Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:40:22.113Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association between consideration of future consequences and food intake is mediated by food choice motives in a French adult population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2024

Marc Bénard
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Margaux Robert
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Caroline Méjean
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France MOISA, Univ Montpellier, INRAE, CIRAD, CIHEAM-IAMM, Montpellier SupAgro, Montpellier, France
Benjamin Allès
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Emmanuelle Kesse-Guyot
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Pauline Paolassini-Guesnier
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
France Bellisle
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Fabrice Etilé
Affiliation:
Paris School of Economics and INRAE, UMR1393 PjSE, 48 Boulevard Jourdan, Paris, France
Gérard Reach
Affiliation:
Department of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolic Diseases, Avicenne Hospital, Bobigny, France
Serge Hercberg
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France Public Health Department, Avicenne Hospital, Bobigny, France
Mathilde Touvier
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
Sandrine Péneau*
Affiliation:
Sorbonne Paris Nord University, Inserm U1153, Inrae U1125, Cnam, Nutritional Epidemiology Research Team (EREN), Epidemiology and Statistics Research Center – University of Paris (CRESS), 74, rue Marcel Cachin, 93017 Bobigny, France
*
*Corresponding author: Email s.peneau@eren.smbh.univ-paris13.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives:

Consideration of future consequences (CFC) distinguishes individuals who adopt behaviours based on immediate needs and concerns from individuals who consider the future consequences of their behaviours. We aimed to assess the association between CFC and diet, and testing the mediating role of food choice motives on this relationship.

Design:

Individuals (aged ≥ 18 years) completed the CFC-12 questionnaire in 2014, at least three 24-h dietary records, and a food choice motive questionnaire. A multiple mediator analysis allowed to assess the mediating effect of food choice motives on the cross-sectional association between CFC and diet, adjusted for socio-demographic factors.

Setting:

Data from the NutriNet-Santé cohort study.

Participants:

27 330 participants.

Results:

CFC was associated with all food choice motives (P < 0·001), with the strongest positive associations for avoidance for environmental reasons, absence of contaminants and health motives and the strongest negative associations for innovation and convenience. Positive total effects were found between CFC and the consumption of healthy food groups (fruits and vegetables, whole-grain foods, legumes), and negative total effects for alcohol, meat and poultry and processed meat (P < 0·001). CFC was positively associated with diet quality (P < 0·001). Across food groups, major mediators of these relationships were higher health (8·4–32·6%), higher environmental (13·7–22·1 %) and lower innovation (7·3–25·1 %) concerns.

Conclusions:

CFC was associated with healthier dietary intake, essentially mediated by a greater motivation of future-oriented participants for self-centred and altruistic outcomes, including health and environment. Focusing on the awareness of future benefits in public health interventions might lead to healthier dietary behaviours.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Current western eating patterns, which are characterised by high dietary fat, salt and sugar and low fruit and vegetable consumption, can lead to undesirable impacts on health and environment(Reference Tilman and Clark1). To encourage healthy food choices, there is a need to better understand the determinants of consumer motives when purchasing and eating food. Psychological factors represent important determinants of food choice(Reference Shepherd and Raats2). Consideration of future consequences (CFC) is a psychological construct that measures the extent to which people consider the potential consequences of their actions and the extent by which they are influenced by these potential consequences(Reference Strathman, Gleicher and Boninger3). Individuals with higher CFC are therefore expected to adopt a future-oriented lifestyle that favours their distant goals and long-term concerns over immediate needs.

CFC is associated with health decision-making behaviours(Reference Joireman and King4), such as less smoking in undergraduate students(Reference Daugherty and Brase5), more physical exercise in students(Reference Daugherty and Brase5,Reference Joireman, Shaffer and Balliet6) and greater preventive behaviours including getting screened for diabetes in an adult population(Reference Orbell and Hagger7). These data suggest that individual differences in CFC could lead to differences in food choice and dietary intake. CFC has been, for example, shown to be positively associated with self-perceived healthy eating behaviours in students(Reference Joireman and King4,Reference Joireman, Shaffer and Balliet6,Reference van Beek, Handgraaf and Antonides8) or healthier food choices in a representative sample of the Norwegian population(Reference Tórtora and Ares9). More specifically, CFC was negatively associated with intention to consume fast food(Reference Dunn, Mohr and Wilson10) or to display emotional eating behaviour(Reference Bénard, Bellisle and Etilé11), while in students, CFC was positively associated with a lower alcohol consumption(Reference Strathman, Gleicher and Boninger3). The addition of CFC to a Health Belief Model improves its prediction of healthy behaviour, as the predictive capacity increases compared with models without CFC, which may be explained by the fact that CFC is a significant determinant of healthy dietary behaviour(Reference Orji, Vassileva and Mandryk12). In another study, no association was observed with saturated fat or fruit and vegetables intake, which may be due to a potential mediation by intention(Reference Mullan, Allom and Brogan13). In our study, we hypothesise that adults with higher CFC would favour choice and consumption of healthier food groups compared with individuals who favour immediate ‘rewards’.

