Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T01:26:48.310Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reconsolidation: Turning consciousness into memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Mark Solms*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch 7701, South Africa. mark.solms@uct.ac.za

Abstract

The purpose of learning is not to maintain records but to generate predictions. Successful predictions remain implicit; only prediction errors (“surprises”) attract consciousness. This is what Freud had in mind when he declared that “consciousness arises instead of a memory-trace.” The aim of reconsolidation, and of psychotherapy, is to improve predictions about how to meet our needs in the world.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bargh, J. (2014) Our unconscious mind. Scientific American 310:3037.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. & Friston, K. (2010) The default mode, ego functions and free energy: A neurobiological account of Freudian ideas. Brain 133:1265–83.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2010) Self comes to mind. Pantheon.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1895) The neuro-psychoses of defence. In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, vol. 3. ed., Strachey, J., pp. 4661. Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1900) The interpretation of dreams . In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, vols. 4–5. ed., Strachey, J.. Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1911) Formulations on the two principles of mental functioning. In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, vol. 12. Ed. Strachey, J., pp. 215–26. Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1920) Beyond the pleasure principle. In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, vol.18. ed., Strachey, J., pp. 764. Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:127–38.Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H. (1866) Concerning the perceptions in general. In: Treatise on physiological optics, 3rd edition, trans. Southall, J.. Dover.Google Scholar
Merker, B. (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30:63134.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998) Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Solms, M. (2013) The conscious id. Neuropsychoanalysis 15:519.Google Scholar
Solms, M. (2014) A neuropsychoanalytical approach to the hard problem of consciousness. Integrative Neuroscience 13:113.Google Scholar
Solms, M. & Panksepp, J. (2012) The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sciences 2(2):147–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar