Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T21:51:06.390Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adherence to the MIND diet is associated with 12-year all-cause mortality in older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2020

Janie Corley*
Affiliation:
Lothian Birth Cohorts Group, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email Janie.Corley@ed.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To prospectively evaluate the association of three dietary patterns: the MIND (Mediterranean-DASH diet intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay) diet; a Mediterranean-type diet and a traditional diet, with all-cause mortality over a 12-year period in an older sample.

Design:

A longitudinal birth cohort study. We ascertained dietary patterns using FFQ data at baseline (2004–2007) and mortality using linkage data. Cox regression was used to estimate mortality hazard ratios (HR) with adjustment for confounders.

Setting:

The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 (LBC1936) study in Edinburgh, Scotland.

Participants:

Dietary patterns were ascertained in 882 participants, mean age 69·5 (±0·8) years, at baseline. During the 12-year follow-up (to October 2019), 206 deaths occurred.

Results:

In the basic-adjusted model, all three dietary patterns were significantly associated with mortality, the MIND diet and Mediterranean-type diet with a lower risk and the traditional diet with a higher risk. In fully adjusted models, MIND diet score was inversely related to all-cause mortality (HR 0·88; 95 % CI 0·79, 0·97) such that the risk of death was reduced by 12 % per unit increase in MIND diet score. Participants in the top compared with the bottom third of MIND diet score had a 37 % lower risk of death (HR 0·63; 95 % CI 0·41, 0·96). No significant associations with the Mediterranean-type or traditional dietary patterns were observed in the final multivariate model.

Conclusions:

Our findings suggest that closer adherence to the MIND diet is associated with a significantly lower risk of all-cause mortality, over 12 years of follow-up, and may constitute a valid public health recommendation for prolonged survival.

Type
Research paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Quality of diet is fundamental to health. Epidemiological evidence suggests that the Mediterranean diet, characterised by an abundance of fruits, vegetables, fish, legumes, olive oil and wholegrains, is an optimal diet for promoting longevity and preventing chronic ageing-related disease(Reference Trichopoulou and Benetou1). Conversely, eating a more traditional or processed diet has been linked to poorer health outcomes and an increased risk of mortality(Reference Atkins, Whincup and Morris2Reference O’Sullivan, Hafekost and Mitrou4). Assessing the relationship between diet and risk of mortality is of scientific interest because it can shed light on the possible health benefits gained by adhering to certain dietary patterns and therefore has the potential to translate into public health recommendations for reducing the risk of premature death.

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of prospective studies report clear and consistent evidence that individuals who adhere to a Mediterranean diet are less likely to die(Reference Sofi, Abbate and Gensini5,Reference Sofi, Macchi and Abbate6) . Averaged across fourteen prospective cohort studies, a two-unit increase in Mediterranean diet score was associated with decreased mortality from any cause by approximately 8 % (pooled hazard ratio (HR) 0·92; 95 % CI 0·91, 0·93)(Reference Sofi, Macchi and Abbate6). Recently, a larger meta-analysis of twenty-nine prospective cohort studies reported similar findings, supporting an inverse association (pooled HR 0·90, 95 % CI 0·89, 0·91), whereby a two-unit increase in the adherence to a Mediterranean diet was associated with a 10 % lower risk of all-cause mortality(Reference Soltani, Jayedi and Shab-Bidar7). However, the magnitude of the ‘protective’ effect varies markedly between individual studies. For example, the SUN cohort based in Spain reported a substantial 17 % reduced risk of mortality per two-unit increase in Mediterranean score and a 62 % reduced risk in the high compared with the low adherence groups(Reference Martinez-Gonzalez, Guillen-Grima and de Irala8). Evidence suggests stronger associations in Mediterranean populations where the consumption of the beneficial components of the Mediterranean diet is higher(Reference Soltani, Jayedi and Shab-Bidar7,Reference Trichopoulou, Kouris-Blazos and Wahlqvist9) .

Few studies have focused on older adults. In the only meta-analysis of the Mediterranean diet with survival in the elderly (≥65 years, n 11 738) to date, a two-unit increase in Mediterranean diet score was associated with a 10 % lower risk of all-cause mortality(Reference Bonaccio, Di Castelnuovo and Costanzo10). This pooled survival estimate, averaged across seven prospective studies, is comparable with those reported among the mixed age cohorts, as above(Reference Sofi, Abbate and Gensini5Reference Soltani, Jayedi and Shab-Bidar7). However, in one UK study, a healthy dietary pattern was protective against mortality in the elderly only when participants practised at least one other healthy lifestyle factor such as never smoking, occasional drinking or physical activity(Reference Zhao, Ukawa and Okada11).

The MIND (Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay) diet(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang12) has been developed in recent years and is based on elements of the Mediterranean diet and the DASH (Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension) diet. The MIND diet shares many food groups with both diets and emphasises plant-based foods but with modifications reflecting the best scientific evidence for nutrition and the ageing brain. As such, it specifically promotes the consumption of green leafy vegetables and berries, but not overall fruit intake, for which the overall evidence is weak(Reference Loef and Walach13), and limited intakes of animal-based and high-saturated fat foods. Several studies have reported that the MIND diet is associated with slower cognitive decline(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang12), reduced rates of cognitive impairment(Reference Hosking, Eramudugolla and Anstey14), Alzheimer’s disease(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang15) and Parkinson’s disease(Reference Agarwal, Wang and Buchman16). In some studies, the MIND diet score was a better predictor of cognitive decline(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang12) and incidence of cognitive impairment(Reference Hosking, Eramudugolla and Anstey14) compared with the Mediterranean diet.