Food choices are influenced by various motives such as healthiness(Reference Baudry, Péneau and Allès14), taste(Reference Baudry, Péneau and Allès14,Reference Kourouniotis, Keast and Riddell15) or price(Reference Baudry, Péneau and Allès14). Environmental motives have been shown to play an increasing role in food choice decisions(Reference Baudry, Péneau and Allès14) and therefore on dietary intake(Reference Allès, Péneau and Kesse-Guyot16). In turn, there are numerous factors influencing food choice motives such as biological, social and cultural factors(Reference Shepherd and Raats2). In particular, individual differences of CFC could explain differences in food choice motives, but to our knowledge, there are no data available in the literature on this potential association. We hypothesised that specific food choice motives result from considering immediate (e.g. taste or price) or future (e.g. health or environment) consequences and that food choice motives could mediate the association between CFC and food intake.

The purposes of the present cross-sectional study were to assess the associations between CFC and dietary intakes (diet quality and food groups) and to evaluate the potential mediating roles of food motives, with a focus on sustainability, on these associations in a large sample of French adults.

Methods

Population

This study was conducted as part of the NutriNet-Santé study, which is a large ongoing web-based prospective cohort started in France in May 2009. The rationale, design and methods of the study have been described elsewhere(Reference Hercberg, Castetbon and Czernichow17). The overall aim of this study is to explore the relationships between nutrition and health, as well as the determinants of eating behaviour and nutritional status. Eligible participants are adults (age ≥ 18 years) of a French population who have access to the Internet. Participants were recruited through a vast multimedia campaign (television, radio, national and regional newspapers, posters, Internet). At inclusion, participants have to complete several web-based questionnaires via a secure website (http://www.etude-nutrinet-sante.fr) to assess their diet, physical activity, anthropometric measures, lifestyle characteristics, socio-economic conditions and health status. Participants then complete this same set of questionnaires every year. Another set of optional questionnaires related to determinants of eating behaviours, nutritional status and specific health-related aspects are sent to every participant each month. In the present cross-sectional study, we included participants who had completed the CFC questionnaire, the food motives questionnaire and at least three dietary records.

Data collection

Consideration of future consequences

CFC was assessed with the French version of the CFC-12 questionnaire(Reference Demarque, Apostolidis and Chagnard18) completed from June to November 2014. The CFC-12 is a 12-item self-report questionnaire(Reference Strathman, Gleicher and Boninger3) developed to measure the extent to which individuals consider distant v. immediate consequences of their behaviour. Each item is measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘extremely uncharacteristic’ to ‘extremely characteristic’. An example of the items of the CFC-12 is as follows: I consider how things might be in the future, and try to influence those things with my day-to-day behavior. The total score is obtained by summing all the item ratings and by dividing the result by 12 for a final score ranging from 1 to 5 (higher scores indicating greater CFC). Good internal consistency was obtained in our sample with a Cronbach’s α of 0·79.

Assessment of food choice motives during purchase

Data regarding food choice motives were collected from July to December 2013 using a validated questionnaire to measure food choice motives including sustainability concerns at the time of purchase(Reference Le Moullec, Deheeger and Preziosi19). The questionnaire is based on sixty-three items with items focusing on food in general (thirty-two items) or on specific food groups (meat/fish/fruits and vegetables/dairy products) (thirty-one items). Questions are formulated as follows: ‘When I purchase food/meat/fish/fruits and vegetables/dairy products, I take into account…’. Participants are asked to rate their level of agreement on a 5-point Likert scale from ‘I strongly disagree’ to ‘I strongly agree’. Respondents can also indicate that they do not know. The underlying structure of the questionnaire was determined by exploratory factor analysis and then internally validated by confirmatory factor analysis(Reference Sautron, Péneau and Camilleri20). Reliability was also assessed by internal consistency of selected dimensions and test–retest repeatability. Nine food choice dimensions were identified following the confirmatory factor analysis(Reference Tilman and Clark1): ethics and environment (18 items, e.g. production waste, impact on earth’s resources)(Reference Shepherd and Raats2), local and traditional production (12 items, e.g. proximity of production, support for small-scale producers)(Reference Strathman, Gleicher and Boninger3), taste (4 items)(Reference Joireman and King4), price (6 items)(Reference Daugherty and Brase5); avoidance for environmental reasons (4 items, e.g. not buying meat for environmental reasons)(Reference Joireman, Shaffer and Balliet6), health (6 items, e.g. health impact, nutritional composition)(Reference Orbell and Hagger7); convenience (4 items, e.g. cooking convenience)(Reference van Beek, Handgraaf and Antonides8), innovation (4 items, e.g. original or innovative product, innovative fabrication/conservation process) and(Reference Tórtora and Ares9) absence of contaminants (5 items, e.g. additives, exposure to chemicals). The ‘avoidance for environmental reasons’ dimension is different from the motivation ‘ethics and environment’ as it implies a radical commitment for conserving the environment. Since each dimension consisted of different number of items (from 4 to 18 items), all dimensions of food motives were transformed into values ranging from 0 (no concern) to 10 (strong concern) to standardise ratings.

