Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T06:57:03.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part II - Sociocultural Anthropology and Evolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Antweiler, C. (2016). Our Common Denominator: Human Universals Revisited (D. Kerns, Trans.). Oxford/New York: Berghahn Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1981). Evolution et sexualité humaine. In Crépault, C., Lévy, J., & Gratton, H., eds., Sexologie Contemporaine. Quebec: Les Presses de l’Université du Québec, pp. 103118.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1989). The elastic between genes and culture. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10(1–3), 111129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (2006). Sometimes the bus does wait. In Barkow, J. H., ed., Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 359.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human Universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M, & von Hippel, W. (2018). Psychological barriers to evolutionary psychology: Ideological bias and coalitional adaptations. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 6(1), 148158.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1965). The impact of the concept of culture on the concept of man. In Platt, J. R., ed., New Views of the Nature of Man. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 93118.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1983). Le Regard Éloigné. Paris: Plon.Google Scholar
Salazar, C. (2018). Explaining Human Diversity: Cultures, Minds, Evolution. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Symons, D. (1979). The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Amarna Letters (2001). Translated by Moran, W. L.. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Arnold, K., & Owens, I. (1998). Cooperative breeding in birds: A comparative test of the life history hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 739745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentley, G., & Mace, R. (2009). Substitute Parents: Biological and Social Perspectives on Alloparenting in Human Societies. Oxford: Berghahn.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (1986). Despotism and Differential Reproduction: A Darwinian View of History. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (1992). Roman polygyny. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 309349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2005). Politics as sex: The Old Testament case. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 326346.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2009). But what is government itself but the greatest of all reflections on human nature? Politics and the Life Sciences, 28, 102105.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2012). Means, variances and ranges in reproductive success: Comparative evidence. Human Behavior and Evolution, 33, 309317.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2013). Darwin’s question: How can sterility evolve? In Summers, K. & Crespi, B., eds., Human Social Evolution: The Foundational Works of Richard D. Alexander. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 365374.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2014). Eusociality in history. Human Nature, 25, 8099.Google Scholar
Betzig, L. L. (2016). Mating systems. In Shackelford, T. & Weekes-Shackelford, V., eds., Encycopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Berlin: Springer, pp. 111.Google Scholar
Bird, R. B., & Bird, D. (2002). Constraints on knowing or constraints on growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature, 13, 239267.Google Scholar
Book of the Dead (1967). Translated by W. Budge. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Carneiro, R. L. (1970). A theory of the origin of the state. Science, 169, 733738.Google Scholar
Choe, J., & Crespi, B. (1997). Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Cockburn, A. (2006). Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 273, 13751383.Google Scholar
Crespi, B. (2014). The insectan ape. Human Nature, 25, 627.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, M. B., & Boomsma, J. J. (2006). Are workers of Atta leafcutter ants capable of reproduction? Insectes Sociaux, 53, 136140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebrey, P. (2002). Women and the Family in Chinese History. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1997). Predicting family dynamics in social vertebrates. In Krebs, J. & Davies, N., eds., Behavioural Ecology, 4th ed. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific, pp. 228253.Google Scholar
Fisher, M. (2001). The sons of Ramesses II. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Foster, K., & Ratneiks, F. (2005). A new eusocial vertebrate? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20, 363364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gannon, S. (2011). Exclusion as language and the language of exclusion: Tracing regimes of gender through linguistic representations of the “eunuch.” Journal of the History of Sexuality, 20, 127.Google Scholar
Goldin, P. (2002). The Culture of Sex in Ancient China. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Grayson, A. K. (1995). Eunuchs in power. In Dietrich, M. & Oretz, O., eds., Festschrift für Wolfram Freiherrn. Neukirchen: Alter Orient und Altes Testament, pp. 8598.Google Scholar
Hammer, M. F., Mendez, F. L., Cox, M. P., Woerner, A. E., & Wall, J. D. (2008). Sex-biased evolutionary forces shape genomic patterns of human diversity. PLoS Genetics, 4, e1000202.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., & Coxworth, J. (2013). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity: A review of findings and future directions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 22, 294302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1989). Hardworking Hadza grandmothers. In Standen, V. & Foley, R., eds., Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals. London: Blackwell, pp. 341366.Google Scholar
Haydock, J., & Koenig, W. D. (2002). Reproductive skew in the polygyandrous acorn woodpecker. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 71787183.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. (2005). Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural Perspectives. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 38633870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hölldobler, B., & Wilson, E. O. (2009). The Superorganism. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Hölldobler, B., & Wilson, E. O. (2011). The Leafcutter Ants. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Hopkins, K. (1963). Eunuchs in politics in the later Roman Empire. Proceedings of the Cambridge Philological Society, 9, 6280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants and Natural Selection. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Imperial Administrative Records: Palace and Temple Administration (1992). Edited by Fales, F. M & Postgate, M.. Helsinki: University of Helsinki Press.Google Scholar
