Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:41:40.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

23 - Antecedents and Consequences of Evaluative Concerns Experienced During Intergroup Interaction: When and How Does Group Status Matter?

from Part III - Prejudice Reduction and Analysis in Applied Contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2016

Jacquie D. Vorauer
Affiliation:
University of Manitoba
Matthew Quesnel
Affiliation:
University of Manitoba
Chris G. Sibley
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Fiona Kate Barlow
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Get access

Summary

Individuals often evidence substantial egocentrism by virtue of being preoccupied with themselves and how they appear to others during social interaction. For example, people overestimate the extent to which others’ behavior is caused by and directed toward them personally (Zuckerman et al., 1983), perceive that others pay more attention to them than is actually the case (Gilovich, Medvec, & Savitsky, 2000), and even exaggerate others’ focus on their failures and mishaps (Savitsky, Epley, & Gilovich, 2001). Such egocentric biases may sometimes seem counterintuitive, as when they lead individuals to reach overly pessimistic conclusions about how they are viewed. However, the biases are readily understandable through the lens of contemporary theories emphasizing how individuals’ fundamental motivation to maintain a sense of belonging and acceptance leads them to continuously monitor their social standing with others (e.g., Leary & Downs, 1995).

Critical from an intergroup relations perspective is that individuals do not leave this motivation for social acceptance – or the egocentric biases that it fosters – behind when they enter intergroup interaction situations. Although myriad forces are of course operative in these settings, individuals’ basic human desire to understand and manage their social standing with others remains very much in play.

What does this mean for intergroup relations? The information search model (Vorauer, 2006) drew on existing research and theory to make a range of predictions regarding antecedents and consequences of evaluative concerns during intergroup interaction. A key premise of the model was that these concerns could have a variety of negative implications for intergroup relations. Although at the time clear evidence supported of some of the model's propositions, other propositions were largely speculative. In the decade or so since then, research has been conducted that speaks to many of these originally untested, or sparsely tested, ideas, and more data from members of minority or lower-status groups are available now than was initially the case.

