991b). From tools to theories: A heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology. Psychological Review, 98, 254–267. [Introduction, 24, 27] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1991c). How to make cognitive illusions disappear: Beyond heuristics and biases. European Review of Social Psychology, 2, 83–115. [Introduction, 24] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1993). The bounded rationality of probabilistic mental models. In Rationality: Psychological and philosophical perspectives (pp. 284–313), K. Manktelow & D. Over (Eds.). London: Routledge. [24] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1994). Why the distinction between single-event probabilities and frequencies is important for psychology (and vice versa). In Subjective probability (pp. 129–161), G. Wright & P. Ayton (Eds.). New York: Wiley. [Introduction, 14, 39] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1996). On narrow norms and vague heuristics: A reply to Kahneman and Tversky (1996). Psychological Review, 103, 592–596. [24] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Ecological intelligence: An adaptation for frequencies. In The evolution of mind (pp. 9–29), D. Dellarosa Cummins & C. Allen (Eds.). New York: Oxford University Press. [Introduction, 14] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–669. [2, 24, 30, 31] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102, 684–704. [2, 24] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Hell, W., & Blank, H. (1988). Presentation and content - the use of base rates as a continuous variable. Journal of Experimental Psychology - Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 513–525. [2,13] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Kleinbolting, H. (1991). Probabilistic mental models: A Brunswikian theory of confidence. Psychological Review, 98, 506–528. [13, 31, 39] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Swijtink, Z., Porter, T., Daston, L., Beatty, J., & Krtiger, L. (1989). The empire of chance: How probability changed science and even life. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [31] Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart, New York: Oxford University Press. [Introduction, 2, 30, 31] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In Unintended thought (pp. 189–211), J. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.). New York: Guilford. [2,7] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. (1990). Unbelieving the unbelievable: Some problems in the rejection of false information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 601–613. [6] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46, 107–119. [2, 6,9,10,16] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. (1993). The assent of man: Mental representation and the control of belief. In The handbook of mental control (pp. 57–87), D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker, (Eds.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [9,10] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. (1999). What the mind's not. In Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 3–11), S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.). New York: Guilford. [2, 9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 227–236. [9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J., & Wilson, T. D. (in press). The future is now: Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. [16] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D. S., & Malone, P. S. (1990). Unbelieving the unbelievable: Some problems in the rejection of false information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 601–613. [9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1988). Of thoughts unspoken: Social inference and the self-regulation of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 685–694. [9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., McNulty S. E., Giuliano, T. A., & Benson, J. E. (1992). Blurry words and fuzzy deeds: The attribution of obscure behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 18–25. [9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38. [9, 36] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Miller, A. G., & Ross, L. (1998). Speeding with Ned: A personal view of the correspondence bias. In Attribution and social interaction: the legacy of Edward E. Jones (pp. 5–66), J. M. Darley & J. Cooper (Eds.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [6] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Osborne, R. E. (1989). Thinking backward: Some curable and incurable consequences of cognitive busyness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 940–949. [8, 9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet person receivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740. [6, 8,9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can't not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 221–233. [6, 9] Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2000). Miswanting: Some problems in the forecasting of future affective states. In Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 178–198), J. Forgas (Ed.). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. [14,16] Google Scholar
Gilboa, I., & Schmeidler, D. (1994). Additive representations of non-additive measures and the Choquet integral. Annals of Operation Research, 51, 43–65. [25] Google Scholar
Gill, M., & Gilbert, D. T. (1999). Sequential operations in self-perception. Unpublished data. [9] Google Scholar