Assessment of food groups consumption and diet quality

We selected participants who completed at least three 24-h dietary records between the 2 years preceding and the 2 years following the completion of the CFC-12 questionnaire. Participants are requested to complete two records during the week, and one during the week-end for a better representativeness. For each day, participants indicate food consumed and portions sizes. The 24-h dietary record is completed by using an interactive interface and is designed for self-administration on the Internet(Reference Touvier, Kesse-Guyot and Méjean21). Participants report all foods and beverages consumed at breakfast, lunch, dinner and other eating occasions. They estimate the amounts eaten using standard measurements or using validated photographs(Reference Le Moullec, Deheeger and Preziosi19). These photographs represent > 250 foods (corresponding to 1,000 generic foods). Participants can choose between 7 portion sizes for most food products: 3 main portion sizes plus 2 intermediate and 2 extreme sizes. Nutrient intakes are estimated by using the published NutriNet-Santé food composition table including > 2,000 foods(Reference Nutrinet-Santé22). Mean daily food intake (in g/d) is weighted for the type of day of the week (weekday or weekend). Under reporters were identified by using the method proposed by Black(Reference Black23) and were then excluded. The use of the 24-h dietary record has been validated in the NutriNet-Santé cohort(Reference Touvier, Kesse-Guyot and Méjean21). For the present study, we defined seventeen food groups: fruit and vegetables; seafood (e.g. fish and shellfish); meat and poultry; processed meat; eggs; dairy products (e.g. milk, yogurts with less than 12 % of added sugar, cheese); milk-based desserts (e.g. sweet yogurts, flan, cottage cheese, cream desserts); starchy foods; whole-grain foods; legumes; fats (oil, butter and margarines); sugary and fatty foods (e.g. cakes, chocolate, ice cream, pancakes); sugars and confectionery (e.g. honey, jelly, sugar, candy); fast food (e.g. pizzas, hamburgers, sandwiches, hot dogs); appetisers (e.g. crisps, salted biscuits); non-alcoholic beverages (excluding water) and alcoholic beverages.

The modified French National Nutrition and Health Program Guideline Score (mPNNS-GS), which is an a priori nutritional diet quality score reflecting the adherence to the French nutritional recommendations, was assessed. It is based on the PNNS-GS score(Reference Estaquio, Kesse-Guyot and Deschamps24) but accounts for dietary component only, excluding the physical activity component. The score includes twelve components: eight refer to food serving recommendations (fruit and vegetables; starchy foods; whole-grain products; dairy products; meat, eggs and fish; seafood; vegetable fat; water v. soda) and four refer to moderation of nutrients or food (added fat; salt; sweets; and alcohol). Points are deducted for overconsumptions of salt and added sugars from sweetened foods, and when energy intake exceeds the energy requirement (as assessed by physical activity level and BMR calculated using Schofield equations(Reference Schofield25)) by more than 5 %. The score has a maximum of 13·5 points, with a higher score indicating a better overall nutritional quality of the diet.

Socio-economic and demographic data

Potential confounders of the relationship between CFC, food choice motives and dietary data were collected based on the latest data to the date of completion of the CFC-12, which are provided yearly by the participants after their inclusion: age (years), sex (male, female), education level (primary, secondary, undergraduate and postgraduate), occupational status (unemployed, student, self-employed and farmer, employee and manual worker, intermediate profession, managerial staff and intellectual profession and retired) and monthly income per household unit. Monthly income per household unit was calculated using information about income and household composition. The number of people in the household was converted into a number of consumption units according to the Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development equivalence scale: one consumption unit is attributed for the first adult in the household, 0·5 for other persons aged 14 or older and 0·3 for children under 14(26). Categories of monthly income were defined as follows: < 1,200; 1,200–1,799; 1,800–2,299; 2,300–2,699; 2,700–3,699 and > 3,700 euros per household unit as well as ‘unwilling to answer’.

Statistical analysis

A total of 51 394 participants of the NutriNet-Santé cohort study completed the CFC-12 questionnaire. Among them, 439 participants were excluded because they presented an acquiescence bias (agreeing or disagreeing to all questions without consideration of reversed items). From the 50 955 remaining participants, 27 396 participants completed the questionnaire on food choice motives and at least three dietary records to compose the final sample. Compared with excluded participants (the 23 559 participants who only completed the CFC-12 questionnaire), included participants were older, had a higher proportion of female, a higher proportion of individuals with university education and a lower BMI (P < 0·0001).

Relationships between CFC and individual characteristics were described with Pearson correlations for continuous variables and linear regressions for categorical variables. Multivariable multiple mediator analyses were performed using structural equation modelling to test the mediation of food choice motives on the relationship between CFC and dietary data. Multiple mediator analyses were used instead of several single mediator analyses because of correlations between food choice motives(Reference VanderWeele and Vansteelandt27).

Indirect effects were computed as products of overall effects of CFC on food choice motives by overall effects of food choice motives on dietary data. The estimation of mediation effects was based on the significance of the indirect effects with 99·9 % CI to take into account multiple testing(Reference Fairchild and McDaniel28). CI were based on bias-corrected bootstrapping (n 10 000). Effect size for mediation was calculated as a ratio of the indirect effect to the total effect, multiplied by 100 (only when the total effect was significant). Positive effect sizes were interpreted as mediation and negative effect sizes were interpreted as suppression (or inconsistent mediation). Suppression effect appears in a mediation model when direct and indirect effects have opposite signs(Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood29). While it is assumed that a mediation effect reduces the relationship between the independent and dependent variables (by partially explaining this relationship), suppression effect increases the magnitude of this relationship.

