Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T06:47:15.987Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The gendered ‘I’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Clare Jackson
Affiliation:
University of York
Susan A. Speer
Affiliation:
University of Manchester
Elizabeth Stokoe
Affiliation:
Loughborough University
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In English, the word ‘I’ does not contain any categorical information about the speaker; neither gender, age, class nor race. It is what Schegloff (1996c: 440) calls ‘reference simpliciter’; it does simple self-reference and its use ‘masks the relevance of the referent and the reference for the talk’ (Schegloff, 1996c: 446) because it is ‘opaque with respect to all the usual key categorical dimensions – age, gender, status and the like’ (Schegloff, 2007c: 123). Building on Schegloff's cogent analysis, the key claim I wish to make in this chapter is that there are instances where a speaker's self-referential ‘I’ can be rendered hearably gendered in the context of its production. In other words, my data show instances in which the ordinary, unremarkable form of self-reference is itself hearably gendered, without the speaker's categorical membership being explicitly linguistically produced. I call this use of ‘I’ the gendered-I. Drawing on extracts from the CTS corpus, I show how the self-reference ‘I’ can be gendered.

I locate my work in the emerging discipline of feminist conversation analysis (see Kitzinger, 2000a; 2007a; Speer, 2002a; 2005a; Stokoe & Weatherall, 2002a) in which gendered identities (and others) are seen as emergent, locally occasioned and routinely constituted in interaction. Feminist conversation analysis resonates with ethnomethodological feminisms (Kitzinger, 2000a: 166), in which men/boys and women/girls are not regarded as always-and-forever talking as gendered beings, but rather may produce themselves or be produced as gendered in the taken-for-granted, routinized details of interaction (see Garfinkel, 1967; Kessler & McKenna, 1978: 136; West & Zimmerman, 1987: 13–14).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×