Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T17:22:58.927Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

IQ and mental disorder in young men

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Erik Lykke Mortensen*
Affiliation:
Department of Health Psychology, Institute of Public Health, University of Copenhagen
Holger Jelling Sørensen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry (Amager), Copenhagen University Hospital, Copenhagen
Hans Henrik Jensen
Affiliation:
Department of Health Psychology, Institute of Public Health, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
June Machover Reinisch
Affiliation:
Kinsey Institute for Research in Sex, Gender and Reproduction, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana
Sarnoff A. Mednick
Affiliation:
Social Science Research Institute and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
*
Erik Lykke Mortensen, Department of Health Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5A, PO Box 2099, 1014 Copenhagen K, Denmark. Tel: +45 35327839; fax: +45 3532 7748; e-mail: e.l.mortensen@pubhealth.ku.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Most research investigating the relationship between IQ and risk of mental disorder has focused on schizophrenia.

Aims

To illuminate the relationship between IQ test scores in early adulthood and various mental disorders.

Method

For 3289 men from the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort, military IQ test scores and information on psychiatric hospitalisation were available. We identified 350 men in the Danish Psychiatric Central Register, and compared the mean IQ test scores of nine diagnostic categories with the mean scores of 2939 unregistered cohort controls.

Results

Schizophrenia and related disorders, other psychotic disorders, adjustment, personality, alcohol and substance-use-related disorders were significantly associated with low IQ scores, but this association remained significant for the four non-psychotic disorders only when adjusting for comorbid diagnoses. For most diagnostic categories, test scores were positively associated with the length of the interval between testing and first admission. ICD mood disorders as well as neuroses and related disorders were not significantly associated with low IQ scores.

Conclusions

Low IQ may be a consequence of mental disease or a causal factor in psychotic and non-psychotic disorders.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Longitudinal research shows that many mental disorders do not arise de novo in adult life but have their roots in childhood (Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994; Reference Caspi, Moffit and NewmanCaspi et al, 1996; Reference Cannon, Walsh and HollisCannon et al, 2001). Most researchers investigating the relationship between cognitive development and risk of mental disorder have concentrated their efforts on schizophrenia. Thus, Swedish and Israeli conscript studies have consistently supported the existence of a robust relationship between low IQ test scores and risk of schizophrenia (Reference David, Malmberg and BrandtDavid et al, 1997; Reference Davidson, Reichenberg and RabinowitzDavidson et al, 1999; Reference Gunnell, Harrison and RasmussenGunnell et al, 2002). Comparatively few studies have examined possible relationships between IQ and other psychiatric disorders, although associations between low scores on IQ tests and risk of non-schizophrenic psychotic disorders (Reference David, Malmberg and BrandtDavid et al, 1997) as well as risk of severe depression (Reference Zammit, Allebeck and DavidZammit et al, 2004) have also been observed in the Swedish conscript study. Perhaps more remarkably, a recent cross-sectional study has provided evidence that most diagnostic categories are associated with low IQ scores in male adolescents (Reference Weiser, Reichenberg and RabinowitzWeiser et al, 2004). Hence, there is a rationale for conducting longitudinal studies examining whether low IQ test scores are specifically associated with schizophrenia or are a marker of increased vulnerability to the development of a broader range of psychiatric disorders. We conducted a prospective study spanning about 20 years and linking data on IQ in early adulthood with the risk of hospitalisation for a broad range of psychiatric disorders.

METHOD

The Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort

The Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort consists of 9125 individuals born during the period October 1959 to December 1961 at the maternity department of the Copenhagen University Hospital, Rigshospitalet. A total of 8400 infants survived the first month after birth. When the cohort was established, demographic, socio-economic, prenatal and postnatal data were recorded prospectively during pregnancy, at delivery and at a 1-year examination (Reference Zachau-Christiansen and RossZachau-Christiansen & Ross, 1975). Parental social status was recorded at the 1-year examination using a combined scale, rated from 1 to 8, based on breadwinner's occupation, breadwinner's education, type of income (wage/salary) and quality of housing.

The Danish Psychiatric Central Register

The Danish Psychiatric Central Register has been computerised since 1 April 1969 (Reference Munk-Jørgensen and MortensenMunk-Jørgensen & Mortensen, 1997). It contains data on all admissions to Danish psychiatric in-patient facilities. Until 1994, diagnoses were coded according to ICD–8 (World Health Organization, 1974). Since 1994 diagnoses have been coded according to ICD–10 (World Health Organization, 1992).

Draft records

With the exception of individuals with disqualifying diseases (such as epilepsy and diabetes) and individuals who volunteer for military service, all Danish men are required to appear before the draft board when they become liable for conscription at the age of 18 years. Appearing before the draft board primarily involves a medical assessment, but also includes IQ testing. Draft board information including IQ test scores has been collected for all male members of the cohort by the Prenatal Development Project, a large-scale study of the effects of prenatal and perinatal factors on human development (Reference Reinisch, Mortensen and SandersReinisch et al, 1993).