All models were fit with the maximum likelihood estimator and were adjusted on age, sex, education level, occupational status, monthly income per household unit and energy intake (except when energy intake was the outcome). Analyses were not stratified on sex since the interactions between CFC and gender were non-significant considering total effects (P > 0·05 for all food choice motives, P > 0·05 for most food groups) (except fruits and vegetables, meat, poultry and processed meat, whole-grain foods, confectionery and alcoholic beverages). Since frequencies of missing data on confounders were very low (< 0·2 %) (Table 1), complete-case analyses were carried out. Statistical analyses were performed using the lavaan package version 0.5-22(Reference Rosseel30) (R software).

Table 1 Individual characteristics of the 27 396 included participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

CFC, consideration of future consequences, score ranges from 1 to 5.

* P-value based on linear regressions for categorical variables or Fisher’s z transformation for continuous variables.

Mean ± sd, all such value.

Pearson correlations (95 % CI), all such values.

§ Monthly income represents the household income per month calculated per consumption unit (CU). The number of people of the household was converted into a number of CU according to a weighting system: one CU is attributed for the first adult in the household, 0·5 for other persons aged 14 or older and 0·3 for children under 14.

Results

Individual characteristics of the sample

Table 1 shows crude relationships between individual characteristics of the participants and CFC. CFC was negatively correlated with age (P < 0·001), and CFC scores were higher in male (P < 0·001), in participants with a higher level of education (P < 0·001), in participants with a managerial or intellectual profession or in students (P < 0·001) and in participants with a higher monthly income (P < 0·001).

Food choice motives, diet quality, energy intake and food groups consumption

Descriptive characteristics of food choice motives, diet quality, energy intake and food groups consumption are presented in Table 2. Highest scores regarding food choice motives were found for taste, health, absence of contaminants, local and traditional production and price, whereas lowest scores were found for ethics and environment, and innovation. Avoidance for environmental reasons and convenience showed intermediary scores.

Table 2 Descriptive characteristics of food choice motives, diet quality, energy intake and food group consumption in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

* Food choice motives score range from 0 (no concern) to 10 (strong concern).

mPNNS-GS, modified French National Nutrition and Health Program Guideline Score, ranging from 0 (low quality) to 13.5 (high quality).

Association between consideration of future consequences and food choice motives

Table 3 presents the results of the association between the CFC and the nine food choice motives with adjustments on confounders. CFC was significantly associated with all dimensions (P < 0·001). Positive associations were the highest with avoidance for environmental reasons, absence of contaminants and health, while they were more limited with local and traditional production, ethics and environment and taste. Negative associations were the highest with convenience and innovation and more limited with price.

Table 3 Association between consideration of future consequences and food choice motives in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

CFC, consideration of future consequences, score ranges from 1 to 5.

* P-value based on linear regressions adjusted for gender, age, education level, occupational status, and monthly income per household unit and energy intake.

Association between consideration of future consequences, diet quality and food groups consumption

Table 4 presents the results of the associations between CFC, diet quality (β (99 % CI) = 0·26 (0·21–0·31)) and food groups consumption. Considering total effects, CFC was positively associated with diet quality and with the consumption of fruits and vegetables, starchy foods, whole-grain foods, legumes, sugars and confectionery and non-alcoholic beverages, while negatively associated with the consumption of meat and poultry, processed meat, milk-based desserts, sugary and fatty foods, fast food, appetisers and alcoholic beverages. Overall, always considering total effects, CFC was not associated with energy intake and consumption of seafood, eggs, dairy products and fats. When considering the direct effects, the associations were relatively similar. Some differences were observed for the association between CFC and the consumption of dairy products and fats, which were significant (positive and negative association, respectively) while the associations between CFC and the consumptions of milk-based desserts, sugary and fatty foods, appetisers and non-alcoholic beverages were non-significant.

Table 4 Mediation of the association between consideration of future consequences, diet quality and food group consumption by the nine food choice motives in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

Significant effects are shown in bold (P < 0·001).

* mPNNS-GS, modified French National Nutrition and Health Program Guideline Score, analysis on 23 591 participants.

β-coefficient (all such values): All estimates correspond to a 1-point increase of CFC (continuous variable ranging from 1 to 5) and are adjusted for gender, age, education level, occupational status, monthly income per household unit and energy intake (except when energy intake was the outcome).

Percentage mediated (all such values): percentage mediated is the ratio of indirect effect on total effect and is not calculated when the total effect is non-significant. Positive percentages indicate mediation effect, whereas negative percentages indicate suppression effect (indirect and total effects have opposite signs).

