Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T23:23:45.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Recurrent Criminal Behavior and Executive Dysfunction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Manuel Fernando Santos Barbosa*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Porto
Luis Manuel Coelho Monteiro
Affiliation:
Instituto Superior de Ciencias de la Salud – Norte, Porto, Portugal
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to F. Barbosa, Facultad de Psicología y Ciencias de la Educación, Universidad de Porto, Rua Dr. Manuel Pereira da Silva, 4200-392 Porto (Portugal). Phone: +351 22 6079700. Email: fbarbosa@fpce.up.pt

Abstract

Objective:

To experimentally test the hypothesis that people who repeatedly participate in forms of non-violent crime exhibit an executive deficit detected in tests of high ecological validity, having changes in prefrontal functioning as neurophysiologic basis.

Participants and Methods:

A battery to assess executive dysfunction was administered – the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) –to an experimental group of 30 inmates convicted of crimes against property (mean age = 39.3, SD =9.98), and a control group of 30 (mean age = 32.7, SD = 11.8), all male.

Results:

The group of recurrent inmates performed significantly worse than the control group in their global scores on the battery, as well as in the majority of subscales.

Conclusion:

Without removing from consideration the fact that sample size was not very large and, primarily, alerting ourselves to the dangerous hypothesis of a “frontal criminogenesis,” the authors interpret criminal recurrence and resistance to penal measures in terms of the scarcity of control that individuals from the experimental group have over their behavior and its respective consequences.

Objetivo: Verificar experimentalmente la hipótesis de que las personas que participan repetidamente en delitos no violentos exhiben un déficit ejecutivo en pruebas de alta validez ecológica, y cambios en su funcionamiento prefrontal como substrato neurofisiológico.

Participantes y Métodos: Se administró una batería para evaluar la disfunción ejecutiva – el Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) –a un grupo experimental de 30 presos condenados por crímenes contra la propiedad (edad = 39,3, DT = 9,98) y a un grupo control de 30 personas (edad = 32.7, DT =11.8), todos varones.

Resultados: el grupo de presos reincidentes obtuvieron puntuaciones significativamente peores que las del grupo control en la batería, así como en la mayoría de las subescalas.

Conclusión: sin dejar de considerar el hecho de que ninguna de las muestras era de tamaño muy grande y, especialmente alertando acerca de la hipótesis peligrosa de una “criminogénesis frontal”, los autores interpretan le reincidencia criminal y la resistencia a las normas del código penal en términos de la falta de control que los individuos del grupo experimental tienen sobre su comportamiento y sus consecuencias respectivas.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Barbosa, F. (2000). Reincidência criminal: 20 anos de investigação neuropsicológica. Saúde-Mental, 2, 2938.Google Scholar
Barbosa, F. (2001). Psicofisiologia do comportamento criminal reincidente: índices periféricos autonómicos. Saúde-Mental, 3, 1932.Google Scholar
Crawford, J. (1998). Assessment of attention and executive function. [Special issue]. Journal of Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Crawford, J., Blackmore, L., Lamb, A., & Simpson, S. (2000). Is there a differential deficit in fronto-executive functioning in Huntington's disease? Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 1, 420.Google Scholar
Damásio, A. (1994). O erro de Descartes (17th ed.). Sintra, Portugal: PEA.Google Scholar
Folstein, M., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-Mental State,” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groth-Marnat, G. (2000). Neuropsychological assessment in clinical practice: A guide to test interpretation and integration. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hare, R., Frazelle, J., Bus, J., & Jutai, J. (1980). Psychopathy and structure of primary mental abilities. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 2, 7788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, J. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Society, 2, 327347.Google Scholar
Lindman, H. (1974). Analysis of variance in complex experimental designs. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, R., & Yudofsky, S. (1986). Psychiatric evaluation in clinical practice. New York: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Marques-Teixeira, J. (2000). Comportamento criminal: perspectiva biopsicológica. Linda a Velha, Portugal: Vale & Vale.Google Scholar
Meyers, C., Berman, S., Scheibel, R., & Hayman, A. (1992). Case report: Acquired antisocial personality disorder associated with unilateral left orbital frontal lobe damage. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 17, 121125.Google ScholarPubMed
Morgan, A., & Lilienfeld, S. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action. Willed and automatic control of behavior. San Diego: University of California.Google Scholar
Norris, G., & Tate, R. (2000). The Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Ecological, concurrent and construct validity. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10, 3345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pineda, D. (1996). Disfunción ejecutiva en niños con trastornos por deficiencia atencional con hiperactividad (TDAH). Acta Neurológica Colombiana, 12, 1925.Google Scholar
Pineda, D., Cadavid, C., & Mancheno, S. (1996). Características de la función ejecutiva en niños con deficiencia atencional e hiperactividad (DAH). Acta Neurológica Colombiana, 12, 187196.Google Scholar
Saver, J., & Damasio, A. (1991). Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia, 29, 12411249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selby, M., Airy-Eggertsen, A., & Laver, G. (1998). Comparison of neuropsychological test performance in forensic and nonforensic populations. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 16, 4558.Google Scholar
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silver, J., & Yudofsky, S. (1987). Aggressive behavior in patients with neuropsychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Annals, 17, 367370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stuss, D., & Benson, D. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Weyandt, L., & Willis, W. (1994). Executive function in school-aged children: Potential efficacy of tasks in discriminating clinical groups. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 2738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, B., Alderman, N., Burgess, P., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. (1996). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (Bads). London: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
Wilson, B., Evans, J., Emslie, H., Alderman, N., & Burgess, P. (1998). The development of an ecologically valid test for assessing patients with a dysexecutive syndrome. [Special issue]. Journal of Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. Psychology Press.Google Scholar