The MIND diet may be a relevant target for mortality research because it was originally designed to provide dietary guidelines with the specific intention of addressing neuroprotection and is a combination of two diets that have been shown to lower the risk of hypertension, heart attack and stroke(Reference Fung, Rexrode and Mantzoros17,Reference Salehi-Abargouei, Maghsoudi and Shirani18) . Yet, to the best of our knowledge, there has been no attempt to evaluate the MIND diet in relation to mortality and hence validate its presumed efficacy as a public health intervention strategy.

Thus, in the first known study to investigate the MIND diet with mortality, our aim was to evaluate associations between adherence to the MIND diet (using an a priori score developed by Morris et al.)(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang12) and mortality risk over 12 years in a narrow-age cohort of older Scottish men and women. We additionally examine mortality associations with two further dietary patterns – a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and a traditional pattern – both were previously derived for this sample at baseline using a data-driven (a posteriori) approach.

Methods

Study population

The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 (LBC1936) is a group of individuals born in 1936 who took part in the 1947 Scottish Mental Survey, a nationwide survey of intelligence, when they were aged 11 years(19). Individuals who took the original test, and living in the Lothians region of Scotland, were contacted almost 60 years later, of whom 1091 were recruited to the LBC1936. Since baseline (2004–2007, mean age = 69·5 years), participants have subsequently attended additional follow-up assessments at ages 73 (wave 2, n 866), age 76 (wave 3, n 697), age 79 (wave 4, n 550), age 82 (wave 5, n 431) and the study is ongoing. Extensive phenotypic data have been collected including cognitive, neuroimaging, health, medical, genetic and epigenetic, psychosocial and lifestyle measures. Full recruitment and testing procedures of the LBC1936 are described elsewhere(Reference Taylor, Pattie and Deary20).

Exclusion criteria

At baseline, all participants (n 1091) were invited to complete a FFQ at home for return by post. A total of 124 FFQ (11 %) were not completed (ninety-eight were not returned and twenty-six were returned blank). Of the 967 returned FFQ, thirty-nine were excluded with >10 missing items, according to standard FFQ protocol, and a further forty-six were excluded due to having extreme energy intakes, defined as <2·5th or >97·5th centile. Thus, 882 participants provided usable dietary information at baseline.

Dietary assessment

Dietary intake was assessed at baseline using a semi-quantitative 168-item Scottish Collaborative Group FFQ(21). Participants were asked to indicate how often they consumed a given item (using a common unit or portion size) according to a nine-category frequency scale ranging from ‘rarely or never’ to ‘7 or more times/d’. The FFQ has been validated against weighed food intake in British populations(Reference Jia, Craig and Aucott22).

The MIND diet

The MIND score(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang12) comprises fifteen dietary components: ten brain healthy foods (green leafy vegetables, other vegetables, nuts, berries, beans, wholegrains, fish, poultry, olive oil and wine) and five less healthy foods (red meats, butter/margarine, cheese, pastries and sweets and fried/fast food). Olive oil consumption was scored 1 if used as the primary oil. For all other diet score components, the frequency of consumption of each food item associated with that component was summed and then assigned a concordance score of 0, 0·5 or 1. The total MIND diet score was calculated by adding the individual component scores. The Scottish Collaborative Group FFQ did not include questions for butter/margarine consumption as separate items, so the maximum score possible in the current sample was 14, with higher scores indicating greater adherence to the MIND diet.

The Mediterranean-type diet and traditional diet

Dietary patterns in the LBC1936 at baseline were derived previously, by applying principal component analysis to all FFQ items. Full details can be found in Mõttus et al.(Reference Mõttus, McNeill and Jia23). Principal component analysis is a widely used exploratory method in epidemiology to derive population-specific dietary patterns from habitual diet(Reference Panagiotakos24,Reference Schwedhelm, Iqbal and Knüppel25) . Briefly, components were rotated by a Varimax procedure resulting in non-correlated factors to facilitate interpretability. Components were retained based on an eigenvalue >1·25, Scree plots and interpretability. A cut-off of 0·30 was used to determine factor loadings included in each pattern. Two major patterns were extracted and labelled according to the types of foods with the highest factor loadings. The first component was characterised by food loadings (indicating greater consumption) of vegetables, fish, poultry, legumes, pasta, rice, water, tomato-based sauces, olive oil and salad dressing. This component captured a healthy diet and included loadings from foods typical of a Mediterranean diet and, thus, labelled ‘a Mediterranean-type’ dietary pattern. A second component was characterised by food loadings from meat and processed meats such as chicken pies, pasties and sausage rolls, tinned vegetables, peas or beans, carrots, baked beans, bottled sauces, mashed potatoes, custard or other sweet sauces, milk-based puddings and drinking less ground (filter, espresso or cappuccino) coffee. Overall, this diet typifies a traditional Scottish diet which is higher in processed and convenience foods, and thus, this component was labelled a traditional dietary pattern. Factor scores for both dietary patterns were calculated for each participant by summing the intakes of the food groups weighted by their factor loadings. The higher the score, the closer the diet to the dietary pattern, and the lower the score, the further the diet from the dietary pattern.

