Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-768ffcd9cc-rq46b Total loading time: 3.636 Render date: 2022-12-04T12:01:31.769Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "useRatesEcommerce": false } hasContentIssue true

Part VI - Language Disorders, Interventions, and Instruction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Macao Polytechnic University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Acheson, D. J., Hamidi, M., Binder, J. R., & Postle, B. R. (2011). A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (6), 13581367. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21519CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M., E., & Boyle, M. O. (2002). Individual differences in working memory within a nomological network of cognitive and perceptual speed abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 567589. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.4.567CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85106. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Attout, L., & Majerus, S. (2015). Working memory deficits in developmental dyscalculia: The importance of serial order. Child Neuropsychology, 21(4), 432450. doi:10.1080/09297049.2014.922170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., Eldridge, M., Lewis, V., & Thomson, N. (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 518540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language-learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.105.1.158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The multiple-component model. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 2861). Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.00CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., & Ersland, L. (2009). Dyslexic children show short-term memory deficits in phonological storage and serial rehearsal: An fMRI study. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(11), 20172043. doi:10.1080/00207450903139671CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. (2010). Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 192202. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00808.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bledowski, C., Kaiser, J., & Rahm, B. (2010). Basic operations in working memory: Contributions from functional imaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 214(2), 172179. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Büttner, G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2015). Working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading versus spelling: Searching for overlapping and specific cognitive factors. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(6), 622634. doi:10.1177/0022219414521665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chein, J. M., & Fiez, J. A. (2010). Evaluating models of working memory through the effects of concurrent irrelevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 117137. doi:10.1037/a0018200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1997). The development of working memory. In Cowan, N. (Ed.), The development of working memory in childhood (pp. 163199). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 197223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450466. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Weerdt, F., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2013). Working memory in children with reading disabilities and/or mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 461472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, T. M., Flowers, D. L., Luetje, M. M., Napoliello, E., & Eden, G. F. (2016). Functional neuroanatomy of arithmetic and word reading and its relationship to age. NeuroImage, 143, 304315. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fürst, A. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2000). Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmeticMemory & Cognition28(5), 774782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 15391552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C. (2013). Learning disabilities in mathematics: Recent advances. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 239256). Guilford.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Bailey, D. H. (2012). Mathematical cognition deficits in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: A five-year prospective study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 206223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T., Kuo, T., Brinkley, S., & Cowan, N. (2017). The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 183201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, S. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2001). The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computational skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(2), 192227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutton, U. M. Z., & Towse, J. N. (2001). Short-term memory and working memory as indices of children’s cognitive skills. Memory, 9(4–6), 383394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufmann, L., Wood, G., Rubinsten, O., & Henik, A. (2011). Meta-analyses of developmental fMRI studies investigating typical and atypical trajectories of number processing and calculation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(6), 763787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudo, M. F., Lussier, C. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2015). Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity? Intelligence,14, 389433. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. S., Humphrey, M., Mellard, D. F., Woods, K., & Swanson, H. L. (2010). Cognitive processing deficits and students with specific learning disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Learning Disability Quarterly, 33(1), 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanfranchi, S., & Swanson, H. L. (2005). Short-term memory and working memory in children as a function of language-specific knowledge in English and Spanish. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(4), 299319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locascio, G., Mahone, E. M., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficitsJournal of Learning Disabilities43(5), 441454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logie, R. H., Gilhooly, K. J., & Wynn, V. (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 22(4), 395410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, M. J., Schneider, J. M., Middleton, A. E., Ralph, Y., Lopez, M., Ackerman, R. A., & Abel, A. D. (2018). Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade schoolJournal of Experimental Child Psychology166, 679695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mammarella, I. C., Caviola, S., Giofrè, D., & Szűcs, D. (2018). The underlying structure of visuospatial working memory in children with mathematical learning disability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology36(2), 220235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(3), 240260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Assessment of Educational Progress (2017). The condition of education (update 2017). US Department of Education.Google Scholar
National Center for Education Statistics (2020) Common Core of Data (CCD): Local education agency universe surveyU.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Krawitz, A., & Jonides, J. (2013). A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23(2), 264282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2004). Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88(4), 348367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Kester, E. S. (2016). Assessment of language impairment in bilingual children using semantic tasks: Two languages classify better than one. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 192202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin144(1), 4876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., Congying, S., Beilei, L., & Sha, T. (2012). Phonological storage and executive function deficits in children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 452466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., & Fuchs, D. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: Is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 320. doi:10.1177/0022219414521667CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richlan, F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: Dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. NeuroImage, 56(3), 17351742. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., & Vélez-Uribe, I. (2016). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 489503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg-Lee, M., Lovett, M. C., & Anderson, J. R. (2009). Neural correlates of arithmetic calculation strategies. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(3), 270285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B, Accardo, P., & Capute, A (Eds.). Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75119). York Press.Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2010). Does working memory training generalize? Psychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 245276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60(4), 973980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 16571661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1981). Vigilance deficit in learning disabled children: A signal detection analysis. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 22(4), 393399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1983). A developmental study of vigilance in learning-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11(3), 415429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1988). Learning disabled children’s problem solving: Identifying mental processes underlying intelligent performance. Intelligence, 12(3), 261278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (1989). The effects of central processing strategies on learning disabled, mildly retarded, average, and gifted children’s elaborative encoding abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47(3), 370397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1992). Generality and modifiability of working memory among skilled and less skilled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 473-488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993a). An information processing analysis of learning-disabled children’s problem solving. American Educational Research Journal, 30(4), 861893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993b). Executive processing in learning-disabled readers. Intelligence, 17(2), 117149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993c). Working memory in learning disability subgroups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(1), 87114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1995). S-Cognitive Processing Test (S-CPT): A dynamic assessment measure. PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1996). Individual and age-related differences in children’s working memory. Memory & Cognition, 24, 7082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (1999). What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 9861000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (2000). Are working memory deficits in readers with learning disabilities hard to change? