Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T17:23:00.064Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Structural brain correlates of unconstrained motor activity in people with schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Tom F. D. Farrow*
Affiliation:
Sheffield Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory (SCANLab), Department of Academic Clinical Psychiatry
Michael D. Hunter
Affiliation:
Sheffield Cognition and Neuroimaging Laboratory (SCANLab), Department of Academic Clinical Psychiatry
Iain D. Wilkinson
Affiliation:
Department of Academic Radiology
Russell D. J. Green
Affiliation:
SCANLab, Department of Academic Clinical Psychiatry University of Sheffield, UK
Sean A. Spence
Affiliation:
SCANLab, Department of Academic Clinical Psychiatry University of Sheffield, UK
*
Dr Tom F. D. Farrow, SCANLab, Department of Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Northern General Hospital, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK. E-mail: t.f.farrow@sheffield.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Avolition affects quality of life in chronic schizophrenia. We investigated the relationship between unconstrained motor activity and the volume of key executive brain regions in 16 male patients with schizophrenia. Wrist-worn actigraphy monitors were used to record motor activity over a 20 h period. Structural magnetic resonance imaging brain scans were parcellated and individual volumes for anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex extracted. Patients' total activity was positively correlated with volume of left anterior cingulate cortex. These data suggest that the volume of specific executive structures may affect (quantifiable) motor behaviours, having further implications for models of the ‘will’ and avolition.

Type
Short Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Some modular theories of brain organisation propose a relationship between regional volume and psychological function. In this study we recorded the spontaneous, unconstrained motor behaviour of people with schizophrenia and examined whether such behaviour was related to the volume of key executive brain regions: anterior cingulate (Brodmann area (BA) 32) and dorsolateral prefrontal (BA 9 and BA 46) cortices.

The anterior cingulate cortex plays an integrative role in the frontal lobes, translating intentions into actions (Reference PausPaus, 2001). It contributes to motor control, complex executive processing, vigilance, arousal and ‘drive’ (Reference Spence and FrithSpence & Frith, 1999). Together with its involvement in autonomic functioning, these processes suggest that the anterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in overall behavioural control (Reference Devinsky, Morrell and VogtDevinsky et al, 1995), particularly in goal-directed behaviours (Reference Spence and FrithSpence & Frith, 1999). Conversely, anterior cingulate cortex lesions are associated with reduced spontaneous behaviour and attention, as manifest in akinetic mutism (Reference Devinsky, Morrell and VogtDevinsky et al, 1995).

The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (including anterior cingulate cortex) in schizophrenia has been much investigated. Structurally there is evidence of abnormal medial prefrontal gyral patterns and cytoarchitecture, whereas functionally there is evidence of reduced perfusion in the psychomotor poverty sub-syndrome (alogia, flatness of affect and decreased spontaneous movement (Reference Liddle, Friston and FrithLiddle et al, 1992). A negative correlation was found between patients’ poverty scores and left ventro-medial prefrontal grey matter density (Reference Chua, Wright and PolineChua et al, 1997).

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, particularly BA 9 and BA 46, has been implicated in behavioural response selection tasks, including finger movement (Reference Hunter, Green and WilkinsonHunter et al, 2004), random number generation and verbal fluency, all requiring the individual to generate novelty. Therefore, while anterior cingulate cortex is implicated in the quantity of motor activity performed by the organism, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions are implicated in behavioural complexity (e.g. how novel the response patterns are that emerge) (Reference Spence and FrithSpence & Frith, 1999).

We hypothesised that in schizophrenia the extent of motor activity over a prolonged period (20 h) might be constrained by anatomical features of the frontal executive system, specifically the anterior cingulate cortex (BA 32) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9/46).

METHOD

Sixteen right-handed patients with DSM–IV schizophrenia (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) gave written informed consent and participated in this study, approved by the North Sheffield Research Ethics Committee. All participants were community-based out-patients, but for monitoring purposes were admitted to a psychiatric ward for 24 h. Participants had a mean age of 36 years (s.d.=8), a mean illness duration of 14 years (s.d.=8), a mean score on the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Reference AndreasenAndreasen, 1985) of 11.5 (s.d.=2.7), a Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Reference AndreasenAndreasen, 1985) score of 3.6 (s.d.=2.1) and a Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Reference Beck, Steer and BrownBeck et al, 1996) score of 9.5 (s.d.=10.3). Eleven patients were taking oral atypical antipsychotic medication (olanzapine, 6; clozapine, 4; quetiapine, 1), one was taking oral typical medication (sulpiride) and four were receiving typical depot medication (flupenthixol decanoate, 2; fluphenazine decanoate, 1; zuclopenthixol decanoate, 1). Patients underwent a structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan on a 1.5 Tesla system (Eclipse, Philips Medical Systems, Ohio, USA) using a three-dimensional acquisition technique (RF-spoiled FAST; repetition time=15 ms; echo time=4.4 ms; acquisition matrix=256 × 256 × 190 yielding a voxel size of 1mm3) which produced a T1-weighted volume dataset covering the entire brain. Scans were pre-processed using voxel-based morphometry with statistical parametric mapping (SPM2) (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London). Smoothed grey matter segmented maps were parcellated using masks created with WFP_PickAtlas v1.02 (Reference Maldjian, Laurienti and BurdetteMaldjian et al, 2003; see data supplement to online version of this paper). Volumes of anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex grey matter regions were obtained for each participant. Following the scan, participants wore an ‘Actiwatch’ (Cambridge Neurotechnology, UK) measuring their cumulative activity over a 20 h period. The Actiwatch is a wrist-worn device containing a miniature uniaxial accelerometer which produces a digital integration of the amount and duration of all movement over 0.05 g. As an indicator of normal daytime activity, a study of 107 healthy 16- to 19-year-old adolescents recorded mean Actiwatch readings of 162 565 (s.d.=68 620) (a dimensionless measure) over a 24 h period (Nancy Butte, personal communication, 2004). We ran patient-wise parametric bivariate correlations between total motor activity and volumes of bilateral anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

