Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g5fl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-26T18:12:18.018Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Acquiring Conceptual Expertise from Modeling: The Case of Elementary Physics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Get access

Summary

In many domains, the real world is modeled with systems of equations. Such a model uses variables to represent domain properties and equations to represent applications of domain principles. Given a set of true domain relationships expressed as equations, one can deduce new equations from them using only the rules of mathematics, and the new equations will also be true domain relationships. The latter step, wherein mathematical implications are derived from the initial model, can often be done mechanically, for example, by mathematical symbol manipulation programs, spreadsheets, calculators, etc.

Given a real-world situation that is amenable to such analysis, experts and novices understand them quite differently. Whereas novices must go through the whole modeling process by writing equations on paper and solving them, experts can generate many conclusions about the same situations without having to commit anything to paper. For the expert, many domain relationships are just “obvious” or can be easily inferred “by inspection.”

There are limits to the experts' abilities. Although experts usually cannot mentally infer quantitative relationships, such as the exact numerical value for an energy or a velocity, they can infer qualitative relationships, such as whether a quantity is zero, increasing or greater than some other quantity. Thus, it is often said that expertise in such domains is characterized by a conceptual or qualitative understanding of real world situations (VanLehn, 1996). It is sometimes said that they have developed domain-specific intuitions (Simon & Simon, 1978).