The IQ test used by the Danish military draft board is the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP). It is a 45 min group test with four sub-tests (letter matrices, verbal analogies, number series and geometric figures) and a total score ranging from 0 to 78. The total score correlates 0.82 with the Full Scale IQ of Wechsler's Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Reference WechslerWechsler, 1958), indicating that the BPP is a high-quality measure of general IQ (Reference Mortensen, Reinisch and TeasdaleMortensen et al, 1989).

Current sample

The Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort originally included 4668 males, but only 4280 survived the first 4 weeks of life. Individual identification numbers were introduced in Denmark in 1968 and it has been possible to obtain valid numbers for 4116 males in the cohort (164 individuals were lost to study because of childhood mortality or emigration). Data from the Danish Psychiatric Register for these 4116 cohort members were obtained on 31 August 1999. However, BPP scores were only available for about 77% of the 4280 males who survived the first 4 weeks of life, and the current sample comprises 3289 men who appeared before the draft board at the mean age of 19.2 years (s.d.=1.3, range 16.4–26.2).

Diagnostic classification

Of the 3289 men included in our study, 350 (10.6%) were identified in the Danish Psychiatric Central Register. In order to obtain diagnostic categories of reasonable size for analysis, ICD–8 and ICD–10 categories were combined to form the following nine diagnostic categories:

  1. (a) schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorders: 295.x diagnoses according to ICD–8 and F20.x or F21.x diagnoses according to ICD–10;

  2. (b) other psychotic disorders: 297.x and 298.x diagnoses and F20–F29 diagnoses (except F20.x and F21.x);

  3. (c) mood disorders: 296.x diagnoses and F30–F39 diagnoses;

  4. (d) neuroses and related disorders: 300.x diagnoses and F40.x–F42.x and F44.x–F49.x;

  5. (e) adjustment disorders: 307.x diagnoses and F43.x diagnoses;

  6. (f) personality disorders: 301.x diagnoses and F60.x–F61.x or F69.x diagnoses;

  7. (g) alcohol-related disorders: 303.x diagnoses and F10.x diagnoses;

  8. (h) other substance use disorders: 304.x diagnoses and F11.x–F19.x diagnoses;

  9. (i) other diagnoses: diagnoses not belonging in any of the above eight categories. The majority of the patients in this category had diagnoses that might also reflect adjustment disorder (for example, 39 patients had ICD–8 code 793 diagnosis).

Data analysis

The unregistered control sample comprised 2939 individuals. The observed control sample BPP mean score was 40.4, which is close to the 40.9 mean predicted from a regression formula describing secular trends in BPP scores (Reference Teasdale and OwenTeasdale & Owen, 1989), and the standard deviation was 11.3, which is also close to the 11.4 observed for individuals born in 1954–1958 (the average year of birth of the control sample was 1960.8). In addition, the distribution of test scores was close to the normal distribution, and we decided to linearly transform BPP scores to a standardised scale with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 in the control sample, corresponding to the most frequently used IQ scales (Reference WechslerWechsler, 1958).

Using t-tests for independent samples, the mean transformed BPP score of each diagnostic category was compared with the mean of all unregistered control individuals. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to adjust the BPP scores for parental social status and parental registration in the Psychiatric Register. Many patients were registered in several of the main diagnostic categories, and therefore t-tests and ANCOVA analyses of the diagnostic categories are based on partly overlapping patient samples. Consequently, we also conducted a multiple regression analysis with BPP score as the dependent variable and the nine diagnostic categories as independent variables.

In addition, the approximate length of time between appearing before the draft board and the date of first admission was calculated for each individual (the exact date of appearing before the draft board was not available, but the difference between the estimated and the exact date did not exceed 3 months). According to this interval, the individuals in each diagnostic category were classified into three subgroups: individuals who had already been admitted to an in-patient facility when they appeared before the draft board or who were admitted within the subsequent year; individuals who were admitted 1–5 years after the draft board examination; and individuals who were admitted more than 5 years after appearing before the draft board. Using ANOVA, the mean BPP scores in the three subgroups were compared. In addition to overall F-tests, contrasts were computed to evaluate differences between the three subgroup means (Reference Rosenthal, Rosnov and RubinRosenthal et al, 2000). Finally, those in the first of the three subgroups were excluded from the sample, and the multiple regression analysis with BPP score as the dependent variable and the nine diagnostic categories as independent variables was repeated on this reduced sample.

RESULTS

Age at hospital admission and diagnosis

Table 1 shows the numbers in each diagnostic category. Relatively few individuals had been given a diagnosis of mood disorder or a diagnosis of neuroses and related disorders. On the other hand, diagnoses such as adjustment disorders, personality disorders and alcohol-related disorders were common in this all-male sample. It is important to emphasise that the figures reflect hospital admissions only and do not necessarily indicate the ‘true’ cumulative incidence of the disorders in the sample. Furthermore, although the relatively few cases of hospital admission with a diagnosis of mood disorder might to some extent be the result of diagnostic conservatism, it is more likely to be a consequence of the fact that this is an all-male sample.