Gillies, G. T. (1983). The Newtonian Gravitational Constant: An Index of Measurements (BIPM-83/1), Bureau International des Poids et Mesures. [37] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluation and persistence in gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1110–1126. [18,19, 33] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (1990). Differential construal and the false consensus effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 623–634. [36] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn't so: The fallibility of human judgment in everyday life, New York: Free Press. [Introduction, 34, 41] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (2000, June). Motivated skepticism and motivated credulity: Differential standards of evidence in the evaluation of desired and undesired propositions. Address presented at the 12th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Society, Miami Beach, Florida. [27] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Kerr, M., & Medvec, V. H. (1993). Effect of temporal perspective on subjective confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 552–560. [19] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379–395. [16] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Chen, S. (1995). Omission, commission, and dissonance reduction: Coping with regret in the “Monty Hall” problem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 182–190. [21] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky K. (2000). The spotlight effect in social judgment: An egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of one's own actions and appearance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 211–222. [7] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., & Savitsky, K. (1999). The spotlight effect and the illusion of transparency: egocentric assessments of how we're seen by others. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 165–168. [7, 8] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). The illusion of transparency: Biased assessments of others' ability to read our emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332–346. [7,8] Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R. P., & Tversky A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295–314. [35] Google Scholar
Girotto, V., & Gonzalez, M. (2001). Solving probabilistic and statistical problems: A matter of information structure and question form. Cognition, 78, 247–276. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Glaser, R. (1990). Expertise. In The Blackwell dictionary of cognitive psychology (p. 139), M. W. Eysenk, A. N. Ellis, E. Hunt, & P. Johnson-Laird (Eds.). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell Reference. [39] Google Scholar
Gleicher, F, Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., Hetts, J., & Ahn, M. (1995). With an eye toward the future: The impact of counterfactual thinking on affect, attitudes, and behavior. In What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 283–304), N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [21] Google Scholar
Goldaper, S. (1990, June 9). Pistons put the game on the line. The New York Times, p. C46. [21] Google Scholar
Goldberg, J. H., Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Rage and reason: The psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 781–795. [32] Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1965). “Diagnosticians vs. diagnostic signs: The diagnosis of psychosis vs. neurosis from the MMPI,” Psychological Monographs, 79 (9, Whole No. 602). [40] Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1968). Simple models or simple processes? Some research on clinical judgment. American Psychologist, 23, 483–496. [40, 41] Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1970). Man versus model of man: A rationale, plus some evidence, for a method of improving on clinical inferences. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 422–432. [40] Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (1978). Epistemics: The regulative theory of cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 55, 509–523. [24] Google Scholar
Goldsmith, R. W. (1978). Assessing probabilities of compound events in a judicial context. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 19, 103–110. [1] Google Scholar
Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (1996). Satisficing inference and the perks of ignorance. In Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 137–141), G. W. Cottrell (Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [31] Google Scholar
Goldstein, W. M. (1984). The relationship between judgment and choice. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. [23] Google Scholar
Goldstein, W. M., & Einhom, H. J. (1987). Expression theory and the preference reversal phenomena. Psychological Review, 94, 236–254. [6,12, 23] Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141–185), W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.). London: John Wiley & Sons. [16] Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. [14] Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstatter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199. [14] Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative and implemental mindsets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119–1127. [16] Google Scholar
Gonzales, M., & Bonini, N. (1995). Probability judgments in two-outcome situations: What induces a defect in complementarity? Unpublished manuscript, Centre de Recherche en Psychologie Cognitive, University de Provence, Aix-en-Provence, France. [26] Google Scholar