Kadish, G. (1967). Eunuchs in ancient Egypt? In Studies in Honor of John A. Wilson. Chicago, IL: Oriental Institute, pp. 5572.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. B., & Hurado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156185.Google Scholar
Karmin, M., Saaq, L., Vicente, M., et al. (2015). A recent bottleneck of Y chromosome diversity coincides with a global change in culture. Genome Research, 25, 459466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeley, L. (1988). Hunter–gatherer economic complexity and population pressure: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 7, 373411.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. (2013). The Foraging Spectrum, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koenig, W. D., & Mumme, R. (1987). Population Ecology of the Cooperatively Breeding Acorn Woodpecker. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Koenig, W. D., & Dickinson, J. (2004). Evolution of Cooperative Breeding in Birds. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koenig, W. D., & Dickinson, J. (2016). Cooperative Breeding in Vertebrates. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koenig, W. D., Dickinson, J., & Emlen, S. T. (2016). Synthesis: Cooperative breeding in the twenty-first century. In Koenig, W. D. & Dickinson, J. L., eds., Cooperative Breeding in Vertebrates. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 353374.Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2005). Children’s help and the pace of reproduction: Cooperative breeding in humans. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14, 224237.Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L., & Greaves, R. D. (2011). Juvenile subsistence effort, activity levels, and growth patterns: Middle childhood among Pumé foragers. Human Nature, 22, 303326.Google Scholar
Kulke, H., & Rothermund, D. (2004). A History of India, 4th ed. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lippold, S., Xu, H., Ko, A., et al. (2014). Human paternal and maternal demographic histories: Insights from high-resolution Y chromosome and mtDNA sequences. Investigative Genetics, 5, 1330.Google Scholar
Mann, J., Connor, R., Tyack, P., & Whitehead, H. (2000). Cetacean Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meehan, C. L., Quinlan, R., & Malcom, C. (2013). Cooperative breeding and maternal energy expenditure among Aka foragers. American Journal of Human Biology, 25, 4257.Google Scholar
Ogden, D. (1996). Greek Bastardy. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Poznik, G. D., Xue, Y., Mendez, F. L., et al. (2016). Punctuated bursts in human male demography inferred from 1,244 worldwide Y-chromosome sequences. Nature Genetics, 48, 593601.Google Scholar
Pyramid Texts (1995). Translated by J. P. Allen. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Rawson, B. (1986). Children in the Roman familia. In Rawson, B., ed., The Family in Ancient Rome. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, pp. 170200.Google Scholar
Rawson, B. (2011). A Companion to Families in the Greek and Roman Worlds. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Retief, F., Cilliers, F., & Riekert, S. (2005). Eunuchs in the Bible. Acta Theologica, Supplement, 7, 247258.Google Scholar
Scheidel, W. (2009). A peculiar institution? Greco-Roman monogamy in global context. History of the Family, 14, 280291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheidel, W., & Morris, I. (2009). The Dynamics of Ancient Empires. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 118.Google Scholar
Sennacherib (1924). Annals, translated by D. D. Luckenbill. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W., Lacey, E., Reeve, K., & Keller, L. (1995). The eusociality continuum. Behavioral Ecology, 6, 102108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, N., & French, J. (1997). Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stacey, P., & Koenig, W. (1990). Cooperative Breeding in Birds. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Strassmann, B., & Kurapati, N. (2010). Are humans cooperative breeders? Most studies of natural fertility populations do not support the grandmother hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 3539.Google Scholar
Tiglath-Pileser, III (1995). Middle Assyrian Palace Decree, translated by M. T. Roth. Atlanta, GA: Scholars Press.Google Scholar
Tougher, S. (2008). The Eunuch in Byzantine History and Society. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tougher, S. (2017). Roman Castrati: Eunuchs in the Roman Empire. London: Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Tsai, S. H. (1996). Eunuchs of the Ming Dynasty. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Tsai, S. H. (2002). Eunuch power in imperial China. In Tougher, S., ed., Eunuchs in Antiquity and Beyond. Cardiff: Classical Press of Wales, pp. 221233.Google Scholar
Turke, P. W. (1988). Helpers at the nest: Childcare networks on Ifaluk. In Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Turke, P., eds., Human Reproductive Behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 173188.Google Scholar
van Gulik, J. H. (1961). Sexual Life in Ancient China. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Voland, E., Chasiotis, A., & Schiefenhövel, W. (2005). Grandmotherhood: The Evolutionary Significance of the Second Half of Female Life. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Weeks, K. (2006). KV5: A Preliminary Report on the Excavation of the Tomb of the Sons of Rameses II in the Valley of the Kings. New York: American University in Cairo Press.Google Scholar
Weissner, P. (2002). Hunting, healing, and Hxaro exchange: A long term perspective on !Kung (Ju/’hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 130.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Wolff, J., & Sherman, P. (2007). Rodent Societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yan, S., Wang, C. C., Zheng, H. X., et al. (2014). Y chromosomes of 40% Chinese descend from three Neolithic super-grandfathers. PLoS ONE, 9, e105691.Google Scholar
Zerjal, T., Xue, Y., Bertorelle, G., et al. (2003). The genetic legacy of the Mongols. American Journal of Human Genetics, 72, 717721.Google Scholar