Table 1 Psychiatric hospitalisation characteristics of members of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort (values are means)

Diagnostic category n Age when first admitted (years) Interval to first admission (years) Age when first in a diagnostic category (years) Interval to first time in a diagnostic category (years) Total number of admissions n Total number of diagnoses n
All in register 350 26.0 7.0 4.4 1.3
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders 50 24.7 5.8 28.9 10.0 15.4 2.7
Other psychotic disorders 42 25.4 6.4 27.7 8.7 9.9 2.7
Mood disorders 15 28.5 9.8 31.4 12.7 8.1 2.1
Neuroses and related disorders 21 26.6 7.7 29.2 10.4 9.1 2.2
Adjustment disorders 72 27.8 8.9 29.1 10.2 4.5 2.0
Personality disorders 105 23.4 4.7 24.5 5.6 7.4 2.0
Alcohol-related disorders 103 27.4 8.5 28.9 10.0 6.9 2.2
Other substance use disorders 54 24.4 5.5 27.8 8.9 12.0 2.8
Other diagnoses 71 21.7 2.7 1.01

Diagnoses of schizophrenia, personality disorder and substance use disorders seem to be associated with first hospital admission at a relatively early age. Mood disorders, adjustment disorders and alcohol-related disorders, on the other hand, are associated with older age at first admission. A corresponding pattern was observed for the mean intervals between appearing before the draft board and the time of first admission. The mean age at first admission was particularly low for the other diagnoses category, and it is remarkable that this category almost exclusively consisted of patients who had only been admitted once.

Number of admissions and co-diagnoses

Diagnoses of schizophrenia and of substance use disorder were both associated with relatively many admissions. Adjustment disorder, on the other hand, was associated with relatively few admissions (Table 1). For most diagnostic categories the average cohort member was registered in one additional category, but the average individual with the diagnosis of schizophrenia, other psychotic disorder or substance use disorder was registered in two additional categories. Table 2 shows the two most frequently diagnosed comorbid conditions. Chi-squared tests showed that diagnoses of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorder were positively associated (P<0.0001) and that both these diagnoses were positively associated with substance use disorder (P<0.0001 and P<0.001 respectively). Adjustment disorder was negatively associated with schizophrenia (P<0.05) and with personality disorder (P<0.03), whereas personality disorder and substance use disorder showed a positive association (P<0.02).

Table 2 Most frequent and next most frequent comorbid conditions diagnosed in the sample

Diagnostic category Most frequent co-diagnosis (%) Next most frequent co-diagnosis (%)
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders Other psychotic disorder (46) Substance use disorder (38)
Other psychotic disorders Schizophrenia (55) Substance use disorder (33)
Mood disorders Other psychotic disorder (20) Adjustment disorder (20)
Alcohol-related disorder (20)
Neuroses and related disorders Alcohol-related disorder (38) Personality disorder (24)
Adjustment disorders Alcohol-related disorder (26) Personality disorder (19)
Substance use disorder (19)
Personality disorders Alcohol-related disorder (33) Substance use disorder (23)
Alcohol-related disorders Personality disorder (33) Substance use disorder (19)
Substance use disorders Personality disorder (44) Alcohol-related disorder (37)
Other diagnoses Not applicable

Comparison with controls

Table 3 shows the BPP mean scores for the control group and the nine diagnostic categories. Registration in the Psychiatric Register was associated with significantly lower BPP scores for all diagnostic categories except mood disorders and neuroses and related disorders. The differences between the control group mean and the means of most diagnostic categories were substantial, when compared with the size of the control group standard deviation. Among the six diagnostic categories significantly associated with low BPP score, the highest mean was for the category other psychotic disorder and the lowest mean was for the substance use disorder category. Table 3 shows that the mean of the category other psychotic disorder corresponds to a BPP mean of 92.4 and that the mean of the substance use category corresponds to a BPP mean of 86.5. Thus, in terms of standard deviation units, the mean of the former category was 0.5 s.d. below the mean of the controls, whereas the mean of the latter category was 0.9 s.d. below the control mean (7.6/15 and 13.5/15 respectively).

Table 3 Scores on the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) IQ test and parental social status in the control sample and in diagnostic categories

Diagnostic category BPP score Parental social status
Mean (95% CI) s.d. P 1 Mean (95% CI) s.d. P 1
Control group 100.0 (99.5-100.5) 15.0 4.1 (4.0-4.2) 1.9
All in register 90.8 (89.2-92.4) 15.0 0.0001 3.2 (3.0-3.4) 1.5 <0.0001
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders 92.2 (87.7-96.7) 16.2 0.0003 3.1 (2.7-3.5) 1.4 0.0005
Other psychotic disorders 92.4 (87.7-97.1) 15.6 0.0011 3.3 (2.8-3.8) 1.5 0.0109
Mood disorders 97.1 (88.1-106.1) 17.7 0.46 3.7 (2.8-4.6) 1.7 0.40
Neuroses and related disorders 96.6 (91.8-101.4) 11.2 0.30 3.8 (3.0-4.6) 1.5 0.49
Adjustment disorders 89.5 (86.1-92.9) 14.7 <0.0001 3.2 (2.9-3.5) 1.4 0.0001
Personality disorders 90.2 (87.3-93.1) 15.4 <0.0001 3.1 (2.8-3.4) 1.6 <0.0001
Alcohol-related disorders 87.4 (84.4-90.4) 15.4 <0.0001 3.2 (2.9-3.5) 1.4 <0.0001
Substance use disorders 86.5 (82.6-90.4) 14.7 <0.0001 3.1 (2.7-3.5) 1.4 0.0002
Other diagnoses 89.7 (86.3-93.1) 14.5 <0.0001 3.2 (2.8-3.6) 1.6 0.0006