Mediation effects of food choice motives on the association between consideration of future consequences, diet quality and food groups consumption

Table 4 also presents the results of the mediation effects of food choice motives on the relationship between CFC, diet quality and food groups consumption (indirect effect). All food choice motives either mediated or suppressed (inconsistent mediation) the positive relationship between CFC and diet quality, apart from ethics and environment, and convenience. Health was the mediator with the strongest effect (44·2% of the relationship between CFC and diet quality was mediated by health, compared with less than 10% for the other mediators/suppressors). Overall, higher concerns for motivations such as health (from 8·4 % to 32·6 %), ethics and environment (from 2·4 % to 18·5 %) and avoidance for environmental reasons (from 13·7 % to 22·1 %), as well as lower concerns for innovation (from 7·3 % to 25·1 %) were consistent mediators of the relationship between CFC and food group consumption. Taste appeared to be a consistent suppressor of this relationship. The mediation or suppression effects of other food choice motives were more inconsistent across food groups. Absence of contaminants was the motivation with the least significant indirect effects.

Discussion

In the present study, CFC was associated with better nutritional quality of the diet, reflected by higher intakes of food groups such as fruits and vegetables, whole-grain foods and legumes, and lower intakes of food groups such as alcoholic beverages, meat and poultry and processed meat. Overall, the main mediators of these relationships were higher concerns for health and the environment (avoidance for environmental reasons and ethics and environment), as well as lower concerns for innovation, whereas taste was the main suppressor.

Consideration of future consequences and socio-economic and demographic data

Our findings that younger people were more future-oriented are consistent with previous findings. Older people have an increased risk of death, which may result in a shorter time horizon(Reference Adams and White31). Our results also support previous data relating lower socio-economic status and higher immediate orientation, which can be due to perception of scarce control over future outcomes which is in fact a contextually appropriate response to the environment rather than a lack of willpower(Reference Peters, Joireman and Ridgway32). We also showed greater CFC scores in male compared with female. Contradictory results on sex have been reported in the literature, either with no association(Reference Orbell and Hagger33) or with male having higher CFC scores than female(Reference Petrocelli34).

Consideration of future consequences and food choice motives

Our results showed that individuals with a high consideration for the future were more concerned about the environment including avoidance for environmental reasons, and ethics and environment dimensions when purchasing food. They also had higher concerns for their health, as reflected by higher association with health and absence of contaminants dimensions. These future-oriented participants were also more interested in local and traditional production and, to a lesser extent, with taste compared with individuals with a lower CFC. Future-oriented individuals were less interested in convenience, innovation and price of the food products. In agreement with these observations, CFC was found associated with attitudes towards conservation and protection of the environment and the utilisation of natural resources(Reference Milfont, Wilson and Diniz35). CFC also showed associations with ethical decision-making, such as displaying stronger moral reasoning(Reference Hafer36) and more ethical negotiations strategies(Reference Hershfield, Cohen and Thompson37). In addition, CFC has been positively associated with healthier food choice(Reference Tórtora and Ares9) and consumers’ valuation for healthy labels or claims(Reference De Marchi, Caputo and Nayga38) suggesting a greater interest for health motives, while specifically considering future health effects has been shown to improve the nutritional quality of choices and the likelihood of using nutrition information(Reference Gustavson39). All these behaviours are characteristics of long-term goals(Reference Joireman and King4). CFC was also found associated with financial decision-making such as higher level of savings(Reference Loibl, Grinstein-Weiss and Zhan40). The negative association between CFC and price observed in the present study could be the result of a lack of perceived intertemporal dilemma regarding food financial decisions. Our results suggest that future-oriented individuals would tend to make food choices according to potential future outcomes of their behaviour, whether this behaviour leads to more self-centred outcomes (e.g. oriented towards better health) or more altruistic outcomes (e.g. oriented towards the environment).

Consideration of future consequences, diet quality and food groups consumption

The present analysis showed that future-oriented individuals had a healthier diet overall compared with less future-oriented individuals. More specifically, higher CFC was associated with higher intakes of healthy food groups such as fruits and vegetables, starchy foods, whole-grain foods and legumes and negatively associated with unhealthy food groups such as meat and poultry, processed meat, milk-based desserts, sugary and fatty foods, fast food, appetisers and alcoholic beverages. However, CFC was also positively associated with some unhealthy food groups such as sugars and confectionery, and non-alcoholic beverages. Other studies have supported healthier behaviour in individuals with higher CFC, as they were more likely to report self-perceived healthy eating behaviours(Reference Daugherty and Brase5Reference van Beek, Handgraaf and Antonides8,Reference Milfont, Wilson and Diniz35) , less likely to intend to consume fast food(Reference Dunn, Mohr and Wilson10) and less likely to drink alcohol(Reference Strathman, Gleicher and Boninger3,Reference Daugherty and Brase5) compared with individuals with lower CFC. Studies based on relatively small samples of individuals showed no association between CFC and intake of fruit and vegetables or saturated fat(Reference Mullan, Allom and Brogan13). Interestingly, while a study showed no association between CFC and self-perceived healthy eating, a significant association was observed when a food-specific version of the CFC was considered(Reference Dassen, Houben and Jansen41). Finally, another study suggested that both CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate were significant predictor of consumption frequency of functional foods, that is, food product enriched with minerals, vitamins, fatty acids or proteins for health enhancement or disease prevention(Reference Nystrand, Olsen and Tudoran42). Exploring the mediating role of food choice motives allows us to propose explanations for these relationships between CFC and better overall diet quality.