References

Aknin, L. B., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., et al. (2013). Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 635652.Google Scholar
Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009a). The pursuit of status in social groups. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 295298.Google Scholar
Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009b). Why do dominant personalities attain influence in face-to-face groups? The competence-signaling effects of trait dominance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 491503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C., Willer, R., Kilduff, G. J., & Brown, C. E. (2012). The origins of deference: When do people prefer lower status? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 10771088.Google Scholar
Anderson, C., Hildreth, J. A. D., & Howland, L. (2015). Is the desire for status a fundamental human motive? A review of the empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 574601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atran, S. (2002). In Gods We Trust: The Evolutionary Landscape of Religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Atran, S., & Medin, D. (2008). The Native Mind and the Cultural Construction of Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological Classification: Principles of Categorization of Plants and Animals in Traditional Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural Psychology: Research and Applications, 2nd ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blaker, N. M., Rompa, I., Dessing, I. H., et al. (2013). The height leadership advantage of men and women: Testing evolutionary psychology predictions about the perceptions of tall leaders. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16, 1727.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1987). On cultural and reproductive success: Kipsigis evidence. American Anthropologist, 89, 617634.Google Scholar
Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on religion. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 111130.Google Scholar
Brown, D. S. (1991). Human Universals. Boston: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Brown, D. S. (2001). Human universals and their implications. In Roughley, N., ed., Being Humans: Anthropological Universality and Particularity in Transdisciplinary Perspectives. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 156174.Google Scholar
Brown, D. S. (2004). Human universals, human nature and human culture. Daedalus, 133, 4754.Google Scholar
Brown, G. R., Dickins, T. E., Sear, R., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Evolutionary accounts of human behavioural diversity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 313324.Google Scholar
Buss, D. (2003). The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2014). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind, 5th ed. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P., & Picciuto, E. (2014). The origins of creativity. In Paul, E. S. & Kaufman, S. B., eds., The Philosophy of Creativity: New Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 199223.Google Scholar
Chagnon, N. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science, 239, 985992.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2014). Complex kinship patterns as evolutionary constructions, and the origins of sociocultural universals. Current Anthropology, 55, 751783.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2015). Competence, and the evolutionary origins of status and power in humans. Human Nature, 26, 161183.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2016a). The evolutionary origins of kinship structures. Structure and Dynamics, 9, 3351.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2016b). Universal aspects of kinship. In Shackelford, T. K. & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A., eds., Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Sciences. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2017). Psychological adaptations and the production of culturally polymorphic social universals. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11, 6382.Google Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 334347.Google Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103125.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2015). Adaptations for reasoning about social exchange. In Buss, D., ed., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: John Wiley and Sons, pp. 625668.Google Scholar
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335, 11141118.Google Scholar
Fogarty, L., Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2015). Cultural evolutionary perspectives on creativity and human innovation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 30, 736754.Google Scholar
Fox, R. (1980). The Red Lamp of Incest. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
Fox, R. (1989). The cultural animal. In Fox, R., ed., The Search for Society. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press, pp. 134.Google Scholar
Gabora, L., & Kaufmann, S. B. (2010). In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J., eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 279300.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006). Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 7595.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The impact of the concept of culture on the concept of man. In Geertz, C., ed., The Interpretation of Cultures. New York: Basic Books, pp. 3354.Google Scholar
Goodenough, W. H. (1970). Description and Comparison in Cultural Anthropology. Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., & von Rueden, C. (2006). Hunting, social status, and biological fitness. Social Biology, 53, 8199.Google Scholar
Hage, P. (1997). Unthinkable categories and the fundamental laws of kinship. American Ethnologist, 24, 652667.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1991). A biologist looks at anthropology. Man, 26, 583608.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1973). Man’s Place in Nature. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Irons, W. (1979). Cultural and biological success. In Chagnon, N. A. & Irons, W., eds., Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. North Sciutate, MA: Duxbury Press, pp. 257272.Google Scholar
Jones, D. (2004). The universal psychology of kinship: Evidence from language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 211215.Google Scholar