Parental social status and psychiatric history

Table 3 also shows that the pattern of mean parental social status scores corresponds to the pattern of BPP scores. Thus, registration in the Psychiatric Register was associated with significantly lower parental social status, except for mood disorders and for neuroses and related disorders. Table 4 shows that adjusting BPP scores for parental social status or for both parental social status and parental registration in the Psychiatric Register did not change the levels of significance substantially. It is noteworthy, however, that the adjusted regression coefficients for the categories of schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders and of other psychotic disorders indicated significantly lower BPP scores than for the control group only at the 5% level of significance.

Table 4 Regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample, adjusted for parental social status and for parental registration in the Danish Psychiatric Register

Diagnostic category Adjustment for parental social status Adjustment for parental social status and registration
Coefficient (95% CI) P 1 Coefficient (95% CI) P 1
All in register -6.8 (-8.3 to -5.2) <0.0001 -6.5 (-8.1 to -4.9) <0.0001
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders -5.0 (-8.8 to -1.1) 0.0122 -4.5 (-8.4 to -0.6) 0.0234
Other psychotic disorders -5.4 (-9.6 to -1.1) 0.0130 -5.0 (-9.2 to -0.8) 0.0203
Mood disorders -1.6 (-8.7 to 5.4) 0.65 -1.1 (-8.1 to 5.9) 0.76
Neuroses and related disorders -2.5 (-8.4 to 3.4) 0.41 -2.3 (-8.2 to 3.6) 0.44
Adjustment disorders -7.7 (-10.9 to -4.4) <0.0001 -7.4 (-10.6 to -4.1) <0.0001
Personality disorders -7.1 (-9.8 to -4.3) <0.0001 -6.7 (-9.4 to -4.0) <0.0001
Alcohol-related disorders -10.1 (-12.8 to -7.4) <0.0001 -9.9 (-12.6 to -7.1) <0.0001
Substance use disorders -10.5 (-14.3 to -6.8) <0.0001 -10.1 (-13.8 to -6.3) <0.0001
Other diagnoses -7.8 (-11.0 to -4.5) <0.0001 -7.5 (-10.8 to -4.3) <0.0001

Comorbidity

Table 5 shows the results of analysing the data of all cohort members in the study together and using indicator variables to code registration in diagnostic categories. Four diagnostic categories were not significantly associated with low BPP scores: schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders, other psychotic disorders, mood disorders, and neuroses and related disorders. The association with low BPP scores remained significant for five other diagnostic categories: adjustment disorders, personality disorders, alcohol-related disorders, substance use disorders and other diagnoses). Table 2 shows substantial overlap between the schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders categories, but none of these diagnoses showed significant associations with BPP scores when the other category was excluded from the regression model (P=0.89 and P=0.64, respectively). The categories alcohol-related disorders and (in particular) substance use disorders were frequent co-diagnoses of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. Since Tables 3 and 4 show that alcohol and substance use disorders were associated with very low BPP scores, we conducted a regression analysis without indicator variables for these diagnoses. This analysis also showed no significant association between BPP scores and schizophrenia or other psychotic disorders (P=0.26 and P=0.36, respectively).

Table 5 Regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample adjusted for registration in other diagnostic categories

Diagnostic category Coefficient (95% CI) P 1
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders 0.2 (-4.9 to 5.3) 0.94
Other psychotic disorders -1.2 (-6.6 to 4.1) 0.65
Mood disorders 2.1 (-5.6 to 9.8) 0.59
Neuroses and related disorders 3.3 (-3.3 to 9.8) 0.33
Adjustment disorders -6.0 (-9.7 to -2.3) 0.0013
Personality disorders -4.6 (-7.8 to -1.4) 0.0048
Alcohol-related disorders -8.8 (-12.0 to -5.6) <0.0001
Substance use disorders -6.5 (-11.1 to -2.0) 0.0052
Other diagnoses -10.2 (-13.7 to -6.7) <0.0001

Interval between testing and first admission

Table 6 shows the results of classifying all 350 individuals recorded in the Psychiatric Register into three subgroups according to the interval between appearing before the draft board and the time of first admission. One-way ANOVA showed significant overall F-test only for the categories of schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders, adjustment disorders and substance use disorders. For these categories, contrasts showed that the mean of the >5 year subgroup was significantly higher than the means of the subgroups with intervals of <1 year and 1–5 years. The table shows similar trends for the categories of other psychotic disorders and personality disorders, whereas the inconsistent results for mood disorders and neuroses and related disorders most probably reflect small subgroup sample sizes and low power.

Table 6 Scores on the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) IQ test for subgroups classified according to the interval between testing and first admission

Diagnostic category Interval < 1 year Interval 1-5 years Interval > 5 years P 1
n Mean (95% CI) n Mean (95% CI) n Mean (95% CI)
All registered patients 75 88.2 (84.9-91.5) 88 90.3 (87.2-93.4) 187 92.1 (89.9-94.3) 0.15
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders 11 85.9 (75.4-96.4) 15 87.6 (79.8-95.4) 24 98.02 (92.2-103.8) 0.0478
Other psychotic disorders 11 88.9 (77.9-99.9) 9 88.1 (75.4-100.8) 22 95.8 (90.8-100.8) 0.33
Mood disorders 2 90.2 (72.0-108.4) 4 111.8 (105.5-118.1) 9 92.1 (79.6-104.6) 0.15
Neuroses and related disorders 4 93.2 (82.6-103.8) 5 101.6 (94.2-109.0) 12 95.6 (88.6-102.6) 0.50
Adjustment disorders 12 82.4 (74.7-90.1) 12 84.1 (77.9-90.3) 48 92.62 (88.4-96.8) 0.0340
Personality disorders 36 88.1 (83.0-93.2) 25 87.8 (82.1-93.5) 44 93.3 (88.7-97.9) 0.22
Alcohol-related disorders 14 84.4 (76.8-92.0) 22 86.1 (79.5-92.7) 67 88.4 (84.7-92.1) 0.62
Substance use disorders 12 81.2 (73.1-89.3) 15 80.2 (73.9-86.5) 27 92.42 (87.1-97.7) 0.0109
Other diagnoses 12 86.4 (81.1-91.7) 15 91.4 (86.9-95.9) 27 91.2 (81.5-100.9) 0.40

Differences between the <1 year and the 1–5 year subgroups were insignificant and for most categories small. The results of excluding the 75 men in the 51 year subgroup and adjusting for comorbid diagnoses are presented in Table 7. The adjusted regression coefficients in this table do not deviate substantially from the coefficients in Table 5, and the higher P-values in Table 7 seem to reflect loss of power due to smaller group sizes.

Table 7 Reduced sample: regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample adjusted for registration in other diagnostic categories

Diagnostic category Coefficient (95% CI) P 1
Schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders 0.9 (-4.7 to 6.5) 0.74
Other psychotic disorders -1.7 (-7.8 to 4.4) 0.58
Mood disorders 1.1 (-7.1 to 9.4) 0.79
Neuroses and related disorders 2.0 (-5.2 to 9.2) 0.59
Adjustment disorders -5.1 (-9.1 to -1.1) 0.0129
Personality disorders -3.4 (-7.3 to 0.5) 0.08
Alcohol-related disorders -9.3 (-12.7 to -5.9) <0.0001
Substance use disorders -6.4 (-11.5 to -1.4) 0.0123
Other diagnoses -8.6 (-12.9 to -4.2) 0.0001

DISCUSSION

Seven diagnostic categories were associated with low premorbid BPP test scores, but this was not the case for the categories of mood disorders and neuroses and related disorders. The seven categories showed substantial cognitive deficits, ranging from 7.6 to 13.5 on a traditional IQ scale and corresponding to 0.5–0.9 of the BPP standard deviation of the unregistered control sample. The same seven diagnostic categories were also associated with relatively low parental social status, but neither parental social status nor parental psychiatric hospitalisation explains the relatively low BPP scores.

When comorbid diagnoses were included as covariates, the adjusted regression coefficients for five categories (adjustment disorders, personality disorders, alcohol-related disorders, substance use disorders and other diagnoses) indicated significantly lower mean BPP scores than the mean of the unregistered controls, whereas this was not the case for schizophrenia and schizotypal disorders and other psychotic disorders. Except for mood disorders, neuroses and related disorders and other diagnoses, BPP scores were higher – although not necessarily significantly so – for individuals first admitted more than 5 years after BPP testing. Differences in BPP scores between those admitted within 1 year and those admitted 1–5 years after testing were small and inconsistent, and an analysis excluding those admitted within 1 year essentially confirmed the results for the full sample.

It should be kept in mind that these observations refer to associations between the possibly unreliable hospital diagnoses in the Danish Psychiatric Register and BPP performance. However, noise introduced by unreliable diagnoses may weaken rather than strengthen associations between diagnostic categories and BPP scores. In addition, although unreliable diagnoses and misclassification of patients might affect results for individual diagnostic categories (e.g. personality disorders), it is unlikely that such factors explain the overall picture of associations between several broad diagnostic categories and BPP score. Strong evidence that low IQ test scores are not only associated with psychiatric hospitalisation and hospital diagnoses is provided by a recent cross-sectional study of associations between cognitive test scores and psychopathological screening diagnoses in male adolescents appearing before the Israeli draft board. In that study, mean cognitive test scores were significantly poorer for almost all diagnostic groups than for controls, and observed effect sizes were in the range 0.3–1.6 (Reference Weiser, Reichenberg and RabinowitzWeiser et al, 2004).

