Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T04:25:54.860Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of antidepressive therapy on retinal contrast processing in depressive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Emanuel Bubl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Albert-Ludwigs-University of Freiburg
Dieter Ebert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Albert-Ludwigs-University of Freiburg
Elena Kern
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Albert-Ludwigs-University of Freiburg
Ludger Tebartz van Elst*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Albert-Ludwigs-University of Freiburg
Michael Bach
Affiliation:
University Eye Hospital, Albert-Ludwigs-University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
*
Ludger Tebartz van Elst, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Albert-Ludwigs-Universität, Hauptstr. 5, 79104 Freiburg, Germany. Email: tebartzvanelst@uniklinik-freiburg.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Recently, we reported a reduced retinal contrast gain in unmedicated and medicated patients with major depression.

Aims

To analyse whether the contrast gain normalises after successful antidepressive therapy by recording the pattern electroretinogram (PERG) in healthy controls and patients with depression before and after antidepressive therapy.

Method

Fourteen patients diagnosed with major depression were repeatedly scanned and the results compared with that from 40 matched controls.

Results

The retinal contrast gain was lower at baseline in patients with depression, was normalised with remission and correlated with the severity of depression. Patients who did not achieve remission retained significantly lower contrast gain at follow-up.

Conclusions

The study provides evidence for a state-dependent modulation of retinal contrast gain in patients with major depression. Reduced contrast gain normalised after therapy. A PERG-based contrast gain could serve as a state marker of depression.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2012 

For the past few decades there has been an intensive search for biomarkers of psychiatric states such as schizophreniform or depressive syndromes. Currently, different neurobiological signals are being discussed as potential candidates, such as brain volume, cerebral blood flow, plasma proteins, sleep patterns and blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Reference Gong, Wu, Scarpazza, Lui, Jia, Marquand and Huang1Reference Landsness, Goldstein, Peterson, Tononi and Benca5 An objective biomarker of depression could be very helpful in distinguishing classical depressive episodes from depression in the context of other disorders such as schizophrenia or life events. It also might be helpful in objectifying the therapeutic response to medication or psychotherapy.

Electrophysiological, psychophysical and fMRI signals arising from the visual system are further candidates as biomarkers of depression. Visual contrast sensitivity and visual-evoked potential amplitudes are known to be reduced in depression. Reference Bubl, Tebartz van Elst, Gondan, Ebert and Greenlee6,Reference Normann, Schmitz, Fürmaier, Döing and Bach7 Moreover, there is evidence that antidepressant medication can modulate visually evoked potentials in healthy individuals. Reference Normann, Schmitz, Fürmaier, Döing and Bach7 In people with seasonal affective disorder, an initially diminished flash electroretinogram normalised after light therapy, Reference Lavoie, Lam, Bouchard, Sasseville, Charron and Gagné8 and mood states have a surprisingly strong influence on the visual-evoked potential. Reference Joost, Bach and Schulte-Mönting9 A recently published fMRI study described elevated visual cortical activity in sustained remitted patients with depression and a decrease in activity in those who relapsed. Reference Farb, Anderson, Bloch and Segal4

Another candidate as a biomarker for depressive states is the retinal contrast gain. Retinal contrast processing is easy to assess with an electrophysiological signal, which arises at the level of the retina in response to phase reversing stimuli (gratings or checkerboards) with varying contrasts (Fig. 1). This signal is the pattern electroretinogram (PERG), which reflects retinal ganglion cell activity and is recorded at the cornea in response to visual pattern stimulation. Reference Maffei and Fiorentini10,Reference Bach, Hoffman, Heckenlively and Arden11 The PERG can thus serve as a surrogate marker of retinal information processing from photoreceptors to the origin of the optic nerve. It is modulated by changes in the catecholaminergic system, especially the dopamine system with a reduction under dopamine depletion conditions. Reference Tebartz van Elst, Greenlee, Foley and Lucking12,Reference Ghilardi, Marx, Bodis-Wollner, Camras and Glover13 Psychopharmacological treatment with antidepressants leads to alterations in the catecholaminergic system that depicts an important pathophysiological mechanism underlining depression. Reference Hasler, Fromm, Carlson, Luckenbaugh, Waldeck and Geraci14 When stimulating rapidly (faster than about five reversals per second), a somewhat sinusoidal response is obtained in response to the phase-reversing checkerboard. The relationship between the amplitude of this curve and the contrast of the presented grating is nearly linear, i.e. the stronger the contrast, the larger the amplitude. Reference Ben-Shlomo, Bach and Ofri15 Since patients do not have to consciously respond in any way but only have to observe the phase-reversing checkerboard, this measurement is objective – a sort of ‘electrocardiogram of the eye’.