Assessment of potential confounders

Covariates were assessed at the baseline visit when participants underwent interviews with trained psychologists and nurses. Total energy intake (kcal/d) was calculated using full FFQ data. Age 11 IQ was derived from scores obtained from 1947 Scottish Mental Survey records and converted to a standard IQ-type score (where mean = 100, sd = 15). Education was assessed using years of formal full-time education. Own socio-economic status (SES) was coded from 1 (professional) to 5 (unskilled) and calculated from highest obtained occupation data, using the Registrar General’s Classification of Occupations 1980(26). Father’s SES was coded from 1 (professional) to 5 (unskilled) according to the 1950 Classification of Occupations(27). Smoking status was categorised into never smoker, former smoker and current smoker. Depressive symptoms were measured using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale depression subtest(Reference Zigmond and Snaith28), which has a score range from 0 to 21. Physical activity (level) was derived from responses to a questionnaire item which asked: ‘What level of physical activity do you mainly do?’ and responses were on a six-point scale: (1) moving only in connection with necessary (household) chores; (2) walking or other outdoor activities 1–2 times/week; (3) walking or other outdoor activities several times a week; (4) exercising 1–2 times/week to the point of perspiring and heavy breathing; (5) exercising several times/week to the point of perspiring and heavy breathing and (6) keep fit/heavy exercise or competitive sport several times/week. BMI was calculated as weight (in kg)/height (in m)2; height and weight measures were taken by nurses at the time of assessment. History of disease (hypertension, CVD, diabetes and stroke) was binary coded as no (0) and yes (1).

Mortality ascertainment

Mortality status was obtained via linkage data from the National Health Service Central Register, provided by the General Register Office for Scotland (now National Records of Scotland). Participants were followed from baseline assessment date (between 2004 and 2007) to the end of the follow-up period (October 2019). During this period, the LBC1936 study was informed of 277 deaths (25 % of cohort), 206 of whom had provided dietary data at baseline.

Statistical analyses

Participants were divided into tertiles of diet scores for descriptive purposes only. Participant characteristics were compared across tertiles using ANOVA or χ 2 test for categorical variables.

Cox proportional hazard regression models were used to estimate the effects of diet score (analysed as continuous variables) on all-cause mortality. Relative risks were estimated as HR with 95 % CI. In the Cox models, basic-adjusted (age, sex and energy intake) and multivariate-adjusted HR were calculated for each of the three diet scores. In a second (+ demographic/lifestyle-adjusted) model, we additionally included age 11 IQ, education, own SES, father’s SES, smoking status, depression and physical activity. In a third (+ health-adjusted) fully adjusted model, we additionally included BMI, hypertension, CVD, diabetes and stroke.

Post hoc analyses repeated the same models but using a categorical measure of diet (according to tertiles of adherence) if the continuous variable was previously significant in the Cox regression models. We used the lowest tertile of adherence (i.e. least adherent) as the reference group.

Survival to follow-up date (October 2019) was assessed based on the day of initial assessment at baseline. All statistical analyses were done in SPSS (version 25; SPSS Inc.), and a P < 0·05 was considered significant.

Results

Baseline dietary data were available for 882 participants (51·8 % female). The mean age of participants at baseline was 69·5 (±0·8) years. During a mean follow-up time of 12·1 (±3·2) years (median = 13·2 years), there were 206 deaths (41·7 % female). Baseline characteristics of participants according to survival status are presented in Table 1. Participants who died over the follow-up period were more likely to be male, a current or former smoker, less physically active, and to have a lower age 11 IQ score, a lower own SES and father SES, more depressive symptoms, a higher BMI and a self-reported history of disease (hypertension, CVD, diabetes and stroke).

Table 1 Characteristics of participants (n 882) according to mortality status, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

SES, socioeconomic status; prof, professional.

* To convert kcal to kJ, multiply it by 4·184.

Depressive symptoms calculated using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression subscale score, (range 1–21).

Physical activity levels: 1, moving only in connection with necessary (household) chores; 2, walking or other outdoor activities 1–2 times/week; 3, walking or other outdoor activities several times a week; 4, exercising 1–2 times/week to the point of perspiring and heavy breathing; 5, exercising several times/week to the point of perspiring and heavy breathing; 6, keep fit/heavy exercise or competitive sport several times/week.

Table 2 presents the baseline characteristics of participants according to tertiles of each dietary predictor (MIND, Mediterranean-type and traditional diet pattern) score. Higher MIND (P < 0·001) and Mediterranean-type (P = 0·005) diet scores, and lower traditional (P = 0·008) diet scores, were associated with fewer deaths to follow-up. Higher MIND diet adherence (range 0–13, out of a maximum possible score of 14) was associated with being female, and having a higher age 11 IQ, more education, a more professional SES, less smoking and less depressive symptoms. Higher Mediterranean-type diet adherence was associated with being younger, a higher overall energy intake, higher age 11 IQ and more education, a more professional own SES and father SES, less smoking, less depressive symptoms and more exercise. In contrast, higher traditional diet adherence was associated with being older, male, a higher overall energy intake, a lower age 11 IQ, less education, a lower own SES and father SES, more smoking, more depressive symptoms, less exercise and a history of stroke. We note, here, that unlike the Mediterranean-type diet and traditional diet pattern, there was no association between MIND diet adherence and energy intake.

Table 2 Associations between baseline (2004–2007) characteristics and tertiles of the MIND, Mediterranean-type and traditional diet scores, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

SES, socio-economic status; prof, professional.

* To convert kcal to kJ, multiply it by 4·184.

Own SES, % prof, is the % of participants in occupational social classes 1 & 2 (indicating professional occupations).

Father SES, % prof, is the % of participants with father in occupational social classes 1 & 2 (indicating professional occupations).

§ Depressive symptoms calculated using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression subscale score (range 1–21).

Physical activity, % low, is the % of participants in the two least active groups (1, moving only in accordance with household chores, and 2, walking 1–2 times a week).

Table 3 presents the results of Cox proportional hazard models to test the associations between each of the diet scores (as continuous variables) and risk of all-cause mortality over 12 years of follow-up. In the basic-adjusted model (age, sex and energy intake), all three diet patterns were associated with risk of mortality; the MIND diet and Mediterranean-type diet were associated with a lower risk of mortality (HR 0·81; 95 % CI 0·74, 0·88, P < 0·001 and HR 0·74; 95 % CI 0·61, 0·89, P = 0·002, respectively) and the traditional diet with a higher risk of mortality (HR 1·30; 95 % CI 1·13, 1·49, P = 0·001).