Jones, D. (2010). Human kinship, from conceptual structure to grammar. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 367416.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1985). Hunting ability and reproductive success among the Ache foragers: Preliminary results. Current Anthropology, 26, 131133.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M., & Silk, J. B., eds. (2010). Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J., eds. (2010). The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kay, P. (2005). Color categories are not arbitrary. Cross Cultural Research, 39, 7278.Google Scholar
Kluckohn, C. (1953). Universal categories of culture. In Anthropology Today: An Encyclopedic Inventory. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press, pp. 507523.Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). Evolutionary foundations of cultural psychology. In Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D., eds., Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 77105.Google Scholar
Kronenfeld, D. B. (2006). Issues in the classification of kinship terminologies: Toward a new typology. Anthropos, 101, 203219.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., & Reader, S. M. (2003). Comparative perspective on human innovation. In O’Brien, M. J. & Shennan, S. J., eds., Innovation in Cultural Systems: Contributions from Evolutionary Anthropology. Cambridge, MA: MIT press, pp. 4051.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688699.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1983). Le Regard Éloigné. Paris: Plon.Google Scholar
Lukaszewski, A. W., Anderson, C., Simmons, Z. L., & Roney, J. R. (2016). The role of physical formidability in human social status allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 385406.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B. (1944). A Scientific Theory of Culture and Other Essays. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2009). Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal Behavior, 77, 633640.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1945). The common denominators of culture. In Linton, R., ed., The Science of Man in the World Crisis. New York: Columbia University Press, pp. 123142.Google Scholar
Murray, G. R., & Schmitz, J. D. (2011). Caveman politics: Evolutionary leadership preferences and physical stature. Social Science Quarterly, 92, 12151235.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2009). Beyond nature versus culture: Cultural variation as an evolved characteristic. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 15, 223240.Google Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2010). Evolutionary social psychology. In Dunbar, R. & Barrett, L., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 761796.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 763784.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate. New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Re, D. E., DeBruine, L. B., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2013). Facial cues to perceived height influence leadership choices in simulated war and peace contexts. Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 89103.Google Scholar
Sabo, D., Kupers, T. A., & London, W., eds. (2001). Prisons Masculinities. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (2010). Female chacma baboons form strong, equitable, and enduring social bonds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 60, 197204Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature, 15, 343364.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (1999). Social Psychology across Cultures, 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man, 20, 7389.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 24042415.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J., eds., The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., & Cheng, J. T. (2010). A naturalist’s view of pride. Emotion Review, 2, 163177.Google Scholar
von Rueden, C. (2014). The roots and fruits of social status in small-scale human societies. In Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Anderson, C., eds., The Psychology of Social Status. Berlin: Springer, pp. 179200.Google Scholar
von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2011). Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 278, 22232232.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2010). Dominance, power, and politics in non-human and human primates. In Kappeler, P. M. & Silk, J. B., eds., Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Berlin: Springer, pp. 109138.Google Scholar
Weisfeld, G. E., & Dillon, L. M. (2012). Applying the dominance hierarchy model to pride and shame, and related behaviors. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 1541.Google Scholar

References

Abu-Lughod, L. (1996). Writing against culture. In Fox, R. G., ed., Recapturing Anthropology: Working in the Present. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press, pp.137162.Google Scholar
Allen, J. S., & Jobson, R. C. (2016). The decolonizing generation: (Race and) theory in anthropology since the Eighties. Current Anthropology, 57, 129148.Google Scholar
Aoki, K., Wakano, J. Y., & Feldman, M. W. (2005). The emergence of social learning in a temporally changing environment: A theoretical model. Current Anthropology, 46, 334340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apicella, C. L., & Barrett, H. C. (2016). Cross-cultural evolutionary psychology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 9297.Google Scholar
Arnold, M. (1869). Culture and Anarchy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., Hecht, M. L., & Lindsley, S. L. (2006). Redefining Culture: Perspectives across Disciplines. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (2005). Introduction: Sometimes the bus does wait. In Barkow, J. H, ed., Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J., eds. (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, C., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). The hominid entry into the cognitive niche. In Gangestad, S. W. & Simpson, J. A., eds., Evolution of Mind. New York: Guilford, pp. 241248.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2015). The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Broesch, J. (2012). Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 499508.Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., & Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 271, 14431450.Google Scholar