Mediating role of food choice motives

Health motive was the main mediator of the associations between CFC and intakes of fruits and vegetables, processed meat, whole-grain foods, sugary and fatty foods, fast food, appetisers and alcoholic beverages, which suggests that individuals with higher CFC are more likely to attempt complying with nutritional health guidelines than individuals with lower CFC. In France, nutritional guidelines recommended at the time of the study a greater consumption of fruit and vegetables, whole-grain foods and a lower consumption of milk-based desserts (high in sugar), sugary and fatty foods, alcohol, processed meat, fast food, and appetisers(Reference Hercberg, Chat-Yung and Chauliac43).

Environmental concerns (avoidance for environmental reasons and ethics and environment) were also significant mediators of the association between CFC and consumptions of several food groups such as seafood, meat and poultry and processed meat. Reduction of meat, poultry and processed meat consumption would lead to lower nitrogen emissions(Reference Westhoek, Lesschen and Rood44) and lower greenhouse emissions(Reference Westhoek, Lesschen and Rood44) which is likely to encourage individuals with greater future orientation to decrease their intake of such products. Vegetarianism is characterised by no or low consumption of these foods for reasons related to animals’ health and ethical treatment, as well as the environmental protection(Reference Fox and Ward45). Finally, environmental motivations are linked to higher organic food consumptions(Reference Baudry, Péneau and Allès14), which were previously found to be associated with CFC(Reference Bénard, Baudry and Méjean46).

Innovation in food products was another consistent mediator. Since CFC is negatively associated with innovation, these results suggest that the lack of interest of future-oriented individuals towards innovation, which can be seen as a weak response to food marketing, contributes extensively to the differences in intakes of these food groups. Moreover, taste, which can be considered an immediate reward of food intake, was a consistent suppressor although the effect sizes were small. Motivation for taste suppressed higher diet quality, as well as higher intakes of fruits and vegetables, starchy foods, whole-grain foods, legumes and lower intakes of meat and poultry, processed meat, milk-based desserts, sugary and fatty foods, fast food and alcoholic beverages of individuals with higher CFC. These results support previous data suggesting that higher taste preferences are linked with a poorer diet quality(Reference Kourouniotis, Keast and Riddell15). Finally, weak mediations effects were found for price, which is consistent with the weak relationship observed between CFC and price in our sample.

Several motivations were involved simultaneously in most of the associations, showing a complex relationship between all these factors and arguing for better consideration in future works. For example, in the case of seafood, a negative indirect effect was found for environmental concerns while a positive indirect effect was found for health concerns. These indirect effects with opposite signs could potentially explain the non-significant total and direct effects between CFC and seafood, suggesting potential dilemmas in individuals regarding food choices, as previously reported(Reference Péneau, Fassier and Allès47). Conflicting health and environmental recommendations exist for some food groups, as a higher nutritional quality is not always associated with a lower environmental impact(Reference Perignon, Vieux and Soler48).

Strengths and limitations

The main strength of this study is the use of three 24-h dietary records twice a year over a 4-year period, which allowed us to have a good representation of our participants’ food consumption. However, this method also has limitations such as being time consuming, showing desirability bias (survey-related dietary changes) and reported food consumption not always representative(Reference Shim, Oh and Kim49). Another strength is the large study sample size with individuals of various socio-demographic characteristics and nutritional status, which allows the use of multiple covariates to adjust for confounding factors. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that other important confounders were not considered, either for the exposure–outcome relationship, the exposure–treatment relationship or the mediator–outcome relationship. The multiple mediator analysis allowed us to differentiate individual indirect effects from the multiple mediators, but the structural equation modelling approach cannot deal with interaction effects effectively. Another limitation of our study is its cross-sectional design with lack of temporal ordering in the mediation model, which, with other limitations, does not allow us to infer valid causal interpretations. Our study could also present a selection bias because of the method used to recruit participants, which was based on volunteering. Consequently, our participants were more often female and had a higher education, higher income and professional status(Reference Andreeva, Salanave and Castetbon50) and may have higher health awareness than the global population. Therefore, cautious is needed when extrapolating our result to the general population. Other strengths of the present study consist in the validated questionnaire used. The CFC-12 questionnaire has been widely used with health and environmental outcomes, while the food choice motive questionnaire includes motives rarely described in the literature in particular: avoidance for environmental reasons, local and traditional production and innovation dimensions.

Conclusions

Our results showed that CFC was associated with healthier dietary intake such as higher intakes of fruits and vegetables, whole-grain foods, legumes and lower intakes of alcohol, meat and poultry and processed foods. Major mediators of these relationships were a greater motivation of future-oriented participants for both self-centred and altruistic outcomes, including health and environment, but also a lower motivation for innovation. Intervention studies are needed to understand how individual CFC level can be influenced and whether increasing awareness of future benefits could lead to healthier food choices and dietary behaviours.