Table 3 Associations between diet scores* and all-cause mortality over 12 years of follow-up, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

MIND, Mediterranean-DASH intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay; HR, hazard ratio; SES, socioeconomic status.

* Dietary pattern scores are analysed as continuous variables.

Model 1: adjusted for age, sex and energy intake (basic-adjusted).

Model 2: additionally adjusted for age 11 IQ, education, own SES, father’s SES, smoking status, depression symptoms and physical activity (demographic and lifestyle-adjusted).

§ Model 3: additionally adjusted for BMI, hypertension, diabetes, CVD and stroke (health-adjusted).

After multivariate adjustment for demographic and lifestyle confounders (model 2), the associations with the MIND diet and traditional diet remained significant, but the Mediterranean-type diet–mortality associations were attenuated to non-significance. In the final, fully adjusted model (model 3) which additionally adjusted for health variables, only the associations with the MIND diet remained significant (HR 0·88; 95 % CI 0·79, 0·97, P = 0·01). Thus after adjusting for a range of potential confounders, there was a decreased risk of death in those whose diets more closely adhered to the MIND diet at baseline; risk of death was reduced by 12 % per unit increase in MIND diet score.

Additional post hoc analyses examined the MIND diet score as a categorical variable in the same Cox regression models in order to assess the relative risk of mortality between low, medium and high adherence groups. Using the lowest MIND tertile as the reference group, we found no significant difference between the low and medium adherence groups (HR 0·74; 95 % CI 0·52, 1·05, P = 0·09). However, those in the highest MIND adherence tertile had a 37 % lower risk of death (HR 0·63; 95 % CI 0·41, 0·96, P = 0·03) compared with those in the low tertile.

Supplemental Table 1 presents the full results of the multivariable Cox model (model 3) to show the effects of each covariate as well as the dietary pattern scores alone. Smoking (all P < 0·001) had the largest effect sizes across all three diet–mortality models: MIND diet (HR 2·03, 95 % CI 1·62, 2·55); Mediterranean-type diet (HR 2·08, 95 % CI 1·67, 2·59) and traditional diet (HR 2·06, 95 % CI 1·65, 2·57). Own SES, age, education, BMI, CVD and stroke also contributed to the variance in mortality across these models. Energy intake was a confounder of the traditional diet–mortality associations only.

Discussion

In this prospective cohort study of older adults in Scotland, we found that greater adherence to the MIND diet was associated with a 12 % reduction in all-cause mortality over 12 years, per unit increase in MIND score. Individuals in the highest tertile of adherence had 37 % lower odds of premature death compared with those in the lowest group. Associations were present even after adjusting for numerous factors that correlate with a healthy lifestyle. Neither the Mediterranean-type diet nor the traditional diet patterns were significantly associated with mortality in the fully adjusted model. These findings suggest that the MIND diet, originally developed as a strategy to promote healthy cognitive ageing, may also reflect an optimal dietary pattern for prolonged survival.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to evaluate the association between adherence to the MIND diet and mortality. Conformity to the MIND diet has been demonstrated to be more effective in slowing cognitive decline and reducing incidence of Alzheimer’s disease than either the DASH or Mediterranean diets(Reference Hosking, Eramudugolla and Anstey14,Reference Gardener and Rainey-Smith29,Reference van den Brink, Brouwer-Brolsma and Berendsen30) . Our findings indicate an overall impact on health, of the MIND diet, which may extend beyond neuroprotection. Given that diet is a modifiable factor, this report is important because it has the potential to contribute to public health strategy, for the prevention of early death.

There are several biological mechanisms by which the MIND diet could impact health and survival. Although the MIND diet and the Mediterranean diet have similar dietary profiles – both share an emphasis on beneficial food components such as vegetables, fibre, wholegrains and a low red meat intake, which have independently been associated with a reduction in mortality risk(Reference Aune, Giovannucci and Boffetta31Reference Yang, Zhao and Wu35) – the MIND diet uniquely includes separate categories for green leafy vegetables and berries (v. fruit in general) which are reported to protect against neurodegeneration(Reference Morris36). Studies on berries and mortality are rare; however, one population-based Norwegian cohort study of 10 000 men observed a significant inverse association with all-cause mortality over four decades(Reference Hjartåker, Knudsen and Tretli37). In addition, other studies(Reference Iso and Kubota38,Reference Takachi, Inoue and Ishihara39) and a systematic review(Reference Tamakoshi, Tamakoshi and Lin40) have detected significant risk reductions in total mortality with daily intake of green leafy vegetables. The potentially protective effects of green leafy vegetables and berries on health and survival may be explained by their potent antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties, which is demonstrated for a number of their bioactive compounds, such as polyphenols(Reference Carlsen, Halvorsen and Holte41Reference Tufts, Harris and Bukania46). As such, these foods may not only protect against cognitive decline but also physiological decline associated with ageing as well.

Second, unlike the Mediterranean diet score, the MIND diet score incorporates categories for less healthy food components such as ‘pastries and sweets’, ‘fast/fried foods’ and ‘butter or margarine’ due to their potentially detrimental effects on brain health(Reference Morris, Tangney and Wang15). Such foods are high in sugar, saturated fat and trans fatty acids and have an unfavourable effect on a range of health outcomes via pro-inflammatory and pro-oxidative processes(Reference Esposito and Giugliano47). Fast food has been shown to have a detrimental effect on future mortality risk (HR 1·16; 95 % CI 1·04, 1·29; P = 0·004; comparing highest v. lowest quartile) in a large population sample (n 69 582) of older adults aged 50–76 years in the VITAL study(Reference Barrington and White48). In the British Regional Heart Study, adopting a diet that avoids high-sugar components was associated with a reduced odds of all-cause mortality in those aged 60–79 years at baseline(Reference Atkins, Whincup and Morris2) and in CHD mortality over 50 years in 12 763 men in the Seven Countries study(Reference Menotti, Kromhout and Puddu49). Given the results of the present study, it is plausible to assume that the inclusion of healthy and less healthy food components in the calculation of the MIND score may be contributing to the observed associations with reduced mortality.

