No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Constructing contempt
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 October 2017
Abstract
Gervais & Fessler argue that contempt is a natural kind and that its experience cannot be explained by a constructionist account of emotion. We dispute these claims and offer a positive constructionist model of contempt that accounts for the existing evidence and unifies conflicting findings in the literature on contempt.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Alvarado, N. & Jameson, K. A. (1996) New findings on the contempt expression. Cognition and Emotion
10:379–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006a) Are emotions natural kinds?
Perspectives on Psychological Science
1(1):28–58.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2009) Variety is the spice of life: A psychological construction approach to understanding variability in emotion. Cognition and Emotion
23:1284–306.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2013) Psychological construction: The Darwinian approach to the science of emotion. Emotion Review
5:379–89.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. & Satpute, A. B. (2013) Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology
23(3):361–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012.Google Scholar
Cameron, C. D., Lindquist, K. A. & Gray, K. (2015) A constructionist review of morality and emotions: No evidence for specific links between moral content and discrete emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review
19:371–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clore, G. L. & Ortony, A. (2013) Psychological construction in the OCC model of emotion. Emotion Review
5:335–43.Google Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A. & Stillman, P. E. (2013) Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review
5:344–55.Google Scholar
Cushman, F. & Young, L. (2011) Patterns of moral judgment derive from nonmoral psychological representations. Cognitive Science
35:1052–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. & Cordaro, D. (2011) What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review
3:364–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridhandler, B. M. & Averill, J. R. (1982) Temporal dimensions of anger: An exploration of time and emotion. In: Anger and aggression, ed. Averill, J. R., pp. 253–80. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Lindquist, K. A., Barsalou, L. & Barrett, L. F. (2012) Emotion words shape emotion percepts. Emotion
12:314–25.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. & Graham, J. (2016) Paradise lost: How moral psychology would contract if reduced to harm. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Diego, CA, January 2016.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. & Keltner, D. (1999) Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more faces and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion
13(3):225–66.Google Scholar
Heuer, K., Lange, W. G., Isaac, L., Rinck, M. & Becker, E. S. (2010) Morphed emotional faces: Emotion detection and misinterpretation in social anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
41:418–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (2011) Forms and functions of emotions: Matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emotion Review
3:371–78.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. & Haynes, O. M. (1988) On the form and universality of the contempt expression: A challenge to Ekman and Friesen's claim of discovery. Motivation and Emotion
12(1):1–16.Google Scholar
Kreibig, S. D. (2010) Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology
84:394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A. (2013) Emotions emerge from more basic psychological ingredients: A modern psychological constructionist model. Emotion Review
5(4):356–68.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A. & Barrett, L. F. (2008) Emotional complexity. In: Handbook of emotions, ed. Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F., pp. 513–30. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A. & Barrett, L. F. (2012) A functional architecture of the human brain: Emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
16:533–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A., Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E. & Russell, J. A. (2006) Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion
6:125–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A. & Gendron, M. (2013) What's in a word? Language constructs emotion perception. Emotion Review
5:66–71.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Gendron, M., Barrett, L. F. & Dickerson, B. C. (2014) Emotion perception, but not affect perception, is impaired with semantic memory loss. Emotion
14:375–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., MacCormack, J. K. & Shablack, H. (2015a) The role of language in emotion: Predictions from psychological constructionism. Frontiers in Psychology
6: article 444. (Online journal).Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B. & Gendron, M. (2015b) Does language do more than communicate emotion?
Current Directions in Psychological Science
24:99–108.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E. & Barrett, L. F. (2012) The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
35:121–43.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012) Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: The label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology
3: article 54. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054. (Online journal).Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. & Ekman, P. (2004) The relationship among expressions, labels, and descriptions of contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
87(4):529–40. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.529.Google Scholar
Mauss, I. B. & Robinson, M. D. (2009) Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion
23:209–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mill, J. S. (1884) A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive: Being a connected view of the principles of evidence and the methods of scientific investigation. Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011) The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives?
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
35:1791–804.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, E. L. & Ekman, P. (1995) Conceptual and methodological issues in the judgment of facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion
19(2):111–38.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S. & Haidt, J. (1999) The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
76(4):574–86.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991d) The contempt expression and the relativity thesis. Motivation and Emotion
15:149–68.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review
110(1):145–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Suzuki, N. & Ishida, N. (1993) Canadian, Greek, and Japanese freely produced emotion labels for facial expressions. Motivation and Emotion
17:337–51.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A. & Greene, J. D. (2010) Moral judgments recruit domain-general valuation mechanisms to integrate representations of probability and magnitude. Neuron
67:667–77.Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M. & Kousta, S. (2009) Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition
1:219–47.Google Scholar
Vytal, K. & Hamann, S. (2010) Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
22:2864–85.Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., Kang, J., Johnson, T. D., Nichols, T. E., Satpute, A. B. & Barrett, L. F. (2015) A Bayesian model of category-specific emotional brain responses. PLoS Computational Biology
11(4):e1004066. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004066.Google Scholar
Wagner, H. L. (2000) The accessibility of the term ‘contempt’ and the meaning of the unilateral lip curl. Cognition and Emotion
14(5):689–710.Google Scholar
Target article
On the deep structure of social affect: Attitudes, emotions, sentiments, and the case of “contempt”
Related commentaries (26)
A sentimental education: The place of sentiments in personality and social psychology
Above and below the surface: Genetic and cultural factors in the development of values
Affect in social media: The role of audience and the presence of contempt in cyberbullying
Are sentiments subject to selection pressures? The case of oxytocin
Attitude–Scenario–Emotion (ASE) sentiments are superficial
Building a house of sentiment on sand: Epistemological issues with contempt
Constructing contempt
Contempt as the absence of appraisal, not recognition, respect
Contempt – Where the modularity of the mind meets the modularity of the brain?
Contempt, like any other social affect, can be an emotion as well as a sentiment
Deep mechanisms of social affect – Plastic parental brain mechanisms for sensitivity versus contempt
Dominance as a competence domain, and the evolutionary origins of respect and contempt
From disgust to contempt-speech: The nature of contempt on the map of prejudicial emotions
Further implications in analyzing contempt in modern society
How dare you not recognize the role of my contempt? Insight from experimental psychopathology
Including pride and its group-based, relational, and contextual features in theories of contempt
Is humility a sentiment?
On the substantial contribution of “contempt” as a folk affect concept to the history of the European popular institution of charivari
Oxytocin shapes the priorities and neural representations of attitudes and values
Prejudice is a general evaluation, not a specific emotion
Sentiments and the motivational psychology of parental care
Socioecological factors are linked to changes in prevalence of contempt over time
Two kinds of respect for two kinds of contempt: Why contempt can be both a sentiment and an emotion
Warmth and competence as distinct dimensions of value in social emotions
Warmth, competence, and closeness may provide more empirically grounded starts for a theory of sentiments
We need more precise, quantitative models of sentiments
Author response
Seeing the elephant: Parsimony, functionalism, and the emergent design of contempt and other sentiments