In a recent study we reported reduced retinal contrast processing as a strong marker of the depressive status in unmedicated and medicated patients. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 Figure 1 illustrates typical PERG recordings of participants with current depression and healthy controls in this previous study. The difference in contrast gain between patients and controls was remarkably strong and patients could be distinguished from controls at an individual raw-data level with high sensitivity and specificity. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 Thus, the question arises as to whether this easy-to-assess signal might serve as a clinically relevant marker of depression. In particular, the present study examined whether deficient retinal contrast gain (as measured with the PERG) normalises after successful anti-depressive therapies or if it remains abnormal even after remission of depression: is the PERG a state marker or a trait marker?

Method

The basic participant sample for this study is identical to that of our previously published baseline sample: there was no difference between these patients and the patients from the previous study, Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 which consisted of 40 patients (20 in-patients and 20 out-patients) and 40 controls. The 20 in-patients and the 20 out-patients in our previous study both presented with a significant reduction in baseline retinal contrast gain. The baseline contrast gain did not differ significantly between the in- and out-patient groups. In absolute figures the out-patient group presented with a slightly, but non-significantly, decreased contrast gain. The 20 out-patients were diagnosed but not treated in our clinic. They were referred to an out-patient psychiatrist for further treatment and were lost to follow-up.

In order to assess treatment effects on the signal of interest, we tried to reassess all 20 in-patients in the initial in-patient sample (n = 20). In total, 14 in-patients with a diagnosis of major depression and 40 matched controls (control group) without any psychiatric disease took part in this study. We were unable to reassess the remaining six patients: two were not reassessed as a result of their own decision and four were lost to follow-up. All patients received medication and were investigated at the time of admission and after therapy. The baseline data of the 40 healthy controls and the patients have been published previously. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 Here, we report the data of all available follow-up PERG studies to analyse this signal's longitudinal course.

Patients were identified from records in the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University Hospital of Freiburg following approval by the local ethics committee. The study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Patients were included after giving informed consent and were all in-patients admitted to the hospital for further therapy.

Inclusion criteria were a current diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to DSM-IV 17 criteria classified as major depressive disorder, single episode (DSM-IV: 296.2; 7 patients) or recurrent episode (DSM-IV: 296.3; 7 patients). Exclusion criteria were any other DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorder, chronic depression, any other general neurological or medical condition or any eye disease except for correctable refraction errors.

The psychiatric diagnosis was established by senior consultant psychiatrists based on a structured clinical interview following DSM-IV criteria. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs and Weiller18 In addition, all the patients and the control group were assessed psychometrically using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) Reference Hautzinger, Bailer, Worrall and Keller19 and Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) (German translation). 20 The severity of depression was categorised according to the cut-off levels in the BDI manual (German translation): mild to moderate if the BDI was between 11 and 18, and moderate to severe if it was greater than 18. Reference Hautzinger, Bailer, Worrall and Keller19 To detect any confounding effects from somatic antidepressant treatment on the target variable, we assessed the medication taken by the patients according to the unipolar antidepressant composite rating (UACR). Reference Keller21,Reference Leon, Solomon, Mueller, Endicott, Rice and Maser22

This study was done in a naturalistic setting, meaning that our measurements coincided with the normal routine treatment of in-patients. The follow-up was conducted at the time of discharge and treatment outcome was evaluated with BDI and HRSD scores. Following the general recommendation for interpretation of BDI and HRSD scores for non-symptomatic states, we defined remission a priori as a BDI of ≤ 10 and a HRSD of ≤ 7.

We set no restrictions with respect to pharmacological or psychotherapeutic therapy in the context of this study. Therefore, the 14 patients' medication varied greatly. The precise medication taken by the patients at baseline was as follows: one patient was on benzodiazepines; two patients underwent monotherapy with mirtazapine or venlafaxine; four patients took a combination of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) or venlafaxine with mirtazapine; one received a combination of a tricyclic antidepressant and an SSRI; and six were taking a complex combination of more than two psychotropic drugs. The patients' medication at the follow-up was: one patient was not taking medication; three patients underwent monotherapy with mirtazapine, an SSRI or lithium; one received a combination of a tricyclic antidepressant and venlafaxine; eight were under a complex combination of more than two psychotropic drugs; and one received a combination of an SSRI and lithium.

During the course of the study, four patients received antipsychotic medication; three of these individuals had achieved remission and one had not. Owing to the small patient numbers, we could not further elucidate any possible relationships between antipsychotic medication and contrast gain.

At baseline two people who achieved remission received risperidone; in one person the medication was discontinued and in the other person it was switched to quetiapine. Furthermore, one patient with non-remitted depression received quetiapine at baseline and at follow-up. Finally, one patient received quetiapine at follow-up.

Following UACR criteria, four patients on medication received a score of zero at baseline due to low dosage and short duration of antidepressant medication. Two patients on medication were under moderate-intensity treatment and eight patients underwent high-intensity somatic therapy. Accordingly, at the time of follow-up, one patient was on no medication, two were on moderate treatment and 11 patients received high-intensity somatic treatment (including the four patients with non-remitted depression).