The Mediterranean-type dietary pattern was not significantly associated with mortality in our Scottish sample, once we accounted for a number of demographic and health factors, although prior studies have shown this diet to be strongly and inversely associated with death(Reference Martinez-Gonzalez, Guillen-Grima and de Irala8Reference Bonaccio, Di Castelnuovo and Costanzo10,Reference Bellavia, Tektonidis and Orsini50Reference Trichopoulou, Orfanos and Norat58) . The majority of these investigations used the Medi score which measures adherence to a traditional Greek Mediterranean diet(Reference Trichopoulou, Costacou and Bamia57) characterised by high intake of fruit and vegetables, wholegrains, fish, olive oil, a low intake of meat (white and red) and dairy products, and moderate alcohol consumption. Previous studies of older adults support a protective effect of the Mediterranean diet on all-cause mortality in Mediterranean(Reference Trichopoulou, Kouris-Blazos and Wahlqvist9,Reference Trichopoulou, Costacou and Bamia57,Reference Trichopoulou, Orfanos and Norat58) and non-Mediterranean populations(Reference Knoops, Fidanza and Alberti-Fidanza52,Reference Kouris-Blazos, Gnardellis and Wahlqvist53,Reference McNaughton, Bates and Mishra59,Reference Sjögren, Becker and Warensjö60) . However, evidence from a recent meta-analysis suggests that the reduction in mortality may be weaker in countries outside of the Mediterranean basin(Reference Soltani, Jayedi and Shab-Bidar7). Yet in the UK, where typical consumption of specific Mediterranean diet foods may be lower, high compared with low Mediterranean diet score was related to a 22 % decreased risk in overall mortality in adults aged 65 years and older (n 972) in the British Diet and Nutrition Survey(Reference McNaughton, Bates and Mishra59).

Methodologic differences in Mediterranean score derivation may be one explanation for our null finding; in the LBC1936, adherence to the Mediterranean-type pattern was assessed using an a posteriori score using whole-diet data. Dietary patterns derived from data reduction methods have the advantage of considering additive and interactive effects of all dietary constituents(Reference Hu61). A priori defined Mediterranean diet scores, on the other hand, quantify how closely a person’s diet conforms to a set of pre-defined guidelines. The most widely used scoring system was created by Trichopoulou et al. (Reference Trichopoulou, Costacou and Bamia57). Both are valid approaches for determining Mediterranean diet adherence(Reference Bountziouka, Tzavelas and Polychronopoulos62), but one consequence of this is that comparisons between studies can be problematic. Additionally, a 2016 review paper reported that most prospective studies use sample-specific intake thresholds to measure adherence (by percentile categories)(Reference Morris36), and therefore, similar scores can reflect different eating patterns across different cohorts. In the current study, neither wine nor fruit were prominent features of the Mediterranean-type dietary pattern (with observed factor loadings of <0·30) which contrasts with the a priori-derived Mediterranean score. Nonetheless, we note that the Mediterranean-type diet, as defined in our analyses using whole-diet data, was characterised by other food loadings typical of a Mediterranean diet, including a variety of vegetables, fish, legumes and olive oil. Furthermore, we observed a trend for reduced mortality with increasing Mediterranean-type diet adherence, though this association was not robust to multivariate adjustment.

In this report, we also sought to examine the relationship between a less healthy, traditional diet – characterised in our sample by a high intake of meat and processed meats, pies, potatoes, tinned vegetables and milk puddings – and mortality. Though low adherence to either of the healthy diets could be considered a low-quality diet, many different foods are consumed in a traditional diet that are not assessed in the Mediterranean or MIND diets. Therefore, it is important to examine its impact. Even within the UK, the Scottish diet is of poorer quality than in England or Wales in terms of salt, fat content, dietary cholesterol and fruit and vegetable consumption(63). However, the consumption of processed foods is increasing worldwide, and in countries where the overall consumption of plant-based foods is higher, eating processed foods of low nutritional quality is associated with an increased hazard for total mortality(Reference Schnabel, Kesse-Guyot and Allès64,Reference Rico-Campà, Martínez-González and Alvarez-Alvarez65) . In the SUN cohort study in Spain, high consumption of ‘ultra-processed’ foods (>4 servings daily) significantly increased mortality risk by 62 %(Reference Rico-Campà, Martínez-González and Alvarez-Alvarez65). In the present study, the attenuation of the association of the traditional (processed) Scottish diet and increased odds of mortality, by factors such as lower SES, less education and smoking, suggests that a low-quality/low-nutrient diet and premature death are likely connected via sociodemographic pathways across the lifecourse(Reference Chrysohoou and Stefanadis66). The traditional diet pattern and mortality association was also attenuated by baseline morbidity, particularly CVD and stroke.

Longevity is a multi-factorial phenomenon; a host of innate and environmental factors influences physiological ageing. In general, our study additionally highlights the strong contribution of smoking to the diet–mortality relation, an established, and dose-response, risk factor for premature death in people 60 years and older(Reference Gellert, Schöttker and Brenner67). Despite our observation of socio-economic variation in MIND diet scores – the highest adherence group had more education, a more professional occupation and other healthy lifestyle indicators, compared with those in the lowest group – it would appear that the association between the MIND diet and mortality is less likely due to confounding, given that our risk estimates were adjusted for a range of variables, known to correlate with both diet uptake and mortality. Furthermore, risk estimates were essentially unchanged after adjustment for health status, such as obesity, CVD and stroke, potential mediators of the diet-mortality association.