Explaining the association

Several mechanisms may explain the association between IQ and mental disease. An aetiological hypothesis assumes that low IQ has a direct or indirect role in the aetiology of a mental disease; thus, it is possible that low IQ plays a part in the development of risk drinking (Reference Windle and BlaneWindle & Blane, 1989; Reference Mortensen, Jensen and SandersMortensen et al, 2005) or antisocial personality traits (Reference Loney, Whaley-Klahn, Kosier, Van Dusen and MednickLoney et al, 1983; Reference Kandel, Mednick and Kirkegaard-SorensenKandel et al, 1988). For schizophrenia, it might be assumed that low IQ mediates the effects of adverse prenatal factors and birth complications, although there is evidence that IQ may be an independent risk factor (Reference Gunnell, Harrison and RasmussenGunnell et al, 2002). Second, common genetic and environmental factors may affect both the development of mental disease and cognitive development: for example, obstetrical complications might affect both the risk of schizophrenia and cognitive development (Reference BoogBoog, 2004). Thus, we observed that parental social status was associated with both offspring IQ and mental disease – but we also observed that neither parental social status nor genetic (family) disposition to mental disease could explain the link between IQ and disease (parental psychiatric admission is obviously a general and nonspecific indicator of genetic disposition).

A third possibility is that mental disease affects IQ test scores, which might occur in several ways. A developmental hypothesis assumes that personality traits associated with the risk of developing a mental disease adversely affect cognitive development. Thus, deviant personality traits in people who go on to develop schizophrenia may be associated with less optimal cognitive development (Reference Malmberg, Lewis and DavidMalmberg et al, 1998). A test performance hypothesis assumes that premorbid traits or early symptoms affect motivation and test performance when young men appear before the draft board. Thus, some personality disorders might be associated with low motivation, and taking the BPP under the influence of alcohol or other substances might lead to low test scores – although in our sample this seems an unlikely explanation of the association between BPP scores and alcohol-related and other substance use disorders, because a medical examination is part of appearing before the draft board, and because of the average 10-year and 8.9-year intervals between taking the BPP and the first admissions with these diagnoses (see Table 1). Finally, there is a ‘dementia’ hypothesis, according to which cognitive impairment may be the direct result of mental diseases such as alcoholism or schizophrenia. Because of the young age of the individuals in our study, it is doubtful that such a mechanism explains the association between alcoholism and low BPP scores, but it could have a role in very early cases of schizophrenia.

A fourth possibility is that IQ is primarily associated with the risk of hospitalisation, or the likelihood of obtaining a certain diagnosis when hospitalised. Thus, one study has reported that IQ is an independent predictor of lifetime psychiatric contact (Reference Walker, McConville and HunterWalker et al, 2002), and it is likely that IQ is a strong determinant of the ability to cope with crises and life events and that this ability affects the risk of admission to a psychiatric department. This may partly explain the association between adjustment disorders and low IQ and the similar results for the other diagnoses category (typically admitted only once to psychiatric treatment). A link between IQ and risk of admission may also have a role in personality disorders, but it is also possible that low IQ affects the risk of receiving a diagnosis of personality disorder or alcoholism relatively early (i.e. patients with a higher IQ may be able to hide their antisocial behaviour or alcohol problems). However, a recent follow-up study of a non-hospitalised subsample from the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort showed an association between low IQ and self-reported risk drinking, suggesting that the association between low IQ and alcohol-related disorder does not only reflect a link between IQ and risk of hospitalisation with an alcohol diagnosis (Reference Mortensen, Jensen and SandersMortensen et al, 2005).

Our results indicate significant premorbid cognitive deficits in several psychiatric diagnostic categories, and it is likely that many – if not all – of the mechanisms described above contribute to the low premorbid IQ observed for most diagnostic categories. Several of these mechanisms (the aetiological hypothesis, common genetic and environmental factors and the developmental hypothesis) may explain low IQ in neurodevelopmental disorders (Reference BradshawBradshaw, 2001). However, low BPP scores were observed for both psychotic disorders (schizophrenia, and the category of other psychotic disorders, which included paranoid psychosis) and for non-psychotic disorders (adjustment disorders, personality disorders, alcohol-related disorders and other substance use disorders). In interpreting the lack of evidence for low premorbid IQ in mood disorder (affective illness) and neuroses and related disorders, the small number of individuals in these categories should be borne in mind. Thus, although we were able to confirm low IQ in those later developing schizophrenia, our results suggest that low premorbid IQ is not specific for schizophrenia and related disorders, and may indeed show a stronger association with adjustment disorders, personality disorders, and alcohol-related and substance use disorders. The cohort members were 38–40 years old when the follow-up was completed, thus ensuring that the vast majority of incident cases with schizophrenia would have been identified. The in-patient register diagnoses of schizophrenia can be considered to be reliable in Denmark (Reference Munk-JergensenMunk-Jørgensen, 1995).