The control group, identical to that of our previous paper, Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 comprised 40 age- and gender-matched healthy individuals without a history of neurological or mental disorders, all of whom scored within the normal range of the BDI and HRSD. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16

All participants had a visual acuity above 20/25 wearing appropriate correction, measured at the distance used for visual stimulation. Reference Bach23

Stimulation and recording

Stimulation, recording and analysis were performed using the EP2000 system. Reference Bach24 All details were as described in our previous article. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 Briefly, in a dimly lit room, the stimuli were generated with a resolution of 800 × 600 pixels at a frame rate of 75 Hz and displayed on a display covering a field size of 32° × 27.0° at an observation distance of 57 cm with a mean stimulus luminance of 45 cd/m Reference Chen, Wan, O'Reardon, Wang, Wang and Korczykowski2 . Our patients were refracted as necessary to the observation distance. To ensure appropriate fixation and accommodation, they reported digits that appeared in random intervals in place of the fixation target.

To assess the PERG-based contrast gain, a sequence of five checkerboard stimuli with 0.8 check size, contrast-reversing at 12 reversals per second, was presented with contrast levels of 3.2%, 7.3%, 16.2%, 36% and 80%. Each contrast level was presented for 10 s, and then the next contrast was applied, returning finally to the first contrast level. This interleaved sequence was presented until 80 artefact-free trials per contrast (1.0 s length each, containing 12 responses) were accumulated. The interleaved blocking ensured that any sequential effects (for example, fatigue) distributed equally across all contrast values. The protocol was repeated once, and further analysis was based on the vector average of each pair of recordings.

The PERG signals were recorded simultaneously from both eyes using DTL electrodes Reference Bach25 placed at the lower limbus of each eye. These were referenced to gold cup electrodes at the ipsilateral outer canthi; one earlobe was grounded. Participants were instructed to blink infrequently during recording and to maintain a relaxed pose. Sweeps exceeding ±130 μV were rejected as artefacts, and the number of artefacts per condition was saved with the PERG data. Participants reported small digits appearing randomly in place of the fixation cross to ensure correct fixation and accommodation. The test-retest reproducibility of the PERG can be very good with careful electrode placement; when quantified by the coefficient of variation this was reported to be below 10%. Reference Otto and Bach26

The potentials were amplified, filtered (first order 0.5–1 00 Hz) and digitised at 1 kHz with 16-bit resolution. To prevent temporal aliasing, all timing (stimulation, analogue sampling, sweep length) was related to the stimulus monitor frame rate. The recording's total duration was approximately 1 h per participant.

Data analysis

Offline, all traces (Fig. 1, far left column) were Fourier-analysed to calculate the magnitude spectrum (Fig. 1, right part of the left column, top: the spectrum contains responses at the reversal frequency (12 Hz) and its harmonics (24 Hz, 36 Hz), noise at all frequencies and occasional isolated mains interference at 50 Hz). From this, we extracted a noise-free response estimate. Reference Bach and Meigen27,Reference Meigen and Bach28 The 12 Hz response and its harmonic (24 Hz) were combined via their quadratic mean (root mean square value). A linear model of these spectral response magnitudes v. stimulus contrast yielded the PERG-based contrast gain defined by the slope of the linear model. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 This slope will be referred to as the ‘PERG-based contrast gain’ or ‘contrast gain’ throughout this paper and represents the average from both eyes in each participant. The entire analysis is fully automatic and does not require manual peak measurement, thus no need for masking procedures arose.

Statistical analysis

The R statistical system 29 was used by M.B. to analyse the data. Post hoc analysis was performed to test the effects of type of diagnosis and medication. Spearman rank correlations between contrast gain and the BDI and HRSD scores were calculated.

Results

Electrophysiological findings

In Fig. 1, the original PERG data from two participants are illustrated. The left column shows that the PERG amplitude rises in response to increasing contrast of the checkerboard stimulus. The middle column demonstrates less increase in PERG amplitude in response to an increase in the stimulus contrast, which is apparent at the raw-data level of an individual patient. The third column illustrates that this ‘PERG pathology’ normalises in the same patient after remission of depression.

From the original PERG data the contrast gain in μV/100% is derived (Fig. 2(a)). The group comparison revealed a highly significant difference in the contrast gain between healthy controls (left box in Fig. 2(b)) and patients with current depression at baseline (middle box in Fig. 2(b), P<0.0001, remaining significant after correcting for multiple testing). The contrast gain of the 10 patients with remitted depression (right box, Fig. 2(b)) did not differ significantly from the controls at follow-up (t-test, P = 0.35) – in fact, its median was even a little higher than the median of the controls. In total, 4 of the 14 medicated patients did not remit during the course of the study as indicated by their BDI and HRSD scores (Table 1). The PERG gain of those patients did not differ significantly from the patient group at baseline (t-test, P = 0.96). Additionally, at baseline there was no significant difference between the non-remitted and the remitted groups (P<0.16).

In summary, the visual deficit as seen in the PERG normalised with remission. These effects are apparent on an individual basis when comparing the contrast gain at baseline with follow-up (Fig. 3 and Table 2).