Type
Chapter
Information
Development of Professional Expertise
Toward Measurement of Expert Performance and Design of Optimal Learning Environments
, pp. 356 - 378
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashby, G. F., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). Human category learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 149–178.CrossRef
Bundy, A., Byrd, L., Luger, G., Mellish, C., & Palmer, M. (1979). Solving mechanics problems using meta-level inference. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Joint Conference on AI (pp. 1017–1027). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Chi, M., & VanLehn, K. (2008). Eliminating the gap between the high and low students through meta-cognitive strategy instruction. In Woolf, B. P., Aimeur, E., Nkambou, R., & Lajoie, S. P. (Eds.), Intelligent tutoring systems: 9th International Conference: ITS2008 (pp. 603–613). Amsterdam, NL: IOS Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Common sense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 161–199.CrossRef
Chi, M. T. H. (in press). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), Handbook of research on conceptual change. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5(2), 121–152.CrossRef
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, vol. 1 (pp. 7–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Corbett, A., Wagner, A. Z., Lesgold, S., Ulrich, H., & Stevens, S. M. (2006). The impact of learning of generating vs. selecting descriptions in analyzing algebra example solutions. In Barab, S. A., Hay, K. E., & Hickey, D. T. (Eds.), The 7th International Conference of the Learning Sciences (pp. 99–105). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Day, D. V., & Lord, R. G. (1992). Expertise and problem categorization: The role of expert processing in organizational sense-making. Journal of Management Studies, 29(1), 35–47.CrossRef
Kleer, J., & Brown, J. S. (1984). A qualitative physics based on confluences. Artificial Intelligence, 24, 7–83.CrossRef
Dee-Lucas, D., & Larkin, J. (1991). Equations in scientific proofs: Effects on comprehension. American Education Research Journal, 28(3), 661–682.CrossRef
di Sessa, A. A. (1993). Towards an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10(2 & 3), 105–225.CrossRef
Dufresne, R. J., Gerace, W. J., Hardiman, P. T., & Mestre, J. P. (1992). Constraining novices to perform expert-like problem analyses: Effects on schema acquisition. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(3), 307–331.CrossRef
Elio, R., & Scharf, P. B. (1990). Modeling novice-to-expert shifts in problem-solving strategy and knowledge organization. Cognitive Science, 14, 579–639.CrossRef
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). Protocol analysis and expert thought: Concurrent verbalizations of thinking during experts' performance on representative tasks\. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 223–241). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.CrossRef
Forbus, K. D. (1985). The role of qualitative dynamics in naive physics. In Hobbes, J. R. & Moore, R. C. (Eds.), Formal theories of the commonsense world (pp. 185–226). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Forbus, K. D., & Kleer, J. (1993). Building problem solvers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hake, R. R. (1998). Interactive-engagement vs. traditional methods: A six-thousand student survey of mechanics test data for introductory physics students. American Journal of Physics, 66(4), 64–74.CrossRef
Hardiman, P. T., Dufresne, R. J., & Mestre, J. P. (1989). The relation between problem categorization and problem solving among experts and novices. Memory & Cognition, 17(5), 627–638.CrossRef
Heller, J. L., & Reif, F. (1984). Prescribing effective human problem-solving processes: Problem descriptions in physics. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 177–216.CrossRef
Hestenes, D. (1987). Toward a modeling theory of physics instruction. American Journal of Physics, 55, 440–454.CrossRef
Hestenes, D., & Wells, M. (1992). A mechanics baseline test. The Physics Teacher, 30, 159–166.CrossRef
Hestenes, D., Wells, M., & Swackhamer, G. (1992). Force concept inventory. The Physics Teacher, 30, 141–158.CrossRef
Heyworth, R. M. (1999). Procedural and conceptual knowledge of expert and novice students for the solving of a basic problem in chemistry. International Journal of Science Education, 21(2), 195–211.CrossRef
Hinsley, D. A., Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1977). From words to equations: Meaning and representation in algebra word problems. In Carpenter, P. & Just, M. A. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Jones, R. M., & Fleischman, E. S. (2001). Cascade explains and informs the utility of fading examples to problems. In 23rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 459–464). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Jordan, P., Makatchev, M., Pappuswamy, U., VanLehn, K., & Albacete, P. (2006). A natural language tutorial dialogue system for physics. In Sutcliffe, G. & Goebel, R. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International FLAIRS Conference. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Klenk, M., & Forbus, K. D. (2007). Cognitive modeling of analogy events in physics problem solving from examples. In McNamara, D. S. & Trafton, J. G. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1163–1165). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Kozma, R. B., & Russell, J. (1997). Multimedia and understanding: Expert and novice responses to different representations of chemical phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34(9), 949–968.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRef
Lamberts, K. (1990). A hybrid model of learning to solve physics problems. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3(2), 151–170.CrossRef
Larkin, J. (1981). Enriching formal knowledge: A model for learning to solve textbook physics problems. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Larkin, J. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In Gentner, D. & Stevens, A. (Eds.), Mental models. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Models of competence in solving physics problems. Cognitive Science, 4, 317–345.CrossRef
Larkin, J., Reif, F., Carbonell, J., & Gugliotta, A. (1988). Fermi: A flexible expert reasoner with multi-domain inferencing. Cognitive Science, 12(1), 101–138.CrossRef