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(6), 551566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (2004). Working memory and phonological processing as predictors of children’s mathematical problem solving at different ages. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 648661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (2008). Working memory and intelligence in children: What develops? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 581602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (2011). The influence of working memory growth on reading and math performance in children with math and/or reading disabilities. In Barrouillet, P & Gaillard, V (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian theories and cognitive approaches (pp. 203231). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2012). Intellectual growth in children as a function of domain specific and domain general working memory subgroups. Intelligence, 39, 481492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (2014). Does cognitive strategy training on word problems compensate for working memory capacity in children with math difficulties? Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 831848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (2016). Word problem solving, working memory and serious math difficulties: Do cognitive strategies really make a difference? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(4), 368383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. (2017). Verbal and visual-spatial working memory: What develops over a life span? Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 971995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H (2020). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit: A revised model. In Sperling, R., Martin, A., & Newton, K, Handbook of educational psychology & students with special needs (pp. 1951). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Alexander, J. E. (1997). Cognitive processes as predictors of word recognition and reading comprehension in learning-disabled and skilled readers: Revisiting the specificity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 128158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Alloway, T. P. (2012). Working memory, learning, and academic achievement. APA educational psychology handbook. Vol 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 327366). American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Ashbaker, M. H. (2000). Working memory, short-term memory, speech rate, word recognition and reading comprehension in learning disabled readers: Does the executive system have a role? Intelligence, 28(1), 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Ashbaker, M. H., & Lee, C. (1996). Learning-disabled readers’ working memory as a function of processing demands. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(3), 242275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., & Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004). The relationship between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not a risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 471491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Berninger, V. (1995). The role of working memory in skilled and less skilled readers’ comprehension. Intelligence, 21(1), 83108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Fung, W. (2016). Working memory components and problem-solving accuracy: Are there multiple pathways? Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(8), 11531177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Howard, C. B., & Sáez, L. (2006). Do different components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(3), 252269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., & Jerman, O. (2006). Math disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 76(2), 249274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 249283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Jerman, O., & Zheng, X. (2008). Growth in working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 343379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Jerman, O., & Zheng, X. (2009). Math disabilities and reading disabilities: Can they be separated? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27 (3), 175196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Kehler, P., & Jerman, O. (2010). Working memory, strategy knowledge, and strategy instruction in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(1), 2447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35(2), 151168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J., & Petcu, S. (2018). Math difficulties and working memory growth in English language learner children: Does bilingual proficiency play a significant role? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools49(3), 379394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Kong, J. E., & Petcu, S. D. (2019a). Individual differences in math problem solving and executive processing among emerging bilingual childrenJournal of Experimental Child Psychology187, 25.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J., & Petcu, S. D. (2019b) Growth in math computation among monolingual and English language learners: Does the executive system have a role? Developmental Neuropsychology44(8), 566593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Kong, J, & Petcu, S. (2020) Emerging bilingual children at risk and not at risk for math difficultiesChild Neuropsychology26(4), 489517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Kudo, M., & Guzman-Orth, D. (2016). Cognition and literacy in English language learners at risk for reading disabilities: A latent transition analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(6), 830856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Kudo, M. F., & Van Horn, M. L. (2019c). Does the structure of working memory in EL children vary across age and two language systems? Memory27(2), 174191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Lussier, C. M., & Orosco, M. J. (2015). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 339358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Lussier, C. M., & Orosco, M. J. (2015). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 339358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., & McMurran, M. (2018). The impact of working memory training on near and far transfer measures: Is it all about fluid intelligence? Child Neuropsychology24(3), 370395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Moran, A. S., Bocian, K., Lussier, C., & Zheng, X. (2013). Generative strategies, working memory, and word problem solving accuracy in children at risk for math disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(4), 203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Moran, A., Lussier, C., & Fung, W. (2014). The effect of explicit and direct generative strategy training and working memory on word problem-solving accuracy in children at risk for math difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 37(2), 111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Olide, A. F., & Kong, J. E. (2018). Latent class analysis of children with math difficulties and/or math learning disabilities: Are there cognitive differences? Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(7), 931951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Kudo, M. (2017). Does growth in the executive system of working memory underlie growth in literacy for bilingual children with and without reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(4), 386407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Lussier, C. M. (2012). Cognition and literacy in English language learners at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(2), 302320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Lussier, C. M. (2015). Growth in literacy, cognition, and working memory in English language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 155188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., Lussier, C. M., Gerber, M. M., & Guzman-Orth, D. (2011). The influence of working memory and phonological processing on English language learner children’s bilingual reading and language acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 838856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001a). A subgroup analysis of working memory in children with reading disabilities: Domain-general or domain-specific deficiency? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(3), 249263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001b). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(3), 294321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H., Sáez, L., Gerber, M., & Leafstedt, J. (2004). Literacy and cognitive functioning in bilingual and nonbilingual children at or not at risk for reading disabilitiesJournal of Educational Psychology96(1), 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Siegel, L. (2001a). Elaborating on working memory and learning disabilities: A reply to commentators. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 7, 107129.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Siegel, L. (2001b). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 7, 148.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Zheng, X. (2013). Memory difficulties in children and adults with learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K., & Graham, S. (Eds), Handbook of learning disabilities (2nd ed, pp. 214238). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(3), 260287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uppal, H. K., & Swanson, H. L. (2016). Teachers’ ratings of working memory in English language learners: Do laboratory measures predict classroom analogues? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(6), 871884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 133(6), 10381066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: Memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 188197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review, 23, 275290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, E., Nguyen, A., & Cowan, N. (2018). Theories of working memory: differences in definition, degree of modularity, role of attention and purpose. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 340355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, G., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, G., Ogden, J., & Halford, G. (2017). Resolving conflicts between syntax and plausibility in sentence comprehension. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 13, 1127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L., & Gathercole, S. (2007). The complexities of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 177194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Christiansen, M. (2017). The role of multiword building blocks in explaining L1-L2 differences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 621636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnon, I., & Clark, E. (2011). Why brush your teeth is better than teeth: Children’s word production is facilitated in familiar sentence-frames. Language Learning and Development, 7, 107129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2017). Digging up the building blocks of language: Age-of-acquisition effects for multiword phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 265280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2008). More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannard, C., & Lieven, E. (2012). Formulaic language in L1 acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning. Psychological Science, 19, 241248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2001). Developmental increase in working memory span: Resource sharing or temporal decay? Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrouillet, P., Gavens, N., Vergauwe, E., Gaillard, V., & Camos, V. (2009). Working memory span development: A time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borovsky, A., Elman, J., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 417436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, G., Gray, J., Conway, A., & Braver, T. (2011). Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 674692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chater, N., McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2016). Language as skill: Intertwining comprehension and production. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 244254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chein, J., Moore, A., & Conway, A. (2011). Domain-general mechanisms of complex working memory span. NeuroImage, 54, 550559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M., & Arnon, I. (2017). More than words: The role of multiword sequences in language learning and use. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 542551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M., & MacDonald, M. (1999). Fractionated working memory: Even in pebbles, it’s still a soup stone. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 9798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Faragher, N. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6, 741748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Ullman, M., & Lum, J. (2015). The relation between receptive grammar and procedural, declarative, and working memory in specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornish, H., Dale, R., Kirby, S., & Christiansen, M. (2017). Sequence memory constraints give rise to language-like structure through iterated learning. PLoS ONE, 12(1), Article e0168532. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., Rouder, J. Blume, C., & Saults, J. (2012). Models of verbal working memory capacity: What does it take to make them work? Psychological Review, 119, 480499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., Saults, J., & Blume, C. (2014). Central and peripheral components of working memory storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 18061836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delage, H., & Frauenfelder, U. (2020). Relationship between working memory and complex syntax in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Child Language, 47, 600632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, F., Wulfeck, B., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M., & Bates, L. (2004). The development of complex sentence interpretation in typically developing children compared with children with specific language impairment or early unilateral focal lesions. Developmental Science, 7, 360377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12491260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel de Abreu, P., Conway, A., & Gathercole, S. (2010). Working memory and fluid intelligence in young children. Intelligence, 38, 552561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, P., Nigg, J., & Ferreira, F. (2017). Executive function and intelligence in the resolution of temporary syntactic ambiguity: An individual differences investigation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 12631281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W. (2018). Working memory and executive attention: A revisit. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 190193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, R., Tuholski, S., Laughlin, J., & Conway, A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 128, 309331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J., Saffran, J., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 321335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, V., & Bock, K. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 10111029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferretti, R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 516547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finney, M., Montgomery, J., Gillam, R., & Evans, J. (2014). Role of working memory storage and attention focus switching in children’s comprehension of spoken object relative sentences. Child Development Research, 54, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2007). Is the movement deficit in syntactic SLI related to traces or to thematic role transfer? Brain and Language, 101, 5063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukuda, K., Vogel, E., Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2010). Quantity, not quality: The relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 673679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Madruga, J. A., Vila, J. O., Gómez-Veiga, I., Duque, G., & Elosúa, M. R. (2014). Executive processes, reading comprehension and academic achievement in 3rd grade primary students. Learning and Individual Differences, 35, 4148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Coco, M., Branigan, H. (2018). Impaired implicit learning of syntactic structure in children with developmental language disorder: Evidence from syntactic priming. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R., Montgomery, J., Evans, J., & Gillam, S. (2019). Cognitive predictors of sentence comprehension in children with and without developmental language disorder: Implications for assessment and treatment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21, 240251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillam, R. & Pearson, N. (2004). Test of narrative language. Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Gómez, R., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunow, H., Spaulding, T., Gomez, R., & Plante, E. (2006). The effects of variation on learning word order rules by adults with and without language-based learning disabilities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 158170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamrick, P., Lum, J., & Ullman, M. (2018). Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 7, 14871492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardman, K., & Cowan, N. (2015). Reasoning and memory: People make varied use of the information available in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 700722Google ScholarPubMed
Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E., Veríssimo, J., Dye, C., et al. (2011). Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 23622375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, J., & Bishop, D. (2010). Grammatical difficulties in children with specific language impairment: Is learning deficient? Human Development, 53, 264277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M., & McClelland, J. (2015). Connectionist perspectives on language learning, representation and processing. WIREs Cognitive Science, 6, 235247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, M., Hambrick, D., Tuholski, S., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T., & Engle, R. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48, 171184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, E., & Arciuli, J. (2016). Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87(1), 184193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leclercq, A., Majerus, S., Prigent, G., & Maillart, C. (2013). The impact of dual tasking on sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56, 265280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. (2014). Children with specific language impairment (2nd ed.). MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L., Deevy, P., Fey, M., & Bredin-Oja, S. (2013). Sentence comprehension in specific language impairment: A task designed to distinguish between cognitive capacity and syntactic complexity. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56, 577589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L., Eyer, J., Bedore, L., & Grela, B. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E., Salomo, D., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Two-year-old children’s production of multiword utterances: A usage-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 481507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loaiza, V., & Camos, V. (2018). The role of semantic representations in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 863881.Google ScholarPubMed
Lum, J., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Long-term memory: A review and meta-analysis of studies of declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 282297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A., & Ullman, M. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J., Youssef, G., & Clark, G. (2017). Using pupillometry to investigate sentence comprehension in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 60, 16481660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, M., & Christiansen, M. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 3554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, C., Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. (2007). Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and Grammatical-SLI children. Lingua, 117, 302320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, C., & van der Lely, H. (2006). A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics. Cognition, 100, 302320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 576585.Google Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Eichorn, N., Scheuer, J., & Yoon, J. (2014). Information processing and proactive interference in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 106119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2013). Toward a unified account of comprehension and production in language development. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 3839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2014). Acquiring formulaic language: A computational model. The Mental Lexicon, 9, 419436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2015). Individual differences in chunking ability predict on-line sentence processing. Proceedings from The 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1553–1558.Google Scholar