Duration of antidepressant therapy

The duration of antidepressant treatment was highly similar for the two depression groups (P = 0.74). For the non-remitted group, the mean treatment time was 12.3 weeks (s.d. = 5.0) in contrast to 13.5 weeks (s.d. = 6.4) in the remitted group. Because this study was done in a naturalistic setting, we did not intervene with the individual discharge schedule and ended up with a slightly longer treatment period for the remitted group. In this context four patients were discharged not fully remitted. However, there was no correlation between duration of therapy and contrast gain (rho (ρ) = 0.081, P = 0.78). We compared the contrast gain of the four individuals in the non-remitted group with four individuals in the remitted group matched for treatment duration and found again a highly significant difference in contrast gain (P<0.0003). The treatment time for the matched remitted group was 11.8 weeks (s.d. = 5.5). The same was true when we matched for age (P<0.0003). The mean age of the matched remitted group was 49.5 years (s.d. = 10.8).

A linear mixed-effect model for statistical analysis emphasised that the change in contrast gain depended on the status of the patients as remitted, not on the time of investigation. It detected a highly significant difference in contrast gain for the patients with remitted depression (P<0.001). The effect for investigation at baseline or follow-up was not significant, representing the high reproducibility of the result (P<0.79).

Medication effects

The precise medication taken by the patients at the time of study varied because of our study's naturalistic design. Since most of the 14 patients taking medication received complex antidepressive medication of more than two drugs (see Method), we were unable to identify effects of specific substance classes such as SSRI or tricyclic agents on contrast processing. Furthermore, we observed no significant correlation between intensity of somatic treatment (UACR) and contrast gain. The four individuals in the non-remitted group did not receive any medication not given in the remitted group. In addition, removing the four patients receiving antipsychotic medication did not change the results.

Contrast processing and depressive subsyndrome

The type of depression – whether recurrent or a first depressive episode – had no effect on the mean contrast gain.

Severity of depression

To analyse the effect of severity of depression on contrast gain, we compared the BDI and HRSD with the PERG-based contrast gain. The correlation between both BDI and HRSD depression scores and contrast gain across all the control group and patients with depression in the whole patient group and all visits (ρ = –0.48, P<0.0001 and ρ = –0.40, P<0.001, respectively) was highly significant. When looking only at the patient group and combined visits (visit 1 and 2), the results did not differ (ρ = –0.58, P<0.0001 and ρ = –0.55, P<0.003). The results for the BDI at the follow-up are displayed in Fig. 4.

A receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis was performed to assess the robustness of the results: the BDI limit varied from 6 to 20 (bracketing the nominal threshold score of ≤ 10) and HRSD from 1 to 20 (bracketing the nominal ≤ 7 threshold score).

Fig. 1 Pattern electroretinogram (PERG) recordings from two representative participants. Healthy control (left), a patient with depression (centre) and the same patient with remission on medication (right). These recordings illustrate a clear reduction in PERG amplitudes in an individual with depression (upper row, middle), which returns to normal levels after remission of depression (upper row right; recording of healthy individual upper row left). The stimulus contrast increases from 3.2% (bottom) to 80% (top). There are raw PERG traces for each participant on the left (the ordinate spans 8 mV, the abscissa 950 ms), and on the right, the Fourier magnitude spectra from 1 to 100 Hz. The expected response is at 12 Hz (the reversal rate). For high contrasts there is also a third harmonic response at 24 Hz, and the prominent spectral line at 50 Hz represents a mains artefact well separated in frequency space. For further analysis see Fig. 2. OD, right eye; OS, left eye.

Fig. 2 (a) Pattern electroretinogram (PERG) magnitude v. contrast for two participants (a healthy control and a patient with depression before and after remission, single eye) based on the traces in Fig. 1; (b) Group PERG contrast gain, grand average.

In (a) the symbols represent the spectral response magnitude, the oblique lines depict linear fits. The slope of the linear model is interpreted as the PERG-based contrast gain. In (b) healthy controls (left) and patients in remission (right) do not differ significantly, whereas patients with current depression present with a significantly reduced contrast gain. Boxplot details on the right: the median is indicated by the thick horizontal lines, the notches represent a 95% confidence interval for the medians, the box covers the 25–7 5% percentile range, the ‘antennas’ indicate the range, outliers are indicated by circles.