Study strengths and limitations

Some limitations of the study should be noted. One limitation involves the potential for measurement error and recall bias in FFQ. However, FFQ have been demonstrated to be the most appropriate method in assessing habitual diet in large cohorts(Reference Willett68). It is also important to note that a high Mediterranean-type diet score does not reflect closer adherence to a true Mediterranean diet per se, especially in a non-Mediterranean population. We were unable to assess cause-specific mortality limiting the conclusions drawn with regard to the influence of diet on, for example, risk of cardiovascular, cancer or other deaths. Given that we present results of an observational study, we cannot infer causation or rule out residual confounding. We note that, though randomised controlled trials have the potential to demonstrate that dietary patterns can significantly lower chronic disease risk factors or outcomes, such trials are expensive and onerous to participants; therefore, most evidence will come from observational studies(Reference McCullough69). Finally, the stability of diet over time was unknown, and we cannot discount the possibility that unmeasured confounding factors over time influenced associations between the MIND diet score and the reduced odds of early death.

Generalisability of our results may be limited due to geographical location. Scotland has its own unique diet, and therefore results may differ in other populations. Scotland also has a high rate of mortality especially from CVD, though there has been a significant decline in circulatory disease–mortality in recent years (by 68 % since 1994)(Reference Scotland70). Despite this, the LBC1936 cohort is a self-selected sample from a relatively affluent area, with an interest in participating in research, and is thus more likely to be healthier than the general population.

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to investigate the association between the MIND diet and mortality. Other strengths of the study include its prospective nature, the use of a well-phenotyped, narrow-age sample of older adults, a relatively long follow-up period and multiple measures of diet quality. We were able to control for several confounding variables including rarely available data on childhood IQ and father’s SES; early-life cognitive ability and SES are strongly related to diet choices and health outcomes in adulthood(Reference Dobson, Chow and Morrison71Reference Wraw, Der and Gale73). A validated and comprehensive FFQ was used to assess dietary exposure. In addition, we obtained official linkage registry data on mortality with high reliability.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study shows that closer adherence to the MIND diet was associated with a lower risk of all-cause mortality over 12 years, after controlling for a range of demographic, lifestyle and health variables. This finding suggests that diet, a modifiable lifestyle factor, has the potential to mitigate risk of premature death. Crucially, we demonstrate that the MIND diet may have wider health implications beyond neuroprotection. Given the limited evidence base for this association, further prospective population-based studies and clinical trials are required to replicate this finding.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The author thanks the LBC1936 participants and LBC research team who collected, entered and checked data used in this manuscript; staff at the Rowett Research Institute, University of Aberdeen, for FFQ data entry and assistance, and performing dietary pattern analyses; the Edinburgh Clinical Research Facility (ECRF) nursing staff. The author is grateful to Ian Deary for advice on the data analyses and results. Financial support: This work was supported by a Research Into Ageing programme grant and the Age UK-funded Disconnected Mind project; UK Medical Research Council (MRC; G0701120, G1001245, MR/M013111/1, MR/R024065/1); National Institutes of Health (R01AG054628) and the University of Edinburgh. The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: J.C. designed the study, analysed the data and wrote the article. Ethics of human subject participation: The LBC1936 study was approved by the Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee for Scotland (MREC/01/0/56) and the Lothian Research Ethics Committee (LREC/2003/2/29) for Wave 1 and the Scotland A Research Ethics Committee (07/MRE00/58) for waves 2–5. All research was carried out in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration. All participants provided written informed consent before testing.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980020002979