Acknowledgements

We thank Younes Esseddik, Thi Hong Van Duong, Frédéric Coffinieres, Régis Gatibelza, Maithyly Sivapalan and Paul Flanzy (computer scientists); Nathalie Arnault, Véronique Gourlet, Dr. Fabien Szabo, Julien Allegre and Laurent Bourhis (data-manager/biostatisticians); Cédric Agaesse and Anne-Elise Dussolier (dieticians) for their technical contribution to the NutriNet-Santé study and Nathalie Druesne-Pecollo (operational coordination). We thank all the volunteers of the NutriNet-Santé cohort.

Financial support

The NutriNet-Santé Study was supported by the French Ministry of Health (DGS), the Santé Publique France agency, the French National Institute for Health and Medical Research (INSERM), the French National Institute for Agricultural Research (INRAE), the National Conservatory for Arts and Crafts (CNAM), the Medical Research Foundation (FRM) and the University of Paris 13. This research was part of the FOODPOL project, which was supported by the French National Institute for Agricultural Research (Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique) in the context of the 2013–2017 Metaprogramme ‘Diet impacts and determinants: Interactions and Transitions’.

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Authorship

M.B. conducted the literature review and drafted the manuscript. M.B. performed analyses. M.B., M.R., C.M., B.A., E.K.-G., P.P.-G., F.B., F.E., G.R., S.H. and S.P. were involved in interpreting results and critically reviewed the manuscript. C.M., E.K.-G., S.H. and S.P. were responsible for developing the design and protocol of the study. M.B., M.R, C.M., B.A., E.K.-G., P.P-G., F.B., F.E., G.R., S.H. and S.P. report no conflict of interest.

Ethics of human subject participation

This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures were approved by the International Research Board of the French Institute for Health and Medical Research (IRB Inserm n° 0000388FWA00005831) and the Commission Nationale de l’Informatique et des Libertés (CNIL n° 908450 and n° 909216). Electronic informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was registered at clinicaltrial.org (Clinical Trial no. NCT03335644).