Interval between testing and first admission

In this register-based study the first admission to a psychiatric department was the earliest available indicator of mental disorder. In general, the longer the interval between IQ testing and first admission, the greater the likelihood that a low IQ score is not an effect of mental disorder. Table 1 shows that for all diagnostic categories the average individual was admitted several years after appearing before the draft board, and Table 6 shows that the majority of patients were admitted more than 5 years after BPP testing. For most diagnostic categories these patients obtained higher BPP mean scores than those admitted sooner, but perhaps it is more remarkable that differences between those admitted within 1 year and those admitted 1–5 years after testing were small and inconsistent. This pattern suggests that previous and existing mental disorders do not have substantial negative effects on BPP scores. For schizophrenia and related disorders, the low BPP mean for those admitted within 5 years of testing may reflect the effects of prodromal symptoms on test performance (Reference Rabinowitz, Reichenberg and WeiserRabinowitz et al, 2000), but perhaps more probably a complex development with premorbid traits leading to low cognitive ability, which in turn leads to vulnerability and early psychosis. For schizophrenia as well as adjustment disorders and substance use disorders, the differences between those admitted within 5 years and those admitted later may also reflect links between cognitive ability, coping strategies and risk of hospitalisation. In this perspective it is, however, remarkable that the difference between early and late admission is relatively small for alcohol-related disorders, suggesting that the association between low IQ and alcohol-related disorders is not entirely explained by a link between low IQ and the likelihood of hospitalisation and early alcohol diagnosis.

Comorbidity

Attempts to interpret our results should take into account the facts that individual diagnoses often change over the life span (e.g. from personality disorder to schizophrenia) and that admission to a psychiatric department often results in several discharge diagnoses. Thus, there is evidence from the Swedish conscript study (Reference Lewis, David and MalmbergLewis et al, 2000) that nearly 40% of men who developed schizophrenia would have received a psychiatric diagnosis of a non-psychotic disorder at the age of 18 years. In our study sample, 46% of the men with schizophrenia were also registered in the category of other psychotic disorders, and 38% were registered in the category of other substance use disorders. On the other hand, alcohol-related disorders and personality disorders tended to cluster together (about 33% diagnostic overlap). When we adjusted for all diagnostic categories (Tables 5 and 7), the regression coefficients indicated particularly low BPP scores for alcohol-related disorders and other diagnoses (followed by substance use and adjustment disorders). We believe that adjusting for co-diagnoses is more realistic than constructing a diagnostic hierarchy and assigning each patient to only one diagnostic category (this procedure has to be based on strong assumptions about relationships among mental disorders, and for individual patients these assumptions cannot be verified in a register-based study). However, the results of adjusting for co-diagnoses may be ambiguous, since some diagnostic categories may typically be associated with more co-diagnoses than other categories and there must be much overlapping variance between some of the categories. Our analyses suggested that collinearity with alcohol and substance use disorders or other psychotic disorders did not explain the results for schizophrenia. When interpreting the lack of significance for schizophrenia in Tables 5 and 7, it should also be borne in mind that the unadjusted mean in Table 3 confirms other studies that have not adjusted for co-diagnoses and observed an association between low IQ scores and schizophrenia. The unadjusted mean describes the observed IQ of patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, whereas the adjusted regression coefficients reflect both the pattern of associations among the diagnostic categories and between each diagnostic category and IQ.

Implications of our findings

We have demonstrated that most broad diagnostic categories of mental disorder are associated with relatively low premorbid IQ. Both low IQ and mental disorder may be considered to be indicators of suboptimal brain development, but many mechanisms may contribute to explain the association between IQ and mental disorder. The range of mental disorders and the observed effect sizes should be considered. If our results can be generalised, it is unlikely that any personality trait or other psychological characteristic – perhaps except the broad personality dimension of ‘neuroticism’ – has a stronger association with mental disorder than IQ. This has important implications for both research and clinical practice. Scores on IQ tests correlate with many neuropsychological tests of specific cognitive functions and with many other psychological and social variables (Reference Gottfredson, Sternberg and GrigorenkoGottfredson, 2002). Consequently, the association with IQ should be considered in any theoretical account of the development of the relevant disorders, and – if possible – premorbid IQ should be controlled for in studies comparing patient samples (e.g. people with schizophrenia or alcoholism) and normal controls. In clinical practice, the relatively high frequency of low premorbid IQ in many diagnostic categories should also be taken into account when choosing appropriate therapy and when interpreting the cognitive performance of individual patients (e.g. neuropsychological test scores in cases of long-term alcoholism). It should, however, not be forgotten that patients show large individual differences in IQ scores and that a substantial number of patients in all diagnostic categories are of normal or above-normal IQ (in our study sample about 28% of the 350 patients obtained BPP scores corresponding to an IQ of 100 or higher). Finally, our study describes associations between diagnostic categories and scores on a Danish IQ test, and to evaluate whether our findings can be generalised, similar studies in other countries and cultures are needed.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The mean performance of psychiatric patients on IQ tests is relatively low and evaluation of cognitive function should be an integrated aspect of thorough clinical assessment.

  2. Low premorbid IQ is associated not only with schizophrenia and related disorders, but also with several broad diagnostic categories, including alcohol and substance misuse.

  3. In individual patients poor cognitive function does not necessarily reflect effects of mental disorders (e.g. alcohol misuse), but may rather reflect low premorbid IQ.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The study was register-based; only patients admitted to hospital were included and the diagnostic categories were based on hospital diagnoses.

  2. The study investigated an all-male sample; it is not known to what extent the results may be generalised to female psychiatric patients.