Good, I. J. (1971). The probabilistic explication of information, evidence surprise, causality, explanation, and utility. In Foundations of statistical inference: Proceedings on the foundations of statistical inference, V. P. Godambe & D. A. Sprott (Eds.). Toronto, Ontario: Holt, Rinchart & Winston. [1] Google Scholar
Goodman-Delahunty J., Granhag, P. A., & Loftus, E. F. (1998). How well can lawyers predict their chances of success? Unpublished manuscript, University of Washington. [39] Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1992). Why the child's theory of mind is really a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145–171. [8] Google Scholar
Gough, P. B. (1965). Grammatical transformations and speed of understanding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 107–111. [9] Google Scholar
Gough, P. B. (1966). The verification of sentences: the effects of delay of evidence and sentence length. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 492–496. [9] Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1991). Bully for brontosaurus. Reflections in natural history. New York: Norton. [2, 22] Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1995). Dinosaur in a haystack: Reflections in natural history. New York: Harmony Books. [42] Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1997). The pleasures of pluralism. New York Review of Books, June 26. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Grayson, C. E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Beliefs influence information processing strategies: declarative and experiential information in risk assessment. Social Cognition, 17, 1–18. [5] Google Scholar
Green, A. E., & Bourne, A. J. (1972). Reliability technology. New York: Wiley. [37] Google Scholar
Green, D., Jacowitz, K. E., Kahneman, D., & McFadden, D. (1998). Referendum contingent valuation, anchoring, and willingness to pay for public goods. Resource and Energy Economics, 20, 85–116. [6] Google Scholar
Green, M., Visser, P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Copingwith accountability cross-pressures: Low-effort evasive tactics and high-effort quests for complex compromises. Unpublished manuscript, The Ohio State University. [32] Google Scholar
Greene, S. B., Gerrig, R. J., McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1994). Unheralded pronouns and management by common ground. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 511–526. [8] Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618. [16] Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. [16] Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., Carnot, C. G., Beach, R., & Young, B. (1987). Increasing voter behavior by asking people if the expect to vote. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 315–318. [19] Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. [10] Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., Pratkanis, A. R., Leippe, M. R., & Baumgardner, M. H. (1986). Under what conditions does theory obstruct research progress? Psychological Review, 93, 216–229. [40] Google Scholar
Gregory, W. L., Cialdini, R. B., & Carpenter, K. M. (1982). Self-relevant scenarios as mediators of likelihood estimates and compliance: Does imagining make it so? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 89–99. [4, 17] Google Scholar
Grether, D. M. (1980). Bayes' rule as a descriptive model: The representativeness heuristic. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 95, 537–557. [13] Google Scholar
Grether, D. M. (1992). Testing Bayes' rule and the representativeness heuristic: Some experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 17, 31–57. [13] Google Scholar
Grether, D. M., & Plott, C. R. (1979). Economic theory of choice and the preference reversal phenomenon. American Economic Review, 69, 623–638. [Introduction, 12] Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3. Speech acts, (pp. 41–58), P. Cole and J. L. Morgan (Eds.). New York: Academic Press. [1, 6,8, 20, 32] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., & Buehler, R. (1999). Frequency, probability, and prediction: Easy solutions to cognitive illusions? Cognitive Psychology, 38, 48–78. [Introduction, 14, 19] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., Dunning, D., & Ross, L. (1990). The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1128–1139. [14, 16] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., Gonzalez, R., & Varey, C. (2001). The heuristics and biases approach to judgment under uncertainty. In Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intrapersonal processes, N. Schwarz & A. Tesser (Eds.). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell Publishers. [8, 27, 34, 39] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., & Kahneman, D. (in press). Judgmental heuristics: Human strength or human weakness? In The Psychology of Human Strengths (pp. 319–359), L. Aspinwall & U. Staudinger (Eds.). APA Books. [Introduction] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference and human misunderstanding. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 319–359), M. P. Zanna (Ed.). New York: Academic Press. [Introduction, 8,16, 42] Google Scholar
Griffin, D. W., & Tversky, A. (1992). The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 411–435. [Introduction, 2,6,8,14,15, 27,39] Google Scholar
Griffin, D., & Varey, C. (1996). Towards a consensus on overconfidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 227–231. [39] Google Scholar
Griffith, R. M. (1949). Odds adjustments by American horse-race bettors. American Journal of Psychology, 62, 290–294. [13] Google Scholar