TABLE 1 Gender, age, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) and contrast gain (slope) of the study groups

Group n Gender Male/female, n Age, years Mean (s.d.) Follow-up time Weeks, mean BDI score Mean (s.d.) HRSD score Mean (s.d.) Slope, V/100% Mean (s.d.)
Patients, visit 1
    Later remitted 10 1/9 40.2 (12.8) 25.6 (7.7) 19.2 (6.8) 1.87 (0.58)
    Later not remitted 4 2/2 48.8 (9.8) 24.5 (7.94) 17.8 (3.4) 1.37 (0.59)
Patients, visit 2
    Remitted 10 1/9 40.4 (12.8) 10.3 7.1 (1.8) 4.3 (1.6) 3.32 (0.55)
    Not remitted 4 2/2 48.8 (9.8) 7.5 18.5 (9.4) 9.0 (3.9) 1.72 (0.28)
Healthy controls 40 17/23 43.3 (12.7) 2.2 (1.4) 2.0 (1.3) 3.10 (0.93)

The ROC was characterised via its area under the curve (AUC). The results can be summarised as follows: if threshold HRSD is 7 or higher, and BDI between 10 and 11, the AUC is 1.0, corresponding to 100% specificity and sensitivity. Lower scores for both parameters lead to a loss of sensitivity and a steady decline of AUC down to the chance level 0.5 for HRSD below 4; which is to be expected. For scores above the optimal region of HRSD = 7/BDI = 11 there is a loss of specificity, but AUC only goes down to 0.77 and the decline is much shallower. These calculations demonstrate that the PERG gain has a finite operating region. Specifically, when the depressive state becomes ‘too normal’, PERG gain looses sensitivity.

Discussion

Main findings

In our previous paper we reported a reduced retinal contrast gain as an objective marker of depression in humans. Reference Bubl, Kern, Ebert, Bach and Tebartz van Elst16 The goal of the present study was to investigate the potential differences in this signal in response to successful and unsuccessful therapy. Here we demonstrate a normalisation of what had initially been a reduced retinal contrast gain in patients with depressive disorder after remission from depression.

Although the sample size of our follow-up measurements is small, the results are statistically highly significant. When looking at the individual raw data as illustrated in Figs 3 and 4 it becomes clear that there was a marked improvement in contrast gain in all individuals with remitted depression. The coordinates of contrast change (Fig. 3) clearly separated those whose condition remitted from those whose did not. Figure 4 illustrates that this improvement in contrast gain was accompanied by symptomatic improvement as measured with the BDI.

Thus, the initial reduced contrast gain in the patients normalised in those who remitted, whereas the contrast gain in the non-remitted group remained decreased. The results point to an important and relevant neurophysiological signal with respect to the depressive state.

Neurophysiological interpretation of contrast gain in depression

The question arises how alterations of contrast gain at the level of the retina could be related to the pathophysiology of depression. Changes in contrast processing have also been found in Parkinson's disease. From animal and human studies there is reliable evidence that contrast processing is altered according to

Fig. 3 Relationship between contrast gain at follow-up (visit 2) and baseline (visit 1).

The patients that stayed depressed (indicated by crosses) are near the identity line, whereas those who remitted (diamonds) presented with a significant improvement after remission, and their contrast gain fell within the normal range. PERG, pattern electroretinogram.

TABLE 2 Individual patient data on gender, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) and contrast gain (slope)

T 1 (baseline) T 2 (follow-up)
Patient number Gender BDI score Mean HRSD score Mean Contrast gain slope μV/100%, mean BDI score Mean HRSD score Mean Contrast gain slope μV/100%, mean
Non-remitted group
    1 Female 28 21 2.20 32 14 2.10
    2 Male 34 18 1.26 12 5 1.70
    3 Female 19 13 1.21 18 10 1.44
    4 Male 17 19 0.80 12 7 1.61
Remitted group
    5 Female 35 33 1.59 10 4 3.24
    6 Female 12 9 1.95 6 1 3.19
    7 Female 30 21 2.86 7 5 3.40
    8 Female 18 23 1.44 4 5 3.06
    9 Female 29 18 2.47 9 7 3.25
    10 Female 28 17 1.78 7 3 2.54
    11 Female 23 13 1.35 8 4 3.64
    12 Female 20 18 2.17 8 5 2.95
    13 Female 36 25 2.22 5 5 4.66
    14 Male 25 15 0.89 7 4 3.31

dopaminergic status. Reference Tebartz van Elst, Greenlee, Foley and Lucking12,Reference Ghilardi, Marx, Bodis-Wollner, Camras and Glover13,Reference Bodis-Wollner and Yahr30Reference Langheinrich, van Elst, Lagrèze, Bach, Lücking and Greenlee33 The dopaminergic amacrine cells of the retina are known to be crucially involved in modulating retinal contrast processing. Reference Iuvone, Galli, Garrison-Gund and Neff34,Reference Djamgoz, Hankins, Hirano and Archer35 Therefore, we hypothesise that the strong change in the retinal contrast gain is related to dopaminergic dysfunction of the retina.

In Parkinson's disease, dopaminergic disturbance in the visual system is just one aspect of a more widely distributed dopaminergic pathology predominantly affecting the nigro-striatal system. Reference Tebartz van Elst, Greenlee, Foley and Lucking12 In analogy, the possible disturbance of dopaminergic neurotransmission in the visual system of people with depressive disorder might represent just one facet of a more widely distributed pathology affecting the systemic dopaminergic system of the

Fig. 4 Correlation between contrast gain and severity of depression (Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score) at follow-up.