McCauley, S., & Christiansen, M. (2017). Computational investigations of multiword chunks in language learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 637652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCauley, S., Isbilen, E., & Christiansen, M. (2017). Chunking ability shapes sentence processing at multiple levels of abstraction. In Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 26812686). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. (2000). Relation of working memory to offline and real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 117148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. (2008). Role of auditory attention in the real-time processing of simple grammar by children with specific language impairment: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 499527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J., & Evans, J. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 269288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J., Evans, J., Fargo, J., Schwartz, S., & Gillam, R. (2018). Structural relationship between cognitive processing and syntactic sentence comprehension in children with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 29502976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J., Evans, J., & Gillam, R. (2009). Relation of auditory attention and complex sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: A preliminary study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 123151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J., Evans, J., Gillam, R., Sergeev, A., & Finney, M. (2016). “Whatdunit?” Developmental changes in children’s syntactically based sentence comprehension abilities and sensitivity to word order. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 12811309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J., Gillam, R., Evans, J., Fargo, J., & Schwartz, S. (2019). Comparison of the storage-only deficit and joint mechanism deficit hypotheses of the verbal working memory capacity limitation of children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62, 38083825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J., Gillam, R., Evans, J., & Sergeev, A. (2017). Whatdunit? Sentence comprehension abilities of children with SLI: Sensitivity to word order in canonical and noncanonical sentences. Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research, 60, 26032618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motallebzadeh, K., & Yazdi, M. (2016). The relationship between EFL learners’ reading comprehension ability and their fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and processing speed. Cogent Education, 3, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyle, M., Karasinski, C., Ellis Weismer, S., & Gorman, B. (2011). Grammatical morphology in school-age children with and without language impairment: A discriminant function analysis. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 550560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nee, D., & Jonides, J. (2013). Trisecting representational states in short-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nippold, M. (2017). Reading comprehension deficits in adolescents: Addressing underlying language abilities. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 125131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nippold, M., Hesketh, L., Duthie, J., & Mansfield, T. (2005). Conversational versus expository discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 10481064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M., Mansfield, T., Billow, J., & Tomblin, B. (2008). Expository discourse in adolescents with language impairments: Examining syntactic development. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 356366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nippold, M., Mansfield, T., Billow, J., & Tomblin, B. (2009). Syntactic development in adolescents with a history of language impairments: A follow-up investigation. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osaka, M., Osaka, N., Kondo, H., Morishita, M., Fukuyama, H., Aso, T., & Shibasaki, H. (2003). Neural basis of individual differences in working memory: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 18, 789797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öztekin, I., & Cowan, N. (2015). Editorial: Representational states in memory: Where do we stand? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öztekin, I., Davachi, L., & McElree, B. (2010). Are representations in working memory distinct from representations in long-term memory? Neural evidence in support of a single store. Psychological Science, 21, 11231133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, S., & Posner, M. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 7389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., Patterson, D., Sandoval, M., Vance, C., & Asbjørnsen, A. (2017). An fMRI study of implicit language learning in developmental language impairment. NeuroImage: Clinical, 14, 277285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 14121431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, E., & Joanisse, M. (2010). Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 141165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roid, G., & Miller, L. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised. Stoelting.Google Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6, 557567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T., & Engle, R. (2016). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence: Maintenance and disengagement. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 771779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review in Psychology, 44, 453495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The state of learning disabilities: Facts, trends and emerging issues (2014). National Center for Learning Disabilities (3rd Ed.).Google Scholar
Theakston, A., & Lieven, E. (2017). Multiunit sequences in first language acquisition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9, 588603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thiessen, E. (2017). What’s statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 372, 20160056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. (2016). The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiology model of language learning, knowledge, and use. In Hickok, G. & Small, S. (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp. 953968). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., & Pierpont, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H., & Stollwerck, L. (1997). Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victorino, K., & Schwartz, R. (2015). Control of auditory attention in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 12451257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 21432152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, J., Christiansen, M., Race, D., Acheson, D., & MacDonald, M. (2009). Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 250271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilhelm, O., Hildebrandt, A., & Oberauer, K. (2013). What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwitserlood, P. (1989). The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition, 32, 2564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 38(2), 403414. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3802.403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., & Willis, C. (2005). Working memory abilities in children with special educational needs. Education and Child Psychology, 22.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and other cognitive skills as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almomani, F., Al-Momani, M. O., Garadat, S., Alqudah, S., Kassab, M., Hamadneh, S., Rauterkus, G., & Gans, R. (2021). Cognitive functioning in deaf children using cochlear implants. BMC Pediatrics, 21(1), 71. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02534-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
AuBuchon, A. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2015). Verbal processing speed and executive functioning in long-term cochlear implant users. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(1), 151162. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-13-0259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105,158173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 742775). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., Newman, A. J., Mukherjee, M., Hauser, P., Kemeny, S., Braun, A., & Boutla, M. (2008). Encoding, rehearsal, and recall in signers and speakers: Shared network but differential engagement. Cerebral Cortex, 18(10), 22632274. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebko, J., & Metcalfe-Haggert, A. (1997). Deafness, language skills, and rehearsal: A model for the development of a memory strategy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(3), 131139. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.deafed.a014319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brentari, D. 2019. Sign language phonology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan-Worster, E; MacSweeney, M; Pimperton, H; Kyle, F; Harris, M; Beedie, I; Ralph-Lewis, A., & Hulme, C. (2020) Speechreading ability is related to phonological awareness and single-word reading in both deaf and hearing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 37753785CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R., & Dodd, B. (1980). Hearing by eye. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 8599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., & Woll, B. (2014). Cochlear implantation (CI) for prelingual deafness: The relevance of studies of brain organization and the role of first language acquisition in considering outcome success. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kirk, K. I. (2000). Working memory spans as predictors of spoken word recognition and receptive vocabulary in children with cochlear implants. The Volta Review, 102, 259280.Google ScholarPubMed
Conrad, R. (1970). Short-term memory processes in the deaf. British Journal of Psychology, 61(2), 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 5157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., Li, Y., Glass, B., & Saults, J. S. (2017). Development of the ability to combine visual and acoustic information in working memory. Developmental Science, 21, e12635, 114.Google ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., Rouder, J. N., Blume, C. L., & Saults, J. S. (2012). Models of verbal working memory capacity: What does it take to make them work? Psychological Review, 119, 480499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Souza, D., D’Souza, H., Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Precursors to language development in typically and atypically developing infants and toddlers: The importance of embracing complexity. Journal of Child Language, 44, 591627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, K., Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(2), 238250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desjardin, J. (2006). Family empowerment: Supporting language development in young children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review, 106, 275298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dillon, C. M., Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., & Carter, A. K. (2004). Imitation of nonwords by hearing-impaired children with cochlear implants: Segmental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18(1), 3955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B., Hobson, P., Brasher, J., & Campbell, R. (1983). Deaf children’s short-term memory for lip-read, graphic and signed stimuli. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(4), 353364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Developmental Review, 38, 241268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 65, 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmorey, K., Giezen, M. R., Petrich, J. A. F., Spurgeon, E., & O’Grady Farnady, L. (2017). The relation between working memory and language comprehension in signers and speakers. Acta Psychologica, 177, 6977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D., Horn, D., & Dillon, C. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of vocabulary, reading, and working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 461471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzpatrick, E., Hamel, C., Stevens, A., Pratt, M., Moher, D., Doucet, S., … Na, E. (2016). Sign language and spoken language for children with hearing loss: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 137, e20151974CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 410418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Hitch, G. J. (1993). Developmental changes in short-term memory: A revised working memory perspective. In Collins, A. F., Gathercole, S. E., Conway, M. A., & Morris, P. E. (Eds.), Theories of memory (p. 189209). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A. S. C., & the ALSPAC Team (2005). Developmental consequences of phonological loop deficits during early childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geers, A. E., & Sedey, A. L. (2011). Language and verbal reasoning skills in adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear and Hearing, 32(1 Suppl), 39S48S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G., Tobey, E., & Davidson, L. (2016). Persistent language delay versus late language emergence in children with early cochlear implantation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 59, 155170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottardis, L., Nunes, T., & Lunt, I. (2011). A synthesis of research on deaf and hearing children’s mathematical achievement. Deafness & Education International, 13, 131150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, M. L., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Working memory, deafness, and sign language. In Marshark, M. & Spencer, P. E. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (Vol. 2, pp. 458472). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, M. L., Hall, W. C., & Caselli, N. K. (2019). Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language, 39(4), 367395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, V. L. (1986). Access to spoken language and the acquisition of orthographic structure: Evidence from deaf readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 193212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlor, A. D. B., & Bower, C. (2009). Hearing assessment in infants and children: Recommendations beyond neonatal screening. Pediatrics, 124, 12521263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological coding: evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education, 9(3), 253268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M. S., Kronenberger, W. G., Gao, S., Hoen, H. M., Miyamoto, R. T., & Pisoni, D. B. (2013). Verbal short-term memory development and spoken language outcomes in deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 34(2), 179192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, L. A, Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 3745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, A., Atkinson, J., Marshall, C. R., Botting, N., St Clair, M., & Morgan, G. (2020). Expressive vocabulary predicts non-verbal executive function: A 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Development, 91,e400–14.63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapa, L. L., & Erikson, J. A. (2019). Variability of executive function performance in pre-schoolers with developmental language disorder. Seminars in Speech and Language, 40(4), 243255.Google Scholar
Keehner, M., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Working memory and deafness: Implications for cognitive development and functioning. In Pickering, Susan J. (Ed.), Working memory and education. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Koo, D., Crain, K., LaSasso, C., & Eden, G. (2008). Phonological awareness and short-term memory in hearing and deaf individuals of different communication backgrounds. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, pp. 8399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronenberger, W. G., Pisoni, D. B., Henning, S. C., & Colson, B. G. (2013). Executive functioning skills in long-term users of cochlear implants: A case control study. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38, 902914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lauro, L. J. R., Crespi, M., Papagno, C., & Cecchetto, C. (2014). Making sense of an unexpected detrimental effect of sign language use in a visual task. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19 (3), 358365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lina-Granade, G., Comte-Gervais, I., Gippon, L., Nappez, G., Morin, E., & Truy, E. (2010). Correlation between cognitive abilities and language level in cochlear implanted children. Cochlear Implants International, 11(Suppl. 1), 328331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Crespo, G., Daza, M. T., & Méndez-López, M. (2012). Visual working memory in deaf children with diverse communication modes: Improvement by differential outcomes. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 362368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, J., Jones, A., & Morgan, G. (2016). The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. Journal of Child Language, 43, 537552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacSweeney, M., Campbell, R., & Donlan, C. (1996). Varieties of short-term memory coding in deaf teenagers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(4), 249262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marschark, M., Rhoten, C., & Fabich, M. (2007). Effects of cochlear implants on children’s reading and academic achievement. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 269282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C., Jones, A., Denmark, T., Mason, K., Atkinson, J., Botting, N., & Morgan, G. (2015). Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, P. (2001). Communication mode and the information processing capacity of Hebrew readers with prelingually acquired deafness. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 13(2001), 8396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE). (2019). Cochlear implants for children and adults with severe to profound deafness. NICE technology appraisal guidance 566. [online]. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ta566Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Sansom, E., Low, K., Rice, C., & Caldwell-Tarr, A. (2014). Language structures used by kindergartners with cochlear implants: Relationship to phonological awareness, lexical knowledge and hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 35(5), 506518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell, A., Lowenstein, J.H., Tarr, E., & Holloman, C. (2012). Emergent literacy in kindergartners with cochlear implants, Ear Hear, 33(6) 683697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell-Tarr, A., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2013). Working memory in children with cochlear implants: Problems are in storage, not processing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 77(11), 18861898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nouwens, S., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). How working memory relates to children’s reading comprehension: The importance of domain-specificity in storage and processing. Reading and Writing, 30(1), 105120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1972). Seeing and hearing and time and space. Perception and Psychophysics, 11, 4648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, L., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 12651300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., & Cleary, M. (2003). Measures of working memory span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24(1 Suppl), 106S120S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pisoni, D. B., Kronenberger, W. G., Roman, A. S., & Geers, A. E. ( 2011). Measures of digit span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after more than 10 years of cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 32, 60S74S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, D. (2005). Models of deafness: Cochlear implants in the Australian daily press. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10, 451459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soleymani, Z., Amidfar, M., Dadgar, H., & Jalaei, S. (2014). Working memory in Farsi-speaking children with normal development and cochlear implant. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, D. J., McGregor, K. K., & Bentler, R. A. (2012). Vocabulary and working memory in children fit with hearing aids. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 154167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torkildsen, J. V., Arciuli, J., Haukedal, C. L., & Wie, O. B. (2018). Does a lack of auditory experience affect sequential learning? Cognition, 170, 123129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vugs, B., Knoors, H., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Executive function training in children with SLI: A pilot study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 33(1), 4766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Studies in communication, thought and language. In Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, G. (Eds.), MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). A visuospatial “phonological loop” in working memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition, 25(3), 313320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wingfield, A., Tun, P. A., & McCoy, S. L. (2005). Hearing loss in older adulthood: What it is and how it interacts with cognitive performance. Current Directions in Psycholological Science, 14, 144148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 44(1), 6883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Alloway, T. P. (2010). Improving working memory: Supporting students’ learning. Sage.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P. (2012). Can interactive working memory training improve learning? Journal of Interactive Learning Research, 23(3), 197207.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 2029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Alloway, R. G., & Wootan, S. (2014). Home sweet home: Does where you live matter to working memory and other cognitive skills? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 124, 124131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2013). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Computers & Human Behavior, 29(3), 632638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloway, T. P., & Copello, E. (2013). Working memory: The what, the why, and the how. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 30(2), 105118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and other cognitive skills as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Kirkwood, H. (2008). Working memory rating scale. Pearson.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80(2), 606621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., & Gregory, D. (2013). The predictive ability of IQ and working memory scores in literacy in an adult population. International Journal of Educational Research, 57, 5156 (2013).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloway, T. P., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2011). The relationship between working memory, IQ, and mathematical skills in children. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 133137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aksayli, N. D., Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2019). The cognitive and academic benefits of Cogmed: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 27, 229243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P. J., Lee, K. J., Roberts, G., Spencer-Smith, M. M., Thompson, D. K., Seal, M. L., … & Pascoe, L. (2018). Long-term academic functioning following Cogmed working memory training for children born extremely preterm: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Pediatrics, 202, 9297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136R140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigorra, A., Garolera, M., Guijarro, S., & Hervás, A. (2016). Long-term far-transfer effects of working memory training in children with ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(8), 853867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Ye, M., Chang, L., Chen, W., & Zhou, R. (2018). Effect of working memory updating training on retrieving symptoms of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 507519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colmar, S., Double, K., Davis, N., Sheldon, L., Phillips, N., Cheng, M., & Briddon, S. (2020). Memory mates: An evaluation of a classroom-based, student-focused working memory intervention. Journal of Psychologists and Counsellors in Schools, 30(2), 159171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornoldi, C., Carretti, B., Drusi, S., & Tencati, C. (2015) Improving problem solving in primary school students: The effect of a training programme focusing on metacognition and working memory. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 424439. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlin, E., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., & Neely, A. S. (2008). Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: Immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 720730. doi: 10.1037/a0014296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Amico, A., & Guarnera, M. (2005). Exploring working memory in children with low arithmetical achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 189202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Weerdt, F., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2013). Working memory in children with reading disabilities and/or mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 461472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dentz, A., Guay, M. C., Parent, V., & Romo, L. (2020). Working memory training for adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(6), 918927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietrichson, J., Bøg, M., Filges, T., & Klint Jørgensen, A. M. (2017). Academic interventions for elementary and middle school students with low socioeconomic status: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 87(2), 243282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzpatrick, C., Archambault, I., Janosz, M., & Pagani, L. S. (2015). Early childhood working memory forecasts high school dropout risk. Intelligence, 53, 160165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friso-Van den Bos, I., Van der Ven, S. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 10, 2944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers. Sage.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265−281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, C. T., Long, D. L., Green, D., Iosif, A. M., Dixon, J. F., Miller, M. R., et al. (2012). Will working memory training generalize to improve off-task behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Neurotherapeutics, 9, 639648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giofrè, D., Borella, E., & Mammarella, I. C. (2017). The relationship between intelligence, working memory, academic self-esteem, and academic achievement. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(6), 731747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannon, B., & McNaughton‐Cassill, M. (2011). SAT performance: Understanding the contributions of cognitive/learning and social/personality factors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 528535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, J. L., Nelson, R. A., Thomason, M. E., Sternberg, D. A., Katovich, K., Farzin, F., & Scanlon, M. (2015). Enhancing cognitive abilities with comprehensive training: A large, online, randomized, active-controlled trial. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0134467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawes, Z., & Ansari, D. (2020). What explains the relationship between spatial and mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2014). Taking working memory from the laboratory into schools. Educational Psychologist 34(4), 440450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9F15 (2009).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerns, K. A., Macoun, S., MacSween, J., Pei, J., & Hutchison, M. (2017). Attention and working memory training: A feasibility study in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6(2), 120137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirk, H. E., Gray, K., Riby, D., & Cornish, K. M. (2015). Cognitive training as a resolution for early executive function difficulties in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 38, 145160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johneson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K., Lee, M. P., Ang, S. Y., & Stankov, L. (2009). Do measures of working memory predict academic proficiency better than measures of intelligence? Psychology Science.Google Scholar
Lin, X., Peng, P., & Luo, H. (2021). The deficit profile of elementary students with computational difficulties versus word problem-solving difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 44(2), 110122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linares, R., Borella, E., Lechuga, M. T., Carretti, B., & Pelegrina, S. (2019). Nearest transfer effects of working memory training: A comparison of two programs focused on working memory updating. PLoS ONE, 14(2), e0211321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2012 ). Working-memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 18(1), 6278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezzacappa, E., & Buckner, J. C. (2010). Working memory training for children with attention problems or hyperactivity : A school-based pilot study. School Mental Health, 2(4), 202208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T., & Duncan, G. J. (2019). Kindergarten components of executive function and third grade achievement: A national study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 4961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phakey, N., Sharma, S., Garg, D., Mukherjee, S. B., Sapra, S., Wadhawan, A. N., & Shukla, G. (2021). Development and evaluation of a working memory intervention kit in children with epilepsy in low-resource settings. The Indian Journal of Pediatrics, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redick, T. S., & Lindsey, D. R. (2013). Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 11021113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., Souza, A. S., Von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2019). Is executive control related to working memory capacity and fluid intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(8), 1335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rode, C., Robson, R., Purviance, A., Geary, D., & Mayr, U. (2014). Is working memory training effective? A study in a school setting. PLoS ONE, 9(8), 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G., Quach, J., Spencer-Smith, M., Anderson, P. J., Gathercole, S., Gold, L., … & Wake, M. (2016). Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 170(5), e154568e154568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(2), 180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Working memory training in typically developing children: A meta-analysis of the available evidence. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Buhner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siquara, G. M., dos Santos Lima, C., & Abreu, N. (2018). Working memory and intelligence quotient: Which best predicts on school achievement? Psico, 49(4), 365374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Söderqvist, S., & Bergman Nutley, S. (2015). Working memory training is associated with long term attainments in math and reading. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., & McMurran, M. (2018). The impact of working memory training on near and far transfer measures: Is it all about fluid intelligence? Child Neuropsychology, 24(3), 370395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., & Saez, L. (2003). Memory difficulties in children and adults with learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 182198). Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Thevenot, C., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The strategic use of alternative representations in arithmetic word problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 13111323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titz, C., & Karbach, J. (2014). Working memory and executive functions: Effects of training on academic achievement. Psychological Research, 78(6), 852868 (2014).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Träff, U., Olsson, L., Östergren, R., & Skagerlund, K. (2020). Development of early domain-specific and domain-general cognitive precursors of high and low math achievers in grade 6. Child Neuropsychology, 26(8), 10651090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Molen, M., Van Luit, J., Van der Molen, M., Klugkist, I., & Jongmans, M. (2010). Effectiveness of a computerised working memory training in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(5), 433447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y. (2020). Does Cogmed working memory training improve school-age ADHD children’s academic achievement? Cambridge Educational Research e-Journal 7, 141167.Google Scholar