References

Trichopoulou, A & Benetou, V (2019) Impact of Mediterranean diet on longevity. In Centenarians, pp. 161168 [C Caruso, editor]. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkins, JL, Whincup, PH, Morris, RW et al. (2016) Dietary patterns and the risk of CVD and all-cause mortality in older British men. Br J Nutr 116, 12461255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, SC & Orsini, N (2013) Red meat and processed meat consumption and all-cause mortality: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol 179, 282289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Sullivan, TA, Hafekost, K, Mitrou, F et al. (2013) Food sources of saturated fat and the association with mortality: a meta-analysis. Am J Public Health 103, e31e42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sofi, F, Abbate, R, Gensini, GF et al. (2010) Accruing evidence on benefits of adherence to the Mediterranean diet on health: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 11891196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sofi, F, Macchi, C, Abbate, R et al. (2014) Mediterranean diet and health status: an updated meta-analysis and a proposal for a literature-based adherence score. Public Health Nutr 17, 27692782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soltani, S, Jayedi, A, Shab-Bidar, S et al. (2019) Adherence to the Mediterranean diet in relation to all-cause mortality: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Adv Nutr 10, 10291039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez-Gonzalez, MA, Guillen-Grima, F, de Irala, J et al. (2012) The Mediterranean diet is associated with a reduction in premature mortality among middle-aged adults. J Nutr 142, 16721678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trichopoulou, A, Kouris-Blazos, A, Wahlqvist, ML et al. (1995) Diet and overall survival in elderly people. BMJ 311, 14571460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonaccio, M, Di Castelnuovo, A, Costanzo, S et al. (2018) Mediterranean diet and mortality in the elderly: a prospective cohort study and a meta-analysis. Br J Nutr 120, 841854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, W, Ukawa, S, Okada, E et al. (2019) The associations of dietary patterns with all-cause mortality and other lifestyle factors in the elderly: an age-specific prospective cohort study. Clin Nutr 38, 288296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, MC, Tangney, CC, Wang, Y et al. (2015) MIND diet slows cognitive decline with aging. Alzheimers Dement 11, 10151022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loef, M & Walach, H (2012) Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Aging 16, 626630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosking, DE, Eramudugolla, R & Anstey, KJ (2017) The MIND diet is associated with reduced incidence of 12-year cognitive impairment in an Australian setting. Alzheimers Dement 13, P1429P1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, MC, Tangney, CC, Wang, Y et al. (2015) MIND diet associated with reduced incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 11, 10071014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agarwal, P, Wang, Y, Buchman, AS et al. (2018) MIND diet associated with reduced incidence and delayed progression of Parkinsonism in old age. J Nutr Health Aging 22, 12111215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fung, TT, Rexrode, KM, Mantzoros, CS et al. (2009) Mediterranean diet and incidence and mortality of coronary heart disease and stroke in women. Circulation 119, 10931100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salehi-Abargouei, A, Maghsoudi, Z, Shirani, F et al. (2013) Effects of Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH)-style diet on fatal or nonfatal cardiovascular diseases—incidence: a systematic review and meta-analysis on observational prospective studies. Nutrition 29, 6116118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scottish Council for Research in Education (1949) The Trend of Scottish Intelligence: A Comparison of the 1947 and 1932 Surveys of the Intelligence of Eleven-Year-Old Pupils. London, UK: University of London Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, AM, Pattie, A & Deary, IJ (2018) Cohort profile update: the Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936. Int J Epidemiol 47, 10421042r.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scottish Collaborative Group (2000) Scottish Collaborative Group Food Frequency Questionnaire. Available at https://www.foodfrequency.org/ (accessed November 2019).Google Scholar
Jia, X, Craig, LC, Aucott, LS et al. (2008) Repeatability and validity of a food frequency questionnaire in free-living older people in relation to cognitive function. J Nutr Health Aging 12, 735741.Google ScholarPubMed
Mõttus, R, McNeill, G, Jia, X et al. (2013) The associations between personality, diet and body mass index in older people. Health Psychol 32, 353360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panagiotakos, D (2008) α-Priori versus α-posterior methods in dietary pattern analysis: a review in nutrition epidemiology. Nutr Bull 33, 311315.Google Scholar
Schwedhelm, C, Iqbal, K, Knüppel, S et al. (2018) Contribution to the understanding of how principal component analysis–derived dietary patterns emerge from habitual data on food consumption. Am J Clin Nutr 107, 227235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Office of Population, Censuses and Surveys (1980) Classification of Occupations and Coding Index. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
General Register Office (1956) Census, 1951: Classification of Occupations. London.Google Scholar
Zigmond, AS & Snaith, RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiat Scand 67, 361370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardener, SL & Rainey-Smith, SR (2018) The role of nutrition in cognitive function and brain aging in the elderly. Curr Nutr Rep 7, 139149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Brink, AC, Brouwer-Brolsma, EM, Berendsen, AA et al. (2019) The Mediterranean, Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH), and Mediterranean-DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diets are associated with less cognitive decline and a lower risk of Alzheimer’s disease—a review. Adv Nutr 10, 10401065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aune, D, Giovannucci, E, Boffetta, P et al. (2017) Fruit and vegetable intake and the risk of cardiovascular disease, total cancer and all-cause mortality—a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Int J Epidemiol 46, 10291056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwingshackl, L, Schwedhelm, C, Hoffmann, G et al. (2017) Food groups and risk of all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Clin Nutr 105, 14621473.Google ScholarPubMed
Sinha, R, Cross, AJ, Graubard, BI et al. (2009) Meat intake and mortality: a prospective study of over half a million people. Arch Intern Med 169, 562571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X, Lin, X, Ouyang, YY et al. (2016) Red and processed meat consumption and mortality: dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Public Health Nutr 19, 893905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y, Zhao, LG, Wu, QJ et al. (2015) Association between dietary fiber and lower risk of all-cause mortality: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Am J Epidemiol 181, 8391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, MC (2016) Nutrition and risk of dementia: overview and methodological issues. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1367, 137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hjartåker, A, Knudsen, MD, Tretli, S et al. (2015) Consumption of berries, fruits and vegetables and mortality among 10,000 Norwegian men followed for four decades. Eur J Nutr 54, 599608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iso, H & Kubota, Y (2007) Nutrition and disease in the Japan collaborative cohort study for evaluation of cancer (JACC). Asian Pac J Cancer Prev 8, 3580.Google Scholar