Healthy controls are indicated by small circles, patients in remission by diamonds and patients remaining depressed by crosses. The patients in remission all have a BDI score <10 and their contrast gain fell within the normal range. The four individuals who stayed depressed have a lower contrast gain and, naturally, higher BDI scores. PERG, pattern electroretinogram.

brain. Thus, retinal contrast gain might indicate the functional integrity of the systemic dopaminergic system, in particular the mesolimbic branch. This mesolimbic dopaminergic system innervates critical structures of affective information processing such as the amygdala, the nucleus accumbens and the prefrontal brain.

From a number of studies, there is support for an alteration in the dopaminergic system in depressive disorder. Reference Ebert, Feistel, Kaschka, Barocka and Pirner36Reference Pei, Li, Wang, Diwan, Anisman and Fletcher39 Antidepressant treatment has been found to enhance dopaminergic function and influence electrophysiologically measured responses of visual perception. Reference Normann, Schmitz, Fürmaier, Döing and Bach7,Reference Schilström, Konradsson-Geuken, Ivanov, Gertow, Feltmann and Marcus40Reference Volkow, Wang, Telang, Fowler, Logan and Wong42 The change in visual perception has been reported over a broad variety of antidepressant substance classes or treatment forms. Reference Normann, Schmitz, Fürmaier, Döing and Bach7,Reference Lavoie, Lam, Bouchard, Sasseville, Charron and Gagné8,Reference Seggie, Canny, Mai, McCrank and Waring41

Limitations

Due to the naturalistic setting it was not possible to have complete control over all factors and some limitations of the study have to be addressed. Because of the small sample size per medication group, we are not able to comment on any medication effect on the retinal contrast gain due to the lack of respective statistical power. Therefore we cannot comment on whether more adrenergic drugs influenced the improvement in retinal contrast gain more than, for example, serotonergic drugs. Such questions will have to be answered in future studies. Although, both groups did not differ significantly in the mean duration of antidepressant treatment and we compared the 4 individuals with non-remitted depression with 4 participants whose depression remitted with an even shorter duration of antidepressant treatment, the non-remitted group received a shorter period of antidepressant treatment than the remitted group and we cannot fully exclude the possibility that this might have influenced the outcome. Furthermore, we were not able to balance the follow-up time completely.

Both BDI and HRSD scores correlated strongly with the PERG contrast gain. Therefore, we assume that the improvement and normalisation of contrast gain after successful therapy here is a basic correlate of the depressive state. This view is supported by the fact that we did not find any relationship between duration of antidepressant treatment and contrast gain. Thus, the change in contrast gain seems to be physiologically related to the change in severity of depression. This assumption is further supported by findings from an early study where a change in flash-evoked potentials was related to a reduction in severity of depression as measured with the HRSD. Reference Friedman and Meares43

The ROC analysis revealed that the PERG-based contrast gain mirrors depression state best in intermediate depression. In addition, it should be recognised that there are other factors that reduce specificity of PERG-based contrast gain as a state marker: poor optical imaging (wrong glasses) or glaucoma, for instance, also affect the PERG; such confounding factors need to be excluded.