Griffth, R. W., & Rogers, R. W. (1976). Effects of fear-arousing components of driver education on students' safety attitudes and simulator performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 501–506. [17] Google Scholar
Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason's selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 407–420. [24] Google Scholar
Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1983). The effects of problem content and negation on Wason's selection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 519–533. [24] Google Scholar
Grove, W. M., & Meehl, P. E. (1996). Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective, impressionistic) and formal (mechanical, algorithmic) prediction procedures: The clinical/statistical controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2, 1–31. [40] Google Scholar
Grove, W. M., Zald, D. H., Lebow, B. S., Snitz, B. E., & Nelson, C. (2000). Clinical vs. mechanical prediction: A meta-analysis. Psychological Assessment, 12, 19–30. [40] Google Scholar
Guilmette, T. J., Faust, D., Hart, K., & Arkes, H. R. (1990). A national survey of psychologists who offer neuropsychological services. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 373–392. [40] Google Scholar
Gunther, A. C. (1995). Overrating the X-rating: The third-person perception and support for censorship of pornography. Journal of Communication, 45, 26–39. [36] Google Scholar
Gur, R. C, & Sackheim, H. A. (1979). Self-deception: A concept in search of a phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 147–169. [16] Google Scholar
Gardenfors, P., & Sahlin, N. E. (Eds.). (1989). Decision, probability and utility. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [41] Google Scholar
Haddock, G., Rothman, A. J., Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Forming judgments of attitude certainty, importance, and intensity: the role of subjective experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 771–782. [5] Google Scholar
Hagafors, R., & Brehmer, B. (1983). Does having to justify one's judgments change the nature of the judgment process? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 223–232. [32] Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S., & Dias, M. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality: Or, is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 613–628. [11] Google Scholar
Haidt, J., McCauley C. R., & Rozin, P. (1994). A scale to measure disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713. [11] Google Scholar
Hakmiller, K. L. (1966). Threat as a determinant of downward comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Supplement 1, 32–39. [17] Google Scholar
Halbower, C. C. (1955). A comparison of actuarial vs. clinical prediction to classes discriminated by the Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Minnesota. [40] Google Scholar
Hall, P. (1980). Great planning disasters. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. [14] Google Scholar
Hallenback, C. (1920). Forecasting precipitation in percentages of probability. Monthly Weather Review, 645–647. [39] Google Scholar
Hamer, C. (1981). “Good-fishin!” (2nd ed.). St. Paul, MN: P. F., Inc. [28] Google Scholar
Hammond (1996). Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty, Inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. New York: Oxford. [2,39] Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1955). Probabilistic functioning and the clinical method. Psychological Review, 62, 255–262. [2] Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., & Brehmer, B. (1973). Quasi-rationality and distrust: Implications for international conflict. In Human judgment and social interaction, L. Rappoport & D. A. Summers (Eds.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [1] Google Scholar
Hammond, K., Hursch, C, & Todd, F. (1964). Analyzing the components of Clinical Inference. Psychological Review, 71, 438. [39] Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., & Summers, D. A. (1965). Cognitive dependence on linear and nonlinear cues. Psychological Review, 72, 215–224. [40] Google Scholar
Hampson, S. E., John, O. P., & Goldberg. L. R. (1986). Category breadth and hierarchical structure in personality: Studies of asymmetries in judgments of trait implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 37–54. [18] Google Scholar
Hansen, R. D., & Donoghue, J. M. (1977). The power of consensus: Information derived from one's own and other's behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 294–302. [20] Google Scholar
Hanslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and magic. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 316–330. [34] Google Scholar
Hanson, J. D., & Kysar, D. A. (1999a). Taking behavioralism seriously: Some evidence of market manipulation. Harvard Law Review, 112(7), 1420–1572. [23, 41] Google Scholar
Hanson, J. D., & Kysar, D. A. (1999b). Taking behavioralism seriously: The problem of market manipulation. New York University Law Review, 74 (3), 630–749. [23, 41] Google Scholar
Hanson, J. D., & Kysar, D. A. (2001). The joint failure of economic theory and legal regulation. In Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 229–276), P. Slovic (Ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [23] Google Scholar
Harari, H., & McDavid, J. W. (1973). Name stereotypes and teachers' expectations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 222–225. [23] Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1965). Psychopathy, fear arousal and anticipated pain. Psychological Reports, 16, 499–502. [23]