RESULTS

Over 20 h the mean Actiwatch reading was 106 722 (s.d.=36 553). The mean regional grey matter volume was 4.7 ml (s.d.=0.6) for left anterior cingulate cortex, 5.0 ml (s.d.=0.4) for right anterior cingulate cortex, 5.3 ml (s.d.=1.0) for left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and 4.4 ml (s.d.=0.6) for right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. There were no correlations between patients’ ages or durations of illness (which are themselves of course highly positively correlated) or SANS scores and anterior cingulate cortex or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes. There were negative correlations between patients’ ages and durations of illness and their total activity measures (r=–0.549, P=0.034 and r=–0.621, P=0.014, respectively). There was a negative correlation between SANS avolition score and Actiwatch-measured motor activity (r=–0.52, P=0.047), although this result should be interpreted in the light of the ordinal nature of the SANS and small variance in our patients’ avolition scores (range 2–4). There were no correlations between chlorpromazine-equivalent drug doses or BDI scores and motor activity. Total activity over 20 h was positively correlated with volume of the left anterior cingulate cortex (r=0.487, P=0.028; Fig. 1). There were no significant correlations between cumulative Actiwatch activity and right anterior cingulate cortex or left or right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes (P>0.3).

Fig. 1 Positive correlation in 16 patients with chronic schizophrenia between their total cumulative activity over 20 h and left anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann area 32) volumes.

DISCUSSION

In these people with schizophrenia, motor activity over a 20 h period was correlated with the volume of left anterior cingulate cortex, a finding supportive of our central hypothesis that anatomy may constrain function, in this case spontaneous motor behaviour. In particular, it was the anterior cingulate cortex rather than the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that was implicated, consistent with a methodology that measures ‘volume’ of motor behaviour, without regard to its complexity. This finding is congruent with the role of anterior cingulate cortex in the emergence of motor acts, and its relative dysfunction in psychomotor poverty (Reference Liddle, Friston and FrithLiddle et al, 1992; Reference Chua, Wright and PolineChua et al, 1997) and akinetic mutism (Reference Devinsky, Morrell and VogtDevinsky et al, 1995).

In terms of methodology, ours is the first study of regional brain volume in schizophrenia to use an automated measure of spontaneous motor activity. This novel use of an actigraphy monitor as an objective, scalar measure of bodily movement, as opposed to the more usually quoted, subjective, ordinal measure of avolition (in the SANS), has the potential to augment validity in clinical studies and certainly detects greater between-subject variance. This may be particularly useful in patient assessments where change is anticipated (as with behavioural or pharmacological interventions).

We must be cautious in extrapolating our finding to community, ambulatory patients as our study concerned patients who spent the days in question on a psychiatric ward. Nevertheless, our data do offer the intriguing possibility that anatomy and function are related with respect to spontaneous, unconstrained motor activity. Of course, there is a final caveat in that we do not know the extent to which measured behaviour is purposeful (Reference Spence and FrithSpence & Frith, 1999), but this is the subject of ongoing work (Reference Hunter, Green and WilkinsonHunter et al, 2004).

In summary, our findings suggest that the volume of the left anterior cingulate cortex in people with chronic schizophrenia correlates with an objective measure of their unconstrained motor activity.

Data supplement 1 Extent of left anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann area 32) mask in the axial (1-ventral to 3-dorsal), coronal (4-anterior to 6-posterior) and sagittal (7-medial to 8-lateral) planes.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by an investigator-led grant awarded to S.A.S. by Cephalon UK. M.D.H. is supported by the Wellcome Trust. We thank Professor Nancy Butte, Baylor College of Medicine, Texas, USA for normative Actiwatch data.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). Washington, DC. APA.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1985) Positive vs. negative schizophrenia: a critical evaluation. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 380389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. Q. & Brown, G. K. (1996) Manual for Beck Depression Inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Chua, S. E., Wright, I. C., Poline, J.-B., et al (1997) Grey matter correlates of syndromes in schizophrenia. A semi-automated analysis of structural magnetic resonance images. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 406410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J. & Vogt, B. A. (1995) Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118, 279306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, M. D., Green, R. D. J., Wilkinson, I. D., et al (2004) Spatial and temporal dissociation in prefrontal cortex during action execution. Neuroimage, 23, 11861191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddle, P. F., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., et al (1992) Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 179186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Burdette, J. H., et al (2003) An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage, 19, 12331239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T. (2001) Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 417424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S. A. & Frith, C. D. (1999) Towards a functional anatomy of volition. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 1129.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Positive correlation in 16 patients with chronic schizophrenia between their total cumulative activity over 20 h and left anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann area 32) volumes.

Figure 1

Data supplement 1 Extent of left anterior cingulate cortex (Brodmann area 32) mask in the axial (1-ventral to 3-dorsal), coronal (4-anterior to 6-posterior) and sagittal (7-medial to 8-lateral) planes.

Supplementary material: PDF

Farrow et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Figure S1

Download Farrow et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.4 MB
Supplementary material: File

Farrow et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Farrow et al. supplementary material(File)
File 565 Bytes
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.