Implications

In summary, we report a normalisation of an initially reduced contrast gain in patients who remitted from a depressive episode and postulate that contrast gain is a marker of the depressive state. Based on these findings, we hypothesise that retinal contrast gain is an indicator of the functional state of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system, which in turn might be essentially linked to the clinical state of depression. The results are of high relevance if replicated, because the method would provide a marker in animals and humans to investigate treatment success in both established medications and new compounds.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to the work.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Gong, Q, Wu, Q, Scarpazza, C, Lui, S, Jia, Z, Marquand, A, Huang, X, et al. Prognostic prediction of therapeutic response in depression using high-field MR imaging. Neuroimage 2011; 55: 1497–503.Google Scholar
2 Chen, Y, Wan, HI, O'Reardon, JP, Wang, DJJ, Wang, Z, Korczykowski, M, et al. Quantification of cerebral blood flow as biomarker of drug effect: arterial spin labeling phMRI after a single dose of oral citalopram. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2011; 89: 251–8.Google Scholar
3 Domenici, E, Willé, DR, Tozzi, F, Prokopenko, I, Miller, S, McKeown, A, et al. Plasma protein biomarkers for depression and schizophrenia by multi analyte profiling of case-control collections. PLoS One 2010; 5: e9166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Farb, NAS, Anderson, AK, Bloch, RT, Segal, ZV. Mood-linked responses in medial prefrontal cortex predict relapse in patients with recurrent unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 366–72.Google Scholar
5 Landsness, EC, Goldstein, MR, Peterson, MJ, Tononi, G, Benca, RM. Antidepressant effects of selective slow wave sleep deprivation in major depression: a high-density EEG investigation. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45: 1019–26.Google Scholar
6 Bubl, E, Tebartz van Elst, L, Gondan, M, Ebert, D, Greenlee, MW. Vision in depressive disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry 2009; 10: 377–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Normann, C, Schmitz, D, Fürmaier, A, Döing, C, Bach, M. Long-term plasticity of visually evoked potentials in humans is altered in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62: 373–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Lavoie, M-P, Lam, RW, Bouchard, G, Sasseville, A, Charron, M-C, Gagné, A-M, et al. Evidence of a biological effect of light therapy on the retina of patients with seasonal affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66: 253–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Joost, W, Bach, M, Schulte-Mönting, J. Influence of mood on visually evoked potentials: a prospective longitudinal study. Int J Psychophysiol 1992; 12: 147–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Maffei, L, Fiorentini, A. Electroretinographic responses to alternating gratings before and after section of the optic nerve. Science 1981; 211: 953–5.Google Scholar
11 Bach, M, Hoffman, M. The origin of the pattern electroretinogram (PERG). In Principles and Practice of Clinical Electrophysiology of Vision (eds Heckenlively, J, Arden, G): 185–96. MIT Press, 2006.Google Scholar
12 Tebartz van Elst, L, Greenlee, MW, Foley, JM, Lucking, CH. Contrast detection, discrimination and adaptation in patients with Parkinson's disease and multiple system atrophy. Brain 1997; 120 (Pt 12): 2219–28.Google Scholar
13 Ghilardi, MF, Marx, MS, Bodis-Wollner, I, Camras, CB, Glover, AA. The effect of intraocular 6-hydroxydopamine on retinal processing of primates. Ann Neurol 1989; 25: 357–64.Google Scholar
14 Hasler, G, Fromm, S, Carlson, PJ, Luckenbaugh, DA, Waldeck, T, Geraci, M, et al. Neural response to catecholamine depletion in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder in remission and healthy subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65: 521–31.Google Scholar
15 Ben-Shlomo, G, Bach, M, Ofri, R. Temporal and spatial frequencies interact in the contrast transfer function of the pattern electroretinogram. Vision Res 2007; 47: 1992–9.Google Scholar
16 Bubl, E, Kern, E, Ebert, D, Bach, M, Tebartz van Elst, L. Seeing gray when feeling blue? Depression can be measured in the eye of the diseased. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68: 205–8.Google Scholar
17 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). APA, 1994.Google Scholar
18 Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J ClinPsychiatry 1998; 59 (suppl 20): 2233.Google Scholar
19 Hautzinger, M, Bailer, M, Worrall, H, Keller, F. Das Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI). Überarbeitet und ergänzte Neuauflage. Hans Huber, 1995.Google Scholar
20 Collegium Internationale Psychiatriae Scalarum. Internationale Skalen für Psychiatrie. Beltz Test, 1977.Google Scholar
21 Keller, MB. Undertreatment of major depression. Psychopharmacol Bull 1988; 24: 7580.Google Scholar
22 Leon, AC, Solomon, DA, Mueller, TI, Endicott, J, Rice, JP, Maser, JD, et al. A 20-year longitudinal observational study of somatic antidepressant treatment effectiveness. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 727–33.Google Scholar
23 Bach, M. The Freiburg Visual Acuity Test - variability unchanged by post-hoc re-analysis. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2007; 245: 965–71.Google ScholarPubMed
24 Bach, M. Freiburg Evoked Potentials. Bach, 2000 (http://www.michaelbach.de/ep2000/index.html).Google Scholar
25 Bach, M. Preparation and Montage of DTL-Electrodes. University of Freiburg, 2007. (http://www.uniklinik-freiburg.de/augenklinik/live/homede/mit/bach/ops/dtl_en.html).Google Scholar
26 Otto, T, Bach, M. Retest variability and diurnal effects in the pattern electroretinogram. Doc Ophthalmol 1996; 92: 311–23.Google Scholar
27 Bach, M, Meigen, T. Do's and don'ts in Fourier analysis of steady-state potentials. Doc Ophthalmol 1999; 99: 6982.Google Scholar
28 Meigen, T, Bach, M. On the statistical significance of electrophysiological steady-state responses. Doc Ophthalmol 1999; 98: 207–32.Google Scholar
29 R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2010.Google Scholar
30 Bodis-Wollner, I, Yahr, MD. Measurements of visual evoked potentials in Parkinson's disease. Brain 1978; 101: 661–71.Google Scholar
