We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
Review of Kelly S. Mix, Linda B. Smith and Michael Gasser (eds.). The spatial foundations of language and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2010, xviii + 307 pp., ISBN 978-0-19-955324-2.
Published online by Cambridge University Press:
11 March 2014
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Baldwin, D. A.. 1993. Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology29. 832–843.Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P. K. & Rao, R. P. N.. 1997. Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences20. 723–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L.2001. Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology43. 1–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V.. 1997. Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition64. 1–37.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Marquez, M., Mittelberg, I., Coulson, S. & Spivey, M. J. (eds.) 2007. Methods in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.1987. The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D.1995. The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence73. 31–68.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G.1987. Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Núñez, R.. 2000. Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W.. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, volume 1: Theoreticalpre-requisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Moll, H. & Meltzoff, A. N.. 2011. Perspective-taking and its foundation in joint attention. In Eilan, N., Lerman, H. & Roessler, J. (eds.), Perception, causation, and objectivity. Issues inphilosophy and psychology, 286–304. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B.. 1956. The child's conception of space. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Talmy, L.2000. Towards a cognitive semantics, volume 1: Conceptual structuring systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, A.2007. Construal and perspectivization. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics, 48–81. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Winter, B. & Bergen, B. K.. 2012. Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily: Evidence for the immersed experiencer view. Language and Cognition4. 1–16.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A.. 2004. The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, 35–62. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar