Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-n9wrp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T01:06:37.610Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Brain mechanisms and learning of high level skills

from Part II - Brain development, cognition, and education

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Michael I. Posner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology University of Oregon
Mary K. Rothbart
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology University of Oregon
M. Rosario Rueda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology University of Oregon
Antonio M. Battro
Affiliation:
National Academy of Education, Argentina
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Pierre J. Léna
Affiliation:
Université de Paris VII (Denis Diderot)
Get access

Summary

Overview

Two significant scientific contributions, brain images and gene sequences, are helping scientists and educators to understand individual differences in the acquisition of cognitive and emotional skills. Research on brain mechanisms of attention in children and adults shows three independent neuronal networks for alerting, orienting, and executive control in conflict resolution. The authors' attention network test evaluates the efficiency of each of these three systems and reveals clear individual differences, which relate to evaluations of children's ability to regulate their own behavior. Among factors affecting this capacity for regulation are two genes related to the neurotransmitter dopamine, which relate to both performance and activation of a node of the attention network in the anterior cingulate. From early childhood, effortful control of attention plays a role not only in learning but also in socialization and empathy. Understanding attention regulation and its relation to learning will clearly be informative to teachers and parents.

The Editors

Two major developments have greatly altered the prospects for making a connection between common networks of the human brain and theories of how people differ. First, with the development of neuroimaging, we could glimpse inside the human brain as people think (Posner & Raichle, 1994). When combined with electrical or magnetic recording from outside the skull, it was possible to see in real time the circuits involved in computing aspects of a task.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Educated Brain
Essays in Neuroeducation
, pp. 151 - 165
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen, R. A. (1989). Visual eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 12, 377–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., and Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, RC 165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., and Dolan, R. J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expression of sadness and anger. Brain, 1222, 883–893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4/6, 215–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. T. and Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control in children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Development of the functional visual field. Acta Psychologica, 106, 51–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M. and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 201–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Driver, J., Eimer, M., and Macaluso, E. (2004). Neurobiology of human spatial attention: Modulation, generation, and integration. In Kanwisher, N. and Duncan, J. (eds.), Attention and Performance XX: Functional Brain Imaging of Visual Cognition (pp. 267–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., Newell, F. N., and Emslie, H. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457–460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2004). Individual differences in executive attention predict self-regulation and adolescent psychosocial behaviors. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1–21, 331–340.Google Scholar
Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain mechanisms of conflict. Neuroimage, 18, 42–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, J., Fossella, J. A., Summer, T., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 7406–7411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, M., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Wu, Y., Fossella, J., and Posner, M. I. (2001). Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BioMed Central Neuroscience, 2, 14.Google ScholarPubMed
Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J. M., Pfaff, D. W., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24–36 months of age. Developmental Science, 3/4, 397–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, M. M., Hazan, C., and Goodman, G. S. (1988). Expectation and anticipation of dynamic visual events by 3.5 month-old babies. Child Development, 59, 467–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karnath, H-O., Ferber, S., and Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature, 411, 95–953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. and Kaufman, N. L. (1990). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test – Manual Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Kerns, K. A., Esso, K., and Thompson, J. (1999). Investigation of a direct intervention for improving attention in young children with ADHD. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 273–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., and Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781–791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G. (1995). Children's temperament, mothers' discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., and Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internationalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marrocco, R. T. and Davidson, M. C. (1998). Neurochemistry of attention. In Parasuraman, R. (ed.), The Attention Brain (pp. 35–50). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mills, D. and Mills, C. (2000). Hungarian Kindergarten Curriculum Translation. London Mills Production Limited.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Kossyln, S. M., Cosgrove, G. R., Cassem, E. H., Price, B. H., Nierenberg, A. A., and Rauch, S. L. (2001). Deficits in visual cognition and attention following bilateral anterior cingulotomy. Neuropsychologia, 39, 219–230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The 7th Sir F. C. Bartlett Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. and Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of Mind. Scientific American Books.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the Human Brain. Washington DC: APA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K. and Sheese, B. E. (2007). Attention genes. Developmental Science, 10, 24–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cajal, Ramón y S. (1937). Recollection of My Life. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., and Hershey, K. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 21–39.Google Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Pappert, L., and Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1028–1040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of the time course for resolving conflict. Poster presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B., Saccamanno, L. and Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proc. U.S. National Acad. of Sciences, 102, 14931–14936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumbaugh, D. M. and Washburn, D. A. (1995). Attention and memory in relation to learning: A comparative adaptation perspective. In Lyon, G. R., and Krasengor, N. A. (eds.), Attention, Memory and Executive Function (pp. 199–219). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Nielsen, K. H., and Clinton, A. (1999). An intervention approach for children with teacher and parent-identified attentional difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 581–589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, L., Tsal, Y., and Mevorach, C. (2003). Progressive attentional training program: Effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Proceedings of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, New York, pp. 55–56.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1969). The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1903). Educational Psychology. New York: Teachers College.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veen, V. and Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593–602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venter, J. C., Adams, M. D., Myers, E. W., Li, P. W., Mural, R. J., et al., (2001). The sequence of the human genome. Science, 291, 1304–1335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washburn, D. A. (1994). Stroop-like effects for monkeys and humans: Processing speed or strength of association? Journal of Psychological Science, 5(6), 375–379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×