Takachi, R, Inoue, M, Ishihara, J et al. (2008) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of total cancer and cardiovascular disease: Japan public health center-based prospective study. Am J Epidemiol 167, 5970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamakoshi, A, Tamakoshi, K, Lin, Y et al. (2009) Healthy lifestyle and preventable death: findings from the Japan Collaborative Cohort (JACC) study. Prev Med 48, 486492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlsen, MH, Halvorsen, BL, Holte, K et al. (2010) The total antioxidant content of more than 3100 foods, beverages, spices, herbs and supplements used worldwide. Nutr J 9, 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genkinger, JM, Platz, EA, Hoffman, SC et al. (2004) Fruit, vegetable, and antioxidant intake and all-cause, cancer, and cardiovascular disease mortality in a community-dwelling population in Washington County, Maryland. Am J Epidemiol 160, 12231233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunathilake, KD, Ranaweera, KK & Rupasinghe, HP (2018) In vitro anti-inflammatory properties of selected Green Leafy vegetables. Biomedicines 6, E107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, SV, Edirisinghe, I & Burton-Freeman, BM (2014) Berries: anti-inflammatory effects in humans. J Agric Food Chem 62, 38863903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, MC, Wang, Y, Barnes, LL et al. (2018) Nutrients and bioactives in green leafy vegetables and cognitive decline: prospective study. Neurology 90, e214e22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tufts, HR, Harris, CS, Bukania, ZN et al. (2015) Antioxidant and anti-inflammatory activities of Kenyan leafy green vegetables, wild fruits, and medicinal plants with potential relevance for kwashiorkor. Evid-Based Compl Alt Med 2015, 807158.Google ScholarPubMed
Esposito, K & Giugliano, D (2005) Diet and inflammation: a link to metabolic and cardiovascular diseases. Eur Heart J 27, 1520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrington, WE & White, E (2016) Mortality outcomes associated with intake of fast-food items and sugar-sweetened drinks among older adults in the Vitamins and Lifestyle (VITAL) study. Public Health Nutr 19, 33193326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menotti, A, Kromhout, D, Puddu, PE et al. (2017) Baseline fatty acids, food groups, a diet score and 50-year all-cause mortality rates. An ecological analysis of the seven countries study. Ann Med 49, 718727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellavia, A, Tektonidis, TG, Orsini, N et al. (2016) Quantifying the benefits of Mediterranean diet in terms of survival. Eur J Epidemiol 31, 527530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckland, G, Agudo, A, Travier, N et al. (2011) Adherence to the Mediterranean diet reduces mortality in the Spanish cohort of the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC-Spain). Br J Nutr 106, 15811591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoops, KT, Fidanza, F, Alberti-Fidanza, A et al. (2006) Comparison of three different dietary scores in relation to 10-year mortality in elderly European subjects: the HALE project. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 746755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kouris-Blazos, A, Gnardellis, C, Wahlqvist, ML et al. (1999) Are the advantages of the Mediterranean diet transferable to other populations? A cohort study in Melbourne, Australia. Br J Nutr 82, 5761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lagiou, P, Trichopoulos, D, Sandin, S et al. (2006) Mediterranean dietary pattern and mortality among young women: a cohort study in Sweden. Br J Nutr 96, 384392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinelli, F, Yannakoulia, M, Anastasiou, CA et al. (2015) Mediterranean diet and other lifestyle factors in relation to 20-year all-cause mortality: a cohort study in an Italian population. Br J Nutr 113, 10031011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tong, TY, Wareham, NJ, Khaw, KT et al. (2016) Prospective association of the Mediterranean diet with cardiovascular disease incidence and mortality and its population impact in a non-Mediterranean population: the EPIC-Norfolk study. BMC Med 14, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trichopoulou, A, Costacou, T, Bamia, C et al. (2003) Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and survival in a Greek population. New Eng J Med 348, 25992608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trichopoulou, A, Orfanos, P, Norat, T et al. (2005) Modified Mediterranean diet and survival: EPIC-elderly prospective cohort study. BMJ 330, 991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, SA, Bates, CJ & Mishra, GD (2011) Diet quality is associated with all-cause mortality in adults aged 65 years and older. J Nutr 142, 320325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjögren, P, Becker, W, Warensjö, E et al. (2010) Mediterranean and carbohydrate-restricted diets and mortality among elderly men: a cohort study in Sweden. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 967974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, FB (2002) Dietary pattern analysis: a new direction in nutritional epidemiology. Curr Opin Lipidol 13, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bountziouka, V, Tzavelas, G, Polychronopoulos, E et al. (2011) Validity of dietary patterns derived in nutrition surveys using a priori and a posteriori multivariate statistical methods. Int J Food Sci Nutr 62, 617627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Office for National Statistics (2002) The National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Adults Aged 19 to 64 Years. London: Office for National Statistics.Google Scholar
Schnabel, L, Kesse-Guyot, E, Allès, B et al. (2019) Association between ultraprocessed food consumption and risk of mortality among middle-aged adults in France. JAMA Intern Med 179, 490498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rico-Campà, A, Martínez-González, MA, Alvarez-Alvarez, I et al. (2019) Association between consumption of ultra-processed foods and all cause mortality: SUN prospective cohort study. BMJ 365, l1949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chrysohoou, C & Stefanadis, C (2013) Longevity and diet. Myth or pragmatism? Maturitas 76, 303307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gellert, C, Schöttker, B & Brenner, H (2012) Smoking and all-cause mortality in older people: systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Intern Med 172, 837844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willett, WC (2013) Nutritional Epidemiology, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McCullough, ML (2014) Diet patterns and mortality: common threads and consistent results. J Nutr 144, 795796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scotland, NRS (2019) Under 75, age-standardised death rates for all causes and certain selected causes, Scotland, 1994 to 2018. https://www.nrscotland.gov.uk/files//statistics/age-standardised-death-rates-esp/2018/age-standard-death-rates-18-main-points.pdf (accessed November 2019).Google Scholar
Dobson, KG, Chow, CH, Morrison, KM et al. (2017) Associations between childhood cognition and cardiovascular events in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Can J Cardiol 33, 232242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puolakka, E, Pahkala, K, Laitinen, TT et al. (2018) Childhood socioeconomic status and lifetime health behaviors: the Young Finns Study. Int J Cardiol 258, 289294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wraw, C, Der, G, Gale, CR et al. (2018) Intelligence in youth and health behaviours in middle age. Intelligence 69, 7186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of participants (n 882) according to mortality status, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

Figure 1

Table 2 Associations between baseline (2004–2007) characteristics and tertiles of the MIND, Mediterranean-type and traditional diet scores, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

Figure 2

Table 3 Associations between diet scores* and all-cause mortality over 12 years of follow-up, Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study

Supplementary material: File

Corley supplementary material

Corley supplementary material

Download Corley supplementary material(File)
File 16.7 KB