References

Allen, R. J., Hitch, G. J., Mate, J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Feature binding and attention in working memory: A resolution of previous contradictory findings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(12), 23692383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, R. J., & Waterman, A. H. (2015). How does enactment affect the ability to follow instructions in working memory? Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 555561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astle, D. E., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2020). Beyond the core-deficit hypothesis in developmental disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(5), 431437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, J., Katz, B., Buschkuehl, M., Bunarjo, K., Senger, T., Zabel, C., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2016). Enhancing working memory training with transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(9), 14191432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136R140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4789). Elsevier.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Scott, D. (1971). Short term forgetting in the absence of proactive interference. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23(3), 275283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berryhill, M. E., Peterson, D. J., Jones, K. T., & Stephens, J. A. (2014). Hits and misses: Leveraging tDCS to advance cognitive research. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M. E., & Kelly, S. W. (2010). Do tasks make a difference? Accounting for heterogeneity of performance of children with reading difficulties on tasks of executive function: Findings from a meta‐analysis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 133176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brener, R. (1940). An experimental investigation of memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26(5), 467482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). Children’s arithmetical difficulties: Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65(1), 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, E. M., Ewbank, M. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Holmes, J. (2020). The effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on within- and cross-paradigm transfer following multi-session backward recall training. Brain and Cognition, 141, 105552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, E. M., Nord, C. L., & Holmes, J. (2021). Cognitive plasticity and transcranial electrical stimulation. In Cognitive Training (pp. 85105). Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camos, V., Lagner, P., & Barrouillet, P. (2009). Two maintenance mechanisms of verbal information in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(11), 457469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, J., & Engle, R. W. (1993). Working-memory capacity as long-term memory activation: An individual-differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 11011114.Google ScholarPubMed
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 246251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Friedmann, N. (2014). Developmental dyslexia and the phonological deficit hypothesis. Mind and Language, 29(3), 270285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 11421157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, P,. & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive Load Theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 293332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, L. A., Allen, R. J., Morson, S., Burn, W. K., & Souchay, C. (2014). Working memory and the enactment effect in early Alzheimer’s disease. ISRN Neurology, eCollection 2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Child, A. E., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Willcutt, E. G., & Fuchs, L. S. (2019). A cognitive dimensional approach to understanding shared and unique contributions to reading, math, and attention skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 52(1), 1530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirino, P. T., Child, A. E., & Macdonald, K. T. (2018). Longitudinal predictors of the overlap between reading and math skills. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 54, 99111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coffman, B. A., Clark, V. P., & Parasuraman, R. (2014). Battery powered thought: Enhancement of attention, learning, and memory in healthy adults using transcranial direct current stimulation. NeuroImage, 85, 895908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen Kadosh, R., Levy, N., O’Shea, J., Shea, N., & Savulescu, J. (2012). The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation. Current Biology, 22(4), R108–R111.Google ScholarPubMed
Cornoldi, C., Carretti, B., Drusi, S., & Tencati, C. (2015). Improving problem solving in primary school students: The effect of a training programme focusing on metacognition and working memory. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 424439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., Dittmann, R. W., Holtmann, M., Santosh, P., Stevenson, J., Stringaris, A., Zuddas, A., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(3), 164174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costanzo, F., Rossi, S., Varuzza, C., Varvara, P., Vicari, S., & Menghini, D. (2019). Long-lasting improvement following tDCS treatment combined with a training for reading in children and adolescents with dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 130, 3843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costanzo, F., Varuzza, C., Rossi, S., Sdoia, S., Varvara, P., Oliveri, M., Giacomo, K., Vicari, S., & Menghini, D. (2016). Evidence for reading improvement following tDCS treatment in children and adolescents with dyslexia. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 34(2), 215226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2017). The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 11581170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., & Nugent, L. D. (1997). The role of absolute and relative amounts of time in forgetting within immediate memory: The case of tone-pitch comparisons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(3), 393397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., Bäckman, L., & Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science, 320(5882), 15101512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, N. J., Oberman, L. M., & Earp, B. D. (2014). Transcranial stimulation of the developing brain: A plea for extreme caution. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Smedt, B., Swillen, A., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). Mathematical learning disabilities in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: A review. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 15(1), 410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(10), 475483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D. L., & Holmes, J. (2014). Does working memory training promote the use of strategies on untrained working memory tasks? Memory & Cognition, 42(6), 854862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Science, 16, 915925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwan, K., Gamble, C., Williamson, P. R., & Kirkham, J. J. (2013). Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias: An updated review. PLoS ONE, 8, e66844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, J. G., Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Holmes, J., & Kirkwood, H. (2010). An evaluation of a classroom-based intervention to help overcome working memory difficulties and improve long-term academic achievement. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 9, 227251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elmasry, J., Loo, C., & Martin, D. M. (2015). A systematic review of transcranial electrical stimulation combined with cognitive training. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 33(3), 263278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W., Carullo, J. J., & Collins, K. W. (1991). Individual differences in working memory for comprehension and following directions. Journal of Educational Research, 84(5), 253262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Follmer, D. J. (2018). Executive function and reading comprehension: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 53(1), 4260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers. Sage.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Brown, L., & Pickering, S. J. (2003). Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educational and Child Psychology, 20(3), 109122.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Norris, D. (2019). Working memory training involves learning new skills. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 1942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Durling, E., Evans, M., Jeffcock, S., & Stone, S. (2008). Working memory abilities and children’s performance in laboratory analogues of classroom activities. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(8), 10191037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Holmes, J. (2014). Developmental impairments of working memory: Profiles and interventions. Perspectives on Language and Literacy, 40, 3639.