Accordingly, we review the new research and assess the extent to which the findings support versus contradict key propositions of the model. Because of space limitations, we focus on group memberships defined by ethnic background and broad themes, especially with respect to consequences. We place particular emphasis on the evidence for similarities versus differences across members of higher- and lower-status groups. We conclude by considering implications for intervention and identifying what we see as important next steps for research in this area.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amodio, D. M. (2009). Intergroup anxiety effects on the control of racial stereotypes: A psychoneuroendocrine analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 60–67.Google Scholar
Avery, D. R., Richeson, J. A., Hebl, M. R., & Ambady, N. (2009). It does not have to be uncomfortable: The role of behavioral scripts in Black–White interracial interactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1382.Google Scholar
Barlow, F. K., Hornsey, M. J., Thai, M., Sengupta, N. K., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). The wallpaper effect: The contact hypothesis fails for minority group members who live in areas with a high proportion of majority group members. PloS One, 8(12), e82228.Google Scholar
Barlow, F. K., Louis, W. R., & Terry, D. J. (2010). Minority report: Social identity, cognitions of rejection and intergroup anxiety predicting prejudice from one racially marginalized group towards another. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(5), 805–818. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/754051612?accountid=14569 Google Scholar
Barlow, F. K., Sibley, C. G., & Hornsey, M. J. (2012). Rejection as a call to arms: Inter-racial hostility and support for political action as outcomes of race-based rejection in majority and minority groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 167–177. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02040.x Google Scholar
Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected: Divergent goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 248–264. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018474 Google Scholar
Çelik, P., Lammers, J., van Beest, I., Bekker, M. H. J., & Vonk, R. (2013). Not all rejections are alike; competence and warmth as a fundamental distinction in social rejection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 635–642. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.010 Google Scholar
Croft, A., & Schmader, T. (2012). The feedback withholding bias: Minority students do not receive critical feedback from evaluators concerned about appearing racist. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(5), 1139–1144.Google Scholar
Crosby, J. R., & Monin, B. (2007). Failure to warn: How student race affects warnings of potential academic difficulty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(4), 663–670. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.06.007 Google Scholar
Davies, K., Tropp, L. R., Aron, A., Pettigrew, T. F., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Cross-group friendships and intergroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 332–351. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411103 Google Scholar
Dixon, J., Tropp, L. R., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2010). “Let them eat harmony”: Prejudice-reduction strategies and attitudes of historically disadvantaged groups. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(2), 76–80. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721410363366 Google Scholar
Finchilescu, G. (2010). Intergroup anxiety in interracial interaction: The role of prejudice and metastereotypes. Journal of Social Issues, 66, 334–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01648.x Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (2000). The spotlight effect in social judgment: an egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of one's own actions and appearance. Journal of personality and social psychology, 78(2), 211.Google Scholar
Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 91–107. doi: http://dx.doi.org.uml.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.91 Google Scholar
Gray, H. M., Mendes, W. B., & Denny-Brown, C. (2008). An in-group advantage in detecting intergroup anxiety. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1233–1237. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02230.x Google Scholar
Harber, K. D., Stafford, R., & Kennedy, K. A. (2010). The positive feedback bias as a response to self-image threat. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(1), 207–218. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466609X473956 Google Scholar
Holoien, D. S., Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Alegre, J. M. (2015). Do you really understand? achieving accuracy in interracial relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(1), 76–92. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000003 Google Scholar
Holoien, D. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Downplaying positive impressions: Compensation between warmth and competence in impression management. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 33–41. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.09.001 Google Scholar
Holoien, D. S., & Shelton, J. N. (2012). You deplete me: The cognitive costs of colorblindness on ethnic minorities. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 562–565. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.010 Google Scholar
Kamans, E., Gordijn, E. H., Oldenhuis, H., & Otten, S. (2009). What I think you see is what you get: Influence of prejudice on assimilation to negative meta-stereotypes among Dutch Moroccan teenagers. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(5), 842–851. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.593 Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Interpersonal functions of the self-esteem motive: The self-esteem system as a sociometer. In Leary, M. R. & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 123–144). New York: Plenum Press.
Logan, S., Steel, Z., & Hunt, C. (2015). Investigating the effect of anxiety, uncertainty and ethnocentrism on willingness to interact in an intercultural communication. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(1), 39–52. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022114555762 Google Scholar
MacInnis, C., & Hodson, G. (2012). “Where the rubber hits the road” en route to inter-group harmony: Examining contact intentions and contact behaviour under meta-stereotype threat. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(2), 363–373. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02014.x Google Scholar
Mendes, W. B., & Koslov, K. (2013). Brittle smiles: Positive biases toward stigmatized and outgroup targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 923–933. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029663 Google Scholar
Méndez, E., Gómez, Á., & Tropp, L. R. (2007). When metaperceptions are affected by intergroup processes. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 7(2), 237–250. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/621845460?accountid=14569 Google Scholar
Mendoza-Denton, R., & Page-Gould, E. (2008). Can cross-group friendships influence minority students’ well-being at historically white universities? Psychological Science, 19(9), 933–939. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02179.x Google Scholar
Migacheva, K., & Tropp, L. R. (2013). Learning orientation as a predictor of positive intergroup contact. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16(4), 426–444. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1449308269?accountid=14569 Google Scholar
Murphy, M. C., Richeson, J. A., & Molden, D. C. (2011). Leveraging motivational mindsets to foster positive interracial interactions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(2), 118–131. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00338.x Google Scholar
Neel, R., & Shapiro, J. R. (2012). Is racial bias malleable? Whites’ lay theories of racial bias predict divergent strategies for interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 101–120. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028237 Google Scholar
Page-Gould, E., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). With a little help from my cross-group friend: Reducing anxiety in intergroup contexts through cross-group friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1080–1094. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1080 Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751–783. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751 Google Scholar
Plant, E. A., Devine, P. G., & Peruche, M. B. (2010). Routes to positive interracial interactions: Approaching egalitarianism or avoiding prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(9), 1135–1147. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167210378018 Google Scholar
Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2005). Why do interracial interactions impair executive function? A resource depletion account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 934–947. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.934 Google Scholar
Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2008). The threat of appearing prejudiced and race-based attentional biases. Psychological Science, 19(2), 98–102. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02052.x Google Scholar
Saguy, T., Dovidio, J. F., & Pratto, F. (2008). Beyond contact: Intergroup contact in the context of power relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 432–445.Google Scholar
Sakamoto, Y. (2013). Does she hate me? or does she like me? Evaluative uncertainty during intergroup contact (Order No. AAINR78916). Available from PsycINFO. (1449320007; 2013-99200-277). Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1449320007?accountid=14569
Sasaki, S. J., & Vorauer, J. D. (2010). Contagious resource depletion and anxiety? Spreading effects of evaluative concern and impression formation in dyadic social interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1011–1016. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.003 Google Scholar
Savitsky, K., Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Do others judge us as harshly as we think? Overestimating the impact of our failures, shortcomings, and mishaps. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 44.Google Scholar
Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., Ullrich, J., Dovidio, J. F., & Carmi, D. (2009). Promoting reconciliation through the satisfaction of the emotional needs of victimized and perpetrating group members: The needs-based model of reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(8), 1021–1030.Google Scholar
Shnabel, N., Ullrich, J., Nadler, A., Dovidio, J. F., & Aydin, A. L. (2013). Warm or competent? Improving intergroup relations by addressing threatened identities of advantaged and disadvantaged groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(6), 482–492. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1519507478?accountid=14569 Google Scholar
Shelton, N., Douglass, S., Garcia, R. L., Yip, T., & Trail, T. E. (2014). Feeling (mis)understood and intergroup friendships in interracial interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(9), 1193–1204. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167214538459 Google Scholar
Shelton, J. N., West, T. V., & Trail, T. E. (2010). Concerns about appearing prejudiced: Implications for anxiety during daily interracial interactions. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(3), 329–344. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430209344869 Google Scholar
Siem, B., Von Oettingen, M., Mummendey, A., & Nadler, A. (2013). When status differences are illegitimate, groups’ needs diverge: Testing the needs-based model of reconciliation in contexts of status inequality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(2), 137–148. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1929 Google Scholar
Son, D., & Shelton, J. N. (2011). Stigma consciousness among Asian Americans: Impact of positive stereotypes in interracial roommate relationships. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 2(1), 51–60. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022651 Google Scholar
Stephan, W. G. (2014). Intergroup anxiety: Theory, research, and practice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 239–255. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868314530518 Google Scholar
Taylor, V. J., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Stereotype threat undermines academic learning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 1055–1067. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211406506 Google Scholar
Trawalter, S., Adam, E. K., Chase-Lansdale, P., & Richeson, J. A. (2012). Concerns about appearing prejudiced get under the skin: Stress responses to interracial contact in the moment and across time. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 682–693. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.003 Google Scholar
Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2008). Let's talk about race, baby! When Whites’ and Blacks’ interracial contact experiences diverge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1214–1217. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.013 Google Scholar
Tropp, L. R., & Pettigrew, T. F. (2005). Relationships between intergroup contact and prejudice among minority and majority status groups. Psychological Science, 16(12), 951–957. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01643.x Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D. (2006). An information search model of evaluative concerns in intergroup interaction. Psychological Review, 113(4), 862.Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. (2008). Unprejudiced and self-focused: When intergroup contact is experienced as being about the ingroup rather than the outgroup. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 912–919. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.002 Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D. (2013). Getting past the self: Understanding and removing evaluative concerns as an obstacle to positive intergroup contact effects. In Leary, M. R. & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.), Advances in intergroup contact (pp. 23–48). New York: Psychology Press.
Vorauer, J. D., Gagnon, A., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Salient intergroup ideology and intergroup interaction. Psychological Science, 20(7), 838–845. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02369.x Google Scholar
Vorauer, J., & Kumhyr, S. M. (2001). Is this about you or me? Self- versus other-directed judgments and feelings in response to intergroup interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(6), 706–719. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167201276006 Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., Martens, V., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). When trying to understand detracts from trying to behave: Effects of perspective taking in intergroup interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 811–827. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013411 Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., & Sakamoto, Y. (2008). Who cares what the outgroup thinks? Testing an information search model of the importance individuals accord to an outgroup member's view of them during intergroup interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1467.Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Helpful only in the abstract? Ironic effects of empathy in intergroup interaction. Psychological Science, 20(2), 191–197. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02265.x Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2010). In need of liberation or constraint? How intergroup attitudes moderate the behavioral implications of intergroup ideologies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 133–138. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.013 Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2011). In the worst rather than the best of times: Effects of salient intergroup ideology in threatening intergroup interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 307–320. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023152 Google Scholar
Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2014). Distinct effects of imagine-other versus imagine-self perspective taking on prejudice reduction. Social Cognition, 32(2), 130–147. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.2.130 Google Scholar
West, T. V., Magee, J. C., Gordon, S. H., & Gullett, L. (2014). A little similarity goes a long way: The effects of peripheral but self-revealing similarities on improving and sustaining interracial relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(1), 81–100. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036556 Google Scholar
West, T. V., Pearson, A. R., Dovidio, J. F., Shelton, J. N., & Trail, T. E. (2009). Superordinate identity and intergroup roommate friendship development. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(6), 1266–1272. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.002 Google Scholar
West, T. V., Shelton, J. N., & Trail, T. E. (2009). Relational anxiety in interracial interactions. Psychological Science, 20(3), 289–292. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02289.x Google Scholar
Woodcock, A., Hernandez, P. R., Estrada, M., & Schultz, P. W. (2012). The consequences of chronic stereotype threat: Domain disidentification and abandonment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(4), 635–646. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029120 Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Kernis, M. H., Guarnera, S. M., Murphy, J. F., & Rappoport, L. (1983). The egocentric bias: Seeing oneself as cause and target of others’ behavior. Journal of Personality, 51(4), 621–630. Retrieved from http://uml.idm.oclc.org/login?url=http://search.proquest.com.uml.idm.oclc.org/docview/616922194?accountid=14569 Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×