Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T23:21:52.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 2 - Relative chronology and an ‘Aeolic phase’ of epic

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2012

Øivind Andersen
Affiliation:
Universitetet i Oslo
Dag T. T. Haug
Affiliation:
Universitetet i Oslo
Get access

Summary

Numerous attempts to establish the relative chronology of the songs of the Greek epos, or of particular passages therein, have depended on the linguistic evidence in our received texts. These studies, while producing some suggestive results, have not solved the problem of chronology for early Greek poetry. The language of epic developed in parallel with the colloquial speech of the singers in many ways, but the Kunstsprache has rules and tendencies which work counter to the diachronic development of the singers’ dialect. Also, assumptions about the history of the development of the epics heavily influence the definition of linguistic material as ‘archaic’. As we shall see below, one's model for the development of the songs of the Greek epos, whether through a series of dialectal ‘phases’ or through the diffusion of shared poetic material among various local traditions, has a profound effect on attempts to elicit chronology from the language of the epics.

The mixed dialect of epic

The dialect of the epics mixes archaic forms with innovations; it likewise mixes Ionic forms with forms showing Aeolic phonology and morphology. Linguistic archaisms by definition precede innovations, and concentrations of innovative forms are generally taken to indicate a later work or passage. Still, the presence of any linguistic innovation indicates that a passage could only have been composed after that innovation had occurred. For example, a (metrically guaranteed) ‘neglect’ of digamma could not reasonably have entered epic language until initial [w-] had lost consonantal force in some relevant dialect. Clearly, however, the effects of digamma persisted in epic singing well after the sound itself had disappeared, and thus, any apparent ‘observance’ of digamma, say, need not have entered the text before the loss of the sound in colloquial speech.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×