Loftin, B. R., Mueller, S., Way, B., & Lee, B. (1991). An intelligent tutoring system for physics problem solving. In Proceedings of the contributed sessions 1991 conference on intelligent computer-aided training (p. 127).
Mayfield, W. A., Kardash, C. M., & Kivlighan, D. M. (1999). Differences in experienced and novice counselors' knowledge structures about clients: Implications for case conceptualization. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 504–514.CrossRef
McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., & Green, B. (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: Naive beliefs about the motion of objects. Science, 210(5), 1139–1141.CrossRef
McDermott, J., & Larkin, J. H. (1978). Re-representing textbook physics problems. In Proceedings of the second national conference of the Canadian society for computational studies of intelligence. Toronto, Ontario.Google Scholar
Medin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. (1989). The specific character of abstract thought: Categorization, problem solving and induction. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 5 (pp. 189–223). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Muggleton, S. (1992). Inductive logic programming. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Murray, T., Schultz, K., Brown, D., & Clement, J. (1990). An analogy-based computer tutor for remediating physics misconceptions. Interactive Learning Environments, 1(2), 79–101.CrossRef
Novak, G. S., & Araya, A. A. (1980). Research on expert problem solving in physics. In Proceedings of the first national conference on Artificial Intelligence. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Owen, E., & Sweller, J. (1985). What do students learn while solving mathematics problems? Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(3), 272–284.CrossRef
Pfundt, H., & Duit, R. (1998). Bibliography: Students' alternative frameworks and science education, 5th ed. Kiel, Germany: Institute for Science Education.Google Scholar
Ploetzner, R., & VanLehn, K. (1997). The acquisition of informal physics knowledge during formal physics training. Cognition and Instruction, 15(2), 169–206.CrossRef
Priest, A. G., & Lindsay, R. O. (1992). New light on novice-expert differences in physics problem solving. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 389–405.CrossRef
Ranney, M., & Thagard, P. (1988). Explanatory coherence and belief revision in naive physics. In Patel, V. & Groen, G. J. (Eds.), The Tenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 426–432). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Reif, F., & Scott, L. A. (1999). Teaching scientific thinking skills: Students and computers coaching each other. American Journal of Physics, 67(9), 819–831.CrossRef
Reimann, P., Schult, T. J., & Wichmann, S. (1993). Understanding and using worked-out examples: A computational model. In Strube, G. & Wender, K. F. (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of knowledge (pp. 177–201). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, S., & Norvig, P. (2003). Artificial intelligence: A modern approach, 2nd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Shafto, P., & Coley, J. D. (2003). Development of categorization and reasoning in natural world: Novices to experts, naive similarity to ecological knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 641–649.
Sherin, B. L. (2001). How students understand physics equations. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19(4), 479–541.
Sherin, B. L. (2006). Common sense clarified: The role of intuitive knowledge in physics problem solving. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(6), 535–555.CrossRef
Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A. (1978). Individual differences in solving physics problems. In Siegler, R. S. (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops?Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Smith, M. U. (1992). Expertise and the organization of knowledge: Unexpected differences among genetic counselors, faculty, and students on problem categorization tasks. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(2), 179–205.CrossRef
Sweller, J., Mawer, R. F., & Ward, M. R. (1983). Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 629–661.
Heuvelen, A., & Zou, X. (2001). Multiple representations of work-energy processes. American Journal of Physics, 69(2), 184–194.CrossRef
VanLehn, K. (1996). Cognitive skill acquisition. In Spence, J., Darly, J., & Foss, D. J. (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 47 (pp. 513–539). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
VanLehn, K. (1998). Analogy events: How examples are used during problem solving. Cognitive Science, 22(3), 347–388.CrossRef
VanLehn, K. (1999). Rule learning events in the acquisition of a complex skill: An evaluation of Cascade. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(2), 179–221.CrossRef
VanLehn, K., & Jones, R. M. (1993a). Better learners use analogical problem solving sparingly. In Utgoff, P. E. (Ed.), Machine Learning: Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Conference (pp. 338–345). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
VanLehn, K. (1993b). Learning by explaining examples to oneself: A computational model. In Chipman, S. & Meyrowitz, A. (Eds.), Cognitive models of complex learning (pp. 25–82). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
VanLehn, K., Jones, R. M., & Chi, M. T. H. (1992). A model of the self-explanation effect. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(1), 1–59.CrossRef
VanLehn, K., Jordan, P., Rose, C. P., Bhembe, D., Bottner, M., Gaydos, A., et al. (2002). The architecture of Why2-Atlas: A coach for qualitative physics essay writing. In Cerri, S. A., Gouarderes, G., & Paraguacu, F. (Eds.), Intelligent Tutoring Systems, 2002, 6th International Conference (pp. 158–167). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
VanLehn, K., Lynch, C., Schultz, K., Shapiro, J. A., Shelby, R. H., Taylor, L., et al. (2005). The Andes physics tutoring system: Lessons learned. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence and Education, 15(3), 147–204.
Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the Earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.CrossRef
Weiser, M., & Shertz, J. (1983). Programming problem representation in novice and expert programmers. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 14, 391–396.CrossRef

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×