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory in the classroom. In Pickering, S. (Ed.), Working memory and education. Academic.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 345362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C. (2003). Learning disabilities in arithmetic: Problem-solving differences and cognitive deficits. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 199212). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2014). Testing for near and far transfer effects with a short, face-to-face adaptive working memory training intervention in typical children. Infant and Child Development, 23(1), 84103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, A. T., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Hoy, K. E. (2016). Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimulation, 9(2), 197208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitch, G. J., & McAuley, E. (1991). Working memory in children with specific arithmetical learning difficulties. British Journal of Psychology, 82(3), 375386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, J., Adams, J. W., & Hamilton, C. J. (2008). The relationship between visuospatial sketchpad capacity and children’s mathematical skills. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 272289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, J., Byrne, E. M., Gathercole, S. E., & Ewbank, M. P. (2016). Transcranial random noise stimulation does not enhance the effects of working memory training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(10), 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, J., Guy, J., Kievit, R. A., Bryant, A., Mareva, S., Gathercole, S. E., & CALM Team. (2020). Cognitive dimensions of learning in children with problems in attention, learning, and memory. Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Holmes, J., Woolgar, F., Hampshire, A., & Gathercole, S. E. (2019). Are working memory training effects paradigm-specific? Frontiers in Psychology, 10(May), 1103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 68296833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y.-F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning: Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaroslawska, A. J., Gathercole, S. E., Allen, R. J., & Holmes, J. (2016). Following instructions from working memory: Why does action at encoding and recall help? Memory & Cognition, 44(8), 11831191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaroslawska, A. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Holmes, J. (2018). Following instructions in a dual-task paradigm: Evidence for a temporary motor store in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(11), 24392449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaroslawska, A. J., Gathercole, S. E., Logie, M. R., & Holmes, J. (2016). Following instructions in a virtual school: Does working memory play a role? Memory & Cognition, 44(4), 580589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: Its role in dyslexia and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia, 10(3), 196214. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, C. H., & White, M. A. (1980). Children’s direction-following behavior in grades K-S. The Journal of Educational Research, 74(1), 4348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 20272037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R. A., Hofman, A. D., & Nation, K. (2019). Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning in young children: A replication and extension of the study by Kievit et al. (2017). Psychological Science, 30(8), 12451252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., Gillberg, C. G., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A., Ben-Zur, H., & Nussbaum, A. (1990). Encoding information for future action: Memory for to-be-performed tasks versus memory for to-be-recalled tasks. Memory & Cognition, 18(6), 568578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudo, M. F., Lussier, C. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2015). Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, M.-F., & Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 43(3), 192199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Küper, K., & Karbach, J. (2016). Increased training complexity reduces the effectiveness of brief working memory training: Evidence from short-term single and dual n-back training interventions. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 199208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landerl, K., & Kölle, C. (2009). Typical and atypical development of basic numerical skills in elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 546565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S.-C., Schmiedek, F., Huxhold, O., Röcke, C., Smith, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Working memory plasticity in old age: Practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 731742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., & Geary, D. C. (2013). Developmental gains in visuospatial memory predict gains in mathematics achievement. PLoS ONE, 8(7), e70160–e70160.Google ScholarPubMed
Looi, C. Y., Lim, J., Sella, F., Lolliot, S., Duta, M., Avramenko, A. A., & Cohen Kadosh, R. (2017). Transcranial random noise stimulation and cognitive training to improve learning and cognition of the atypically developing brain: A pilot study. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 4633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2012). Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 18(1), 6278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancuso, L. E., Ilieva, I. P., Hamilton, R. H., & Farah, M. J. (2016). Does transcranial direct current stimulation improve healthy working memory? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(8), 10631089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mareva, S., The CALM Team, & Holmes, J. (2021). Cognitive and academic skills in two developmental cohorts of different ability level: A mutualistic network perspective, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, D. M., Liu, R., Alonzo, A., Green, M., Player, M. J., Sachdev, P., & Loo, C. K. (2013). Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance outcomes from cognitive training? A randomized controlled trial in healthy participants. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 19271936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(3), 240260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lervåg, A., Lyster, S.-A. H., Klem, M., Hagtvet, B., & Hulme, C. (2012). Nonword-repetition ability does not appear to be a causal influence on children’s vocabulary development. Psychological Science, 23(10), 10921098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, M. L., Salimpoor, V. N., Wu, S. S., Geary, D. C., & Menon, V. (2010). Differential contribution of specific working memory components to mathematics achievement in 2nd and 3rd graders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 101109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minear, M., Brasher, F., Guerrero, C. B., Brasher, M., Moore, A., & Sukeena, J. (2016). A simultaneous examination of two forms of working memory training: Evidence for near transfer only. Memory & Cognition, 44, 10101037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, A. B., & Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 4660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 338355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsson, J., Lebedev, A. V., Rydström, A., & Lövdén, M. (2017). Direct-current stimulation does little to improve the outcome of working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 28(7), 907920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Physiology, 527(3), 633639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osterberg, L., & Blaschke, T. (2005). Adherence to medication. New England Journal of Medicine, 353, 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paas, F. G. W. C., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F. G. W. C., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive Load Theory: Instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture, Instructional Science, 32, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: Using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 2745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passolunghi, M. C., & Costa, H. M. (2016). Working memory and early numeracy training in preschool children. Child Neuropsychology, 22(1), 8198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passolunghi, M. C., Mammarella, I. C., & Altoè, G. (2008). Cognitive abilities as precursors of the early acquisition of mathematical skills during first through second grades. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 229250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(1), 4457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pause, B. M., Zlomuzica, A., Kinugawa, K., Mariani, J., Pietrowsky, R., & Dere, E. (2013). Perspectives on episodic-like and episodic memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peijnenborgh, J. C. A., Hurks, P. M., Aldenkamp, A. P., Vles, J. S. H., & Hendriksen, J. G. M. (2016). Efficacy of working memory training in children and adolescents with learning disabilities: A review study and meta-analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5–6), 645672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., & Fuchs, D. (2015). A randomized control trial of working memory training with and without strategy instruction: Effects on young children’s working memory and comprehension. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(1), 6280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, P., & Fuchs, D. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: Is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, P., & Kievit, R. A. (2020). The development of academic achievement and cognitive abilities: A bidirectional perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 1520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 455473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, P., Wang, C., & Namkung, J. (2018). Understanding the cognition related to mathematics difficulties: A meta-analysis on the cognitive deficit profiles and the bottleneck theory. Review of Educational Research, 88(3), 434476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Suppressing irrelevant information from working memory: Evidence for domain-specific deficits in poor comprehenders. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(4), 380391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preßler, A.-L., Könen, T., Hasselhorn, M., & Krajewski, K. (2014). Cognitive preconditions of early reading and spelling: A latent-variable approach with longitudinal data. Reading and Writing, 27(2), 383406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., & Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8),