Bentley, R., Lipo, C. P., Herzog, H. A., & Hahn, M. W. (2007). Regular rates of popular culture change reflect random copying. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 151158.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1940). Race, Language and Culture. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1990). Kipsigis women’s preferences for wealthy men: Evidence for female choice in mammals? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27, 255264.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1998). The demographic transition: Are we any closer to an evolutionary explanation? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 266270.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Schacht, R. (2012). Human behavioral ecology. In eLS. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Bouckaert, R., Lemey, P., Dunn, M., et al. (2012). Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science, 337, 957960.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 125143.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1091810925.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human Universals. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2009). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston, MA: Pearson.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., et al. (1990). International preferences in selecting mates: A study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 547.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In Barringer, H. R., Blanksten, G. I., & Mack, R. W., eds., Social Change in Developing Areas. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman, pp. 1949.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chater, N., Reali, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2009). Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 10151020.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489509.Google Scholar
Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 89111.Google Scholar
Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Sperber, D. (2014). How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130368.Google Scholar
Clifford, J., & Marcus, G. E. (1986). Writing Culture: The Poetics and Politics of Ethnography. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Colleran, H. (2016). The cultural evolution of fertility decline. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150152.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187276.Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1999). That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Crook, J., & Crook, S. J. (1988). Tibetan polyandry: Problems of adaptation and fitness. In Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Turke, P., eds., Human Reproductive Behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 97114.Google Scholar
Currie, T. E., & Mace, R. (2012). Analyses do not support the parasite-stress theory of human sociality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 8385.Google Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide, Foundations of Human Behavior. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dean, L. G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89, 284301.Google Scholar
Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Critical social learning: A solution to Rogers’ paradox of nonadaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109, 727734.Google Scholar
Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14, 126.Google Scholar
Feldman, M. (2014). Echoes of the past: Hereditarianism and a troublesome inheritance.PLoS Genetics, 10, e1004817.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 6179.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 275, 12791285.Google Scholar
Fracchia, J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1999). Does culture evolve? History and Theory, 38, 5278.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M., & Perry, S., eds. (2003). The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. (1974). The evolutionary theories of Charles Darwin and Herbert Spencer. Current Anthropology, 15, 211237.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience, 55, 489499.Google Scholar
Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153172.Google Scholar
Gould, S. (1996). The Mismeasure of Man. London: W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The Spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 205, 581598.Google Scholar
Grafen, A. (1984). Natural selection, kin selection and group selection. In Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., eds., Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific, pp. 6284.Google Scholar
Gray, R. D., Greenhill, S. J., & Ross, R. M. (2007). The pleasures and perils of Darwinizing culture (with phylogenies). Biological Theory, 2, 360375.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J. (2011). Cultural Psychology. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations. American Anthropologist, 103, 9921013.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004). Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic and Behavioral Organization, 53, 335.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2006). Cooperation, punishment, and the evolution of human institutions. Science, 312, 6061.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2015). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 277, 37153724.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., et al. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 14801484.Google Scholar
Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M., Yaworsky, W., & Kickham, K. (2014). Whither the blank slate? A report on the reception of evolutionary biological ideas among sociological theorists. Sociological Spectrum, 34, 489509.Google Scholar
Hruschka, D. J., & Henrich, J. (2013). Institutions, parasites and the persistence of in-group preferences. PLoS ONE, 8, e63642.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2007). The trouble with “evolutionary biology.” Anthropology Today, 23, 37.Google Scholar
Kempe, M., Lycett, S. J., & Mesoudi, A. (2014). From cultural traditions to cumulative culture: Parameterizing the differences between human and nonhuman culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 359, 2936.Google Scholar
Kendal, J., Giraldeau, L. A., & Laland, K. (2009). The evolution of social learning rules: Payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 260, 210219.Google Scholar
Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 52415245.Google Scholar