References

Tilman, D & Clark, M (2014) Global diets link environmental sustainability and human health. Nat 515, 518522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, R, Raats, M (2006) The Psychology of Food Choice. Frontiers in Nutritional Science, NO.3. Wallingford, Oxfordshire: CABI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strathman, A, Gleicher, F, Boninger, DS et al. (1994) The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 66, 742752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joireman, J & King, S (2016) Individual differences in the consideration of future and (more) immediate consequences: a review and directions for future research. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 10, 313326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, JR & Brase, GL (2010) Taking time to be healthy: predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Pers Individ Dif 48, 202207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joireman, J, Shaffer, MJ, Balliet, D et al. (2012) Promotion orientation explains why future-oriented people exercise and eat healthy evidence from the two-factor consideration of future consequences-14 scale. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 38, 12721287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orbell, S & Hagger, M (2006) Temporal framing and the decision to take part in type 2 diabetes screening: effects of individual differences in consideration of future consequences on persuasion. Health Psychol 25, 537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Beek, J, Handgraaf, MJJ & Antonides, G (2017) Time orientation and construal level: effects on eating and exercising behaviour and preferences. Int J Consum Stud 41, 5460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tórtora, G & Ares, G (2018) Influence of time orientation on food choice: case study with cookie labels. Food Res Int 106, 706711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, KI, Mohr, P, Wilson, CJ et al. (2011) Determinants of fast-food consumption. An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Appetite 57, 349357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bénard, M, Bellisle, F, Etilé, F et al. (2018) Impulsivity and consideration of future consequences as moderators of the association between emotional eating and body weight status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 15, 84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orji, R, Vassileva, J & Mandryk, R (2012) Towards an effective health interventions design: an extension of the health belief model. Online J Public Health Inform 4, ojphi.v4i3.4321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullan, B, Allom, V, Brogan, A et al. (2014) Self-regulation and the intention behaviour gap. Exploring dietary behaviours in university students. Appetite 73, 714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baudry, J, Péneau, S, Allès, B et al. (2017) Food choice motives when purchasing in organic and conventional consumer clusters: focus on sustainable concerns (the Nutrinet-Santé cohort study). Nutrients 9, 88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kourouniotis, S, Keast, RSJ, Riddell, LJ et al. (2016) The importance of taste on dietary choice, behaviour and intake in a group of young adults. Appetite 103, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allès, B, Péneau, S, Kesse-Guyot, E et al. (2017) Food choice motives including sustainability during purchasing are associated with a healthy dietary pattern in French adults. Nutr J 16, 58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hercberg, S, Castetbon, K, Czernichow, S et al. (2010) The Nutrinet-Santé Study: a web-based prospective study on the relationship between nutrition and health and determinants of dietary patterns and nutritional status. BMC Public Health 10, 242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demarque, C, Apostolidis, T, Chagnard, A et al. (2010) Adaptation et validation française de l’échelle de perspective temporelle «consideration of future consequences»(CFC). Bull Psychol 63, 351360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Moullec, N, Deheeger, M, Preziosi, P et al. (1996) Validation of the photo manual used for the collection of dietary data in the SU. VI. MAX. study. Cah Nutr Diet 31, 158164.Google Scholar
Sautron, V, Péneau, S, Camilleri, GM et al. (2015) Validity of a questionnaire measuring motives for choosing foods including sustainable concerns. Appetite 87, 9097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Touvier, M, Kesse-Guyot, E, Méjean, C et al. (2011) Comparison between an interactive web-based self-administered 24 h dietary record and an interview by a dietitian for large-scale epidemiological studies. Br J Nutr 105, 10551064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nutrinet-Santé, E (2013) Table de composition des aliments de l’étude Nutrinet-Santé. Paris: Econ.Google Scholar
Black, AE (2000) Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake: basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations. Int J Obes 24, 1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estaquio, C, Kesse-Guyot, E, Deschamps, V et al. (2009) Adherence to the French programme national nutrition sante guideline score is associated with better nutrient intake and nutritional status. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 10311041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schofield, WN (1984) Predicting basal metabolic rate, new standards and review of previous work. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 39, 541.Google Scholar
INSEE (Institut national de la statistique et des études économiques) [National Institute of Statistics and Economic Studies] (2015) Unités de consommation [consumption units]. Available at http://www.insee.fr/en/methodes/default.asp?page=definitions/unite-consommation.htm (accessed January 2016).Google Scholar
VanderWeele, T & Vansteelandt, S (2014) Mediation analysis with multiple mediators. Epidemiol Methods 2, 95115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, AJ & McDaniel, HL (2017) Best (but oft-forgotten) practices: mediation analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 105, 12591271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, DP, Krull, JL & Lockwood, CM (2000) Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci 1, 173181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y (2012) lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw 48, 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, J & White, M (2009) Time perspective in socioeconomic inequalities in smoking and body mass index. Health Psychol 28, 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, BR, Joireman, J & Ridgway, RL (2005) Individual differences in the consideration of future consequences scale correlate with sleep habits, sleep quality, and GPA in university students. Psychol Rep 96, 817824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orbell, S & Hagger, M (2006) Temporal framing and the decision to take part in type 2 diabetes screenin: effects of individual differences in consideration of future consequences on persuasion. Health Psychol 25, 537548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrocelli, JV (2003) Factor validation of the consideration of future consequences scale: evidence for a short version. J Social Psychol 143, 405413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milfont, TL, Wilson, J & Diniz, P (2012) Time perspective and environmental engagement: a meta analysis. Int J Psychol 47, 325334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafer, CL (2000) Investment in long-term goals and commitment to just means drive the need to believe in a just world. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 26, 10591073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hershfield, HE, Cohen, TR & Thompson, L (2012) Short horizons and tempting situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making and behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 117, 298310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Marchi, E, Caputo, V, Nayga, RM et al. (2016) Time preferences and food choices: evidence from a choice experiment. Food Policy 62, 99109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gustavson, CR (2022) Active consideration of future health can be prompted by simple health messages and improves nutritional quality of food choices. Front Nutr 9, 926643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loibl, C, Grinstein-Weiss, M, Zhan, M et al. (2010) More than a penny saved: long-term changes in behavior among savings program participants. J Consum Aff 44, 98126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dassen, FC, Houben, K & Jansen, A (2015) Time orientation and eating behavior: unhealthy eaters consider immediate consequences, while healthy eaters focus on future health. Appetite 91, 1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nystrand, BT, Olsen, SO & Tudoran, AA (2021) Individual differences in functional food consumption: the role of time perspective and the big five personality traits. Appetite 156, 104979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hercberg, S, Chat-Yung, S & Chauliac, M (2008) The French national nutrition and health program: 2001–2006–2010. Int J Public Health 53, 6877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westhoek, H, Lesschen, JP, Rood, T et al. (2014) Food choices, health and environment: effects of cutting Europe’s meat and dairy intake. Glob Environ Change 26, 196205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N & Ward, K (2008) Health, ethics and environment: a qualitative study of vegetarian motivations. Appetite 50, 422429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bénard, M, Baudry, J, Méjean, C et al. (2018) Association between time perspective and organic food consumption in a large sample of adults. Nutr J 17, 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Péneau, S, Fassier, P, Allès, B et al. (2017) Dilemma between health and environmental motives when purchasing animal food products: sociodemographic and nutritional characteristics of consumers. BMC Public Health 17, 876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perignon, M, Vieux, F, Soler, L-G et al. (2017) Improving diet sustainability through evolution of food choices: review of epidemiological studies on the environmental impact of diets. Nutr Rev 75, 217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shim, J-S, Oh, K & Kim, HC (2014) Dietary assessment methods in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiol Health 36, e2014009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreeva, VA, Salanave, B, Castetbon, K et al. (2015) Comparison of the sociodemographic characteristics of the large NutriNet-Santé e-cohort with French census data: the issue of volunteer bias revisited. J Epidemiol Community Health 69, 893898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Individual characteristics of the 27 396 included participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

Figure 1

Table 2 Descriptive characteristics of food choice motives, diet quality, energy intake and food group consumption in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

Figure 2

Table 3 Association between consideration of future consequences and food choice motives in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)

Figure 3

Table 4 Mediation of the association between consideration of future consequences, diet quality and food group consumption by the nine food choice motives in 27 396 participants (NutriNet-Santé study, 2014)