31 Bodis-Wollner, I, Marx, MS, Mitra, S, Bobak, P, Mylin, L, Yahr, M. Visual dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Loss in spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity. Brain 1987; 110: 1675–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32 Glover, A, Ghilardi, MF, Bodis-Wollner, I, Onofrj, M. Alterations in event-related potentials (ERPs) of MPTP-treated monkeys. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1988; 71: 461–8.Google Scholar
33 Langheinrich, T, van Elst, Tebartz, Lagrèze, WA, Bach, M, Lücking, CH, Greenlee, MW. Visual contrast response functions in Parkinson's disease: evidence from electroretinograms, visually evoked potentials and psychophysics. Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 111: 6674.Google Scholar
34 Iuvone, PM, Galli, CL, Garrison-Gund, CK, Neff, NH. Light stimulates tyrosine hydroxylase activity and dopamine synthesis in retinal amacrine neurons. Science 1978; 202: 901–2.Google Scholar
35 Djamgoz, MB, Hankins, MW, Hirano, J, Archer, SN. Neurobiology of retinal dopamine in relation to degenerative states of the tissue. Vision Res 1997; 37: 3509–29.Google Scholar
36 Ebert, D, Feistel, H, Kaschka, W, Barocka, A, Pirner, A. Single photon emission computerized tomography assessment of cerebral dopamine D2 receptor blockade in depression before and after sleep deprivation–preliminary results. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 35: 880–5.Google Scholar
37 Hamilton, JP, Chen, G, Thomason, ME, Schwartz, ME, Gotlib, IH. Investigating neural primacy in major depressive disorder: multivariate Granger causality analysis of resting-state fMRI time-series data. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16: 763–72.Google Scholar
38 Saijo, T, Takano, A, Suhara, T, Arakawa, R, Okumura, M, Ichimiya, T, et al. Electroconvulsive therapy decreases dopamine D2 receptor binding in the anterior cingulate in patients with depression: a controlled study using positron emission tomography with radioligand [11C]FLB 457. J Clin Psychiatry 2010; 71: 793–9.Google Scholar
39 Pei, L, Li, S, Wang, M, Diwan, M, Anisman, H, Fletcher, PJ, et al. Uncoupling the dopamine D1-D2 receptor complex exerts antidepressant-like effects. Nat Med 2010; 16: 1393–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40 Schilström, B, Konradsson-Geuken, A, Ivanov, V, Gertow, J, Feltmann, K, Marcus, MM, et al. Effects of S-citalopram, citalopram, and R-citalopram on the firing patterns of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area, N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor-mediated transmission in the medial prefrontal cortex and cognitive function in the rat. Synapse 2011; 65: 357–67.Google Scholar
41 Seggie, J, Canny, C, Mai, F, McCrank, E, Waring, E. Antidepressant medication reverses increased sensitivity to light in depression: preliminary report. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1989; 13: 537–41.Google Scholar
42 Volkow, ND, Wang, G-J, Telang, F, Fowler, JS, Logan, J, Wong, C, et al. Sleep deprivation decreases binding of [11C]raclopride to dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the human brain. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 8454–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43 Friedman, J, Meares, R. The effect of placebo and tricyclic antidepressants on cortical evoked potentials in depressed patients. Biol Psychol 1979; 8: 291302.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Pattern electroretinogram (PERG) recordings from two representative participants. Healthy control (left), a patient with depression (centre) and the same patient with remission on medication (right). These recordings illustrate a clear reduction in PERG amplitudes in an individual with depression (upper row, middle), which returns to normal levels after remission of depression (upper row right; recording of healthy individual upper row left). The stimulus contrast increases from 3.2% (bottom) to 80% (top). There are raw PERG traces for each participant on the left (the ordinate spans 8 mV, the abscissa 950 ms), and on the right, the Fourier magnitude spectra from 1 to 100 Hz. The expected response is at 12 Hz (the reversal rate). For high contrasts there is also a third harmonic response at 24 Hz, and the prominent spectral line at 50 Hz represents a mains artefact well separated in frequency space. For further analysis see Fig. 2. OD, right eye; OS, left eye.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 (a) Pattern electroretinogram (PERG) magnitude v. contrast for two participants (a healthy control and a patient with depression before and after remission, single eye) based on the traces in Fig. 1; (b) Group PERG contrast gain, grand average.In (a) the symbols represent the spectral response magnitude, the oblique lines depict linear fits. The slope of the linear model is interpreted as the PERG-based contrast gain. In (b) healthy controls (left) and patients in remission (right) do not differ significantly, whereas patients with current depression present with a significantly reduced contrast gain. Boxplot details on the right: the median is indicated by the thick horizontal lines, the notches represent a 95% confidence interval for the medians, the box covers the 25–7 5% percentile range, the ‘antennas’ indicate the range, outliers are indicated by circles.

Figure 2

TABLE 1 Gender, age, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) and contrast gain (slope) of the study groups

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Relationship between contrast gain at follow-up (visit 2) and baseline (visit 1).The patients that stayed depressed (indicated by crosses) are near the identity line, whereas those who remitted (diamonds) presented with a significant improvement after remission, and their contrast gain fell within the normal range. PERG, pattern electroretinogram.

Figure 4

TABLE 2 Individual patient data on gender, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) and contrast gain (slope)

Figure 5

Fig. 4 Correlation between contrast gain and severity of depression (Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score) at follow-up.Healthy controls are indicated by small circles, patients in remission by diamonds and patients remaining depressed by crosses. The patients in remission all have a BDI score <10 and their contrast gain fell within the normal range. The four individuals who stayed depressed have a lower contrast gain and, naturally, higher BDI scores. PERG, pattern electroretinogram.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.