  3. For diagnostic categories such as mood disorders and neuroses and related disorders, sample sizes were too small to permit definitive conclusions about the potential association with IQ.

Acknowledgements

This study has been supported by United States Public Health Service (USPHS) grants HD17655 and HD20263 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to J.M.R.; USPHS grant DA05056 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse to J.M.R.; Sygekassernes Helsefond (Health Insurance Foundation) grant to H.J.S.; grant 9700093 from the Danish Research Councils as well as grant 1400/2-4-1997 from the Danish National Board of Health to E.L.M.

Thanks are due to Vibeke Munk, MA, for help with the manuscript and critical comments.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Boog, G. (2004) Obstetrical complications and subsequent schizophrenia in adolescent and young adult offsprings: is there a relationship? European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 114, 130136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, J. L. (2001) Developmental Disorders of the Frontostriatal System. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cannon, M., Walsh, E., Hollis, C., et al (2001) Predictors of later schizophrenia and affective psychosis among attendees at a child psychiatry department. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 420426.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Moffit, T. E., Newman, D. L., et al (1996) Behavioural observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 10331039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, A. S., Malmberg, A., Brandt, L., et al (1997) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 27, 13111323.Google Scholar
Davidson, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., et al (1999) Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 13281335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottfredson, L. S. (2002) g: Highly general and highly practical. In The General Factor of Intelligence – How General Is It? (eds Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L.), pp. 331380. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gunnell, D., Harrison, G., Rasmussen, F., et al (2002) Associations between premorbid intellectual performance, early life exposures and early-onset schizophrenia: cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 298305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 13981402.Google Scholar
Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., et al (1988) IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 224226.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., David, A. S., Malmberg, A., et al (2000) Non-psychotic psychiatric disorder and subsequent risk of schizophrenia: cohort study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 416420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loney, J., Whaley-Klahn, M. A., Kosier, T., et al (1983) Hyperactive boys and their brothers at 21: predictors of aggressive and antisocial outcomes. In Prospective Studies of Crime and Delinquency (eds Van Dusen, K. T. & Mednick, S. A.). Boston, MA: Kluwer–Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Malmberg, A., Lewis, G., David, A., et al (1998) Premorbid adjustment and personality in people with schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 308313.Google Scholar
Mortensen, E. L., Reinisch, J. M., Teasdale, T. W. (1989) Intelligence as measured by the WAIS and a military draft board group test. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 30, 315318.Google Scholar
Mortensen, E. L., Jensen, H. H., Sanders, S. A., et al (2005) Associations between volume of alcohol consumption and social status, intelligence and personality in a sample of young adult Danes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Munk-Jergensen, P. (1995) Faldende Førstegangsindlseggelsesrater for Skizofreni i Danmark 1970–1991. Århus: Århus University.Google Scholar
Munk-Jørgensen, P. & Mortensen, P. B. (1997) The Danish Psychiatric Central Register. Danish Medical Bulletin, 44, 8284.Google Scholar
Rabinowitz, J., Reichenberg, A., Weiser, M., et al (2000) Cognitive and behavioural functioning in men with schizophrenia both before and shortly after first admission to hospital: cross-sectional analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 2632.Google Scholar
Reinisch, J. M., Mortensen, E. L. & Sanders, S. A. (1993) The prenatal development project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 370, 5461.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., Rosnov, R. L. & Rubin, D. B. (2000) Contrast and Effect Sizes in Behavioural Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Teasdale, T. W. & Owen, D. R. (1989) Continuing secular increases in intelligence and astable prevalence of high intelligence levels. Intelligence, 13, 255262.Google Scholar
Walker, N. P., McConville, P. M., Hunter, D., et al (2002) Childhood mental ability and lifetime psychiatric contact. A 66 year follow-up study of the 1932 Scottish Mental Ability Survey. Intelligence, 30, 233245.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1958) The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Weiser, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., et al (2004) Cognitive performance of male adolescents is lower than controls across psychiatric disorders: a population-based study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110, 471475.Google Scholar
Windle, M. & Blane, H. T. (1989) Cognitive ability and drinking behavior in a national sample of young adults. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 13, 4348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (1974) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD–8). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD–10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Zachau-Christiansen, B. & Ross, E. M. (1975) Babies: Human Development During the First Year. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Zammit, S., Allebeck, P., David, A. S., et al (2004) A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 354360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Psychiatric hospitalisation characteristics of members of the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort (values are means)

Figure 1

Table 2 Most frequent and next most frequent comorbid conditions diagnosed in the sample

Figure 2

Table 3 Scores on the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) IQ test and parental social status in the control sample and in diagnostic categories

Figure 3

Table 4 Regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample, adjusted for parental social status and for parental registration in the Danish Psychiatric Register

Figure 4

Table 5 Regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample adjusted for registration in other diagnostic categories

Figure 5

Table 6 Scores on the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP) IQ test for subgroups classified according to the interval between testing and first admission

Figure 6

Table 7 Reduced sample: regression coefficients coding the difference between each diagnostic category and the control sample adjusted for registration in other diagnostic categories

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.