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1068110686.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1985). Vaulting Ambition: Sociobiology and the Quest for Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2013). Meeting now suggests we will meet again: Implications for debates on the evolution of cooperation. Scientific Reports, 3, 1747.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (1984). Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1917). The superorganic. American Anthropologist, 19, 163213.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckohn, C. (1952). Culture. New York: Vantage.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., & Haselton, M. G. (2006). Making hay out of straw? Real and imagined controversies in evolutionary psychology. In Barkow, J. H., ed., Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 149162.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lamba, S., & Mace, R. (2011). Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1442614430.Google Scholar
Lawson, D. W., Alvergne, A., & Gibson, M. A. (2012). The life-history trade-off between fertility and child survival. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 279, 47554764.Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J., & Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lyle, H. F., & Smith, E. A. (2012). How conservative are evolutionary anthropologists? Human Nature, 23, 306322.Google Scholar
Mace, R., & Holden, C. J. (2005). A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 20, 116121.Google Scholar
Matthews, L. J., Passmore, S., Richard, P. M., Gray, R. D., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2016). Shared cultural history as a predictor of political and economic changes among nation states. PLoS ONE, 11, e0152979.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J., & Warren, N. (1982). Models of cultural and genetic change. Evolution, 36, 620627.Google Scholar
McElreath, R. (2016). Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and Stan. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., & Strimling, P. (2008). When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Current Anthropology, 49, 307316.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2009). The cultural dynamics of copycat suicide. PLoS ONE, 4, e7252.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 124.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 363, 34893501.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2004). Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution, 58, 111.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405423.Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015). Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 303312.Google Scholar
Morgan, L. H. (1877). Ancient Society. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1967). Ethnographic Atlas. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2009). Ecological influences on human behavioral diversity: A review of recent findings. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 24, 618624.Google Scholar
Nettle, D., Gibson, M. A., Lawson, D. W., & Sear, R. (2013). Human behavioral ecology: current research and future prospects. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 10311040.Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2003). Cladistics and Archaeology. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, aac4716.Google Scholar
Pagel, M. (2009). Human language as a culturally transmitted replicator. Nature Reviews: Genetics, 10, 405415.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: W. Morrow.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 89938999.Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. (1995). Darwin Machines and the Nature of Knowledge. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone. Chicago, IL: University Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., Baldini, R., Bell, A., et al. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: a sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e30.Google Scholar
Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819831.Google Scholar
Rose, S., & Rose, H., eds. (2000). Alas Poor Darwin: Arguments against Evolutionary Psychology. London: Jonathon Cape.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1976). The Use and Abuse of Biology: An Anthropological Critique of Sociobiology. Oxford: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006). Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 96101.Google Scholar
Segerstråle, U. C. O. (2000). Defenders of the Truth: The Battle for Science in the Sociobiology Debate and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Servedio, M. R., Brandvain, Y., Dhole, S., et al. (2014). Not just a theory – The utility of mathematical models in evolutionary biology. PLoS Biology, 12, e1002017.Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E. (2016). Models are stupid, and we need more of them. In Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., & Read, S. J., eds., Computational Models in Social Psychology. Hove: Psychology Press, pp. 311331.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (1998). Is Tibetan polyandry adaptive? Methodological and metatheoretical analyses. Human Nature, 9, 225261.Google Scholar
Sociobiology Study Group of Science for the People (1976). Sociobiology: Another biological determinism. BioScience, 26, 182186.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 24052415.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410433.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. (2014). Learning and culture. Edge.org. What Scientific Idea Is Ready for Retirement? www.edge.org/response-detail/25343.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J., eds., The Adapted Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Turchin, P. (2003). Historical Dynamics: Why States Rise and Fall. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Turchin, P. (2015). Ultrasociety: How 10,000 Years of War Made Humans the Greatest Cooperators on Earth. Chaplin, CT: Beresta Books.Google Scholar
Turchin, P., Currie, T. E., Turner, E. A. L., & Gavrilets, S. (2013). War, space, and the evolution of Old World complex societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1638416389.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive Culture. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Viveiros de Castro, E. (2014). Who is afraid of the ontological wolf? Some comments on an ongoing anthropological debate. Presented at: CUSAS Annual Marilyn Strathern Lecture, May 30, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 231262.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Caldwell, C. A., & Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural diffusion in humans and other animals. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 1521.Google Scholar
Williams, G. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 5172.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×