Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Partial productivity of linguistic constructions: Dynamic categorization and statistical preemption

  • ADELE E. GOLDBERG (a1)

Abstract

Grammatical constructions are typically partially but not fully productive, which leads to a conundrum for the learner. When can a construction be extended for use with new words and when can it not? The solution suggested here relies on two complementary processes. The first is dynamic categorization: as learners record the statistics of their language, they implicitly categorize the input on the basis of form and function. On the basis of this categorization process, general semantic and phonological constraints on productivity emerge, and productivity is to a large extent determined by the degree to which the category is well attested by similar exemplars. Occasionally, a semantically sensical and phonologically well-formed instance of a well-attested construction is simply not fully acceptable. It is suggested that a process of statistical preemption is at work in these cases: learners avoid using a construction if an alternative formulation has been systematically witnessed instead. The mechanism proposed for statistical preemption is competition-driven learning: when two competitors are activated but one reliably wins, the loser becomes less accessible over time. In this way, the paradox of partial productivity can be resolved.

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Partial productivity of linguistic constructions: Dynamic categorization and statistical preemption
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Partial productivity of linguistic constructions: Dynamic categorization and statistical preemption
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Partial productivity of linguistic constructions: Dynamic categorization and statistical preemption
      Available formats
      ×

Copyright

Corresponding author

e-mail: adele@princeton.edu

References

Hide All
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. (2015). Explanatory adequacy is not enough: response to commentators on ‘Child language acquisition: why universal grammar doesn’t help’. Language, 91(3), e116e126.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2012). Semantics versus statistics in the retreat from locative overgeneralization errors. Cognition, 123(2), 260279.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., & Chang, F. (2012). The roles of verb semantics, entrenchment and morphophonology in the retreat from dative argument structure overgeneralization errors. Language, 88(1), 4581.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., Jones, R. L., & Clark, V. (2009). A semantics-based approach to the ‘no negative evidence’ problem. Cognitive Science, 33(7), 13011316.
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., & Young, C. R. (2008). The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgements of argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Cognition, 106(1), 87129.
Anderson, M. C., Green, C., & McCulloch, K. C. (2000). Similarity and inhibition in long-term memory: evidence for a two-factor theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 11411159.
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102(1), 68100.
Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J. G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eyetracking. Cognition, 76(1), 1326.
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar (Linguistic Inquiry Monograph 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Artola, A., Brocher, S., & Singer, W. (1990). Different voltage-dependent thresholds for inducing long-term depression and long-term potentiation in slices of rat visual cortex. Nature, 347, 6972.
Baker, C. L. (1979). Syntactic theory and the projection problem. Linguistic Inquiry, 10(4), 533581.
Barðdal, J. (2008). Productivity: evidence from case and argument structure in Icelandic. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bolinger, D. (1977). Meaning and form. London: Longman.
Bowerman, M. (1988). The ‘no negative evidence’ problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In Hawkins, J. (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 73101). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Bowerman, M. (1996). Argument structure and learnability: Is a solution in sight? In Johnson, J., Juge, M. L., & Moxley, J. L. (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-second annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: general session and parasession on the role of learnability in grammatical theory (pp. 454468). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2001). Shaping meaning for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2003). Space under construction: language-specific spatial categorization in first language acquisition. In Language in mind: advances in the study of language and cognition (pp. 387428). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Boyd, J. K., & Goldberg, A. E. (2009). Input effects within a constructionist framework. Modern Language Journal, 93(iii), 418429.
Boyd, J. K., & Goldberg, A. E. (2011). Learning what not to say: the role of statistical preemption and categorization in a-adjective production. Language, 87(1), 5583.
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). The acquisition of language in infant and child. In Reed, C. (Ed.), The learning of language (pp. 795). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Braine, M. D. S., & Brooks, P. J. (1995). Verb argument structure and the problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In Tomasello, M. & Merriman, W. E. (Eds.), Beyond names for things: young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 353376). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, R. H. (2007). Predicting the dative alternation. In Bouma, G., Kraemer, I., & Zwarts, J. (Eds.), Cognitive foundations for interpretation (pp. 6994). Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Science.
Bresnan, J., & Ford, M. (2010). Predicting syntax: processing dative constructions in American and Australian varieties of English. Language, 86(1), 186213.
Bresnan, J., & Hay, J. (2008). Gradient grammar: an effect of animacy on the syntax of give in New Zealand and American English. Lingua, 118(2), 245259.
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75(4), 720738.
Brooks, P. J., and Zizak, O. (2002) Does preemption help children learn verb transitivity? Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 759781.
Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R. (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.
Bybee, J. (1985). Morphology: a study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing.
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10(5), 425455.
Bybee, J. (2010) Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bybee, J., & Eddington, D. (2006). A usage-based approach to Spanish verbs of ‘becoming’. Language, 82(2), 323355.
Casenhiser, D., & Goldberg, A. E. (2005). Fast mapping between a phrasal form and meaning. Developmental Science, 8(6), 500508.
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 133). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clausner, T. C., & Croft, W. (1997). Productivity and schematicity in metaphors. Cognitive Science, 21(3), 247282.
Collins, P. (1995). The indirect object construction in English: an informational approach. Linguistics, 33(1), 3549.
Coppock, E. (2008). The logical and empirical foundations of Baker’s paradox. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Davies, M. (2008a). The Corpus of Contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990–present. Online: <http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/>.
Davies, M. (2008b). The Corpus of Historical American English: 400 million words, 1810–2009. Online: <http://corpus.byu.edu/coha/ >.
Desagulier, G. (2015). A lesson from associative learning: asymmetry and productivity in multiple-slot constructions. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, online: <doi:10.1515/cllt-2015-0012>.
Dryer, M. S. (1986). Primary objects, secondary objects, and antidative. Language, 62, 808845.
Ellis, N. C., & Ferreira-Junior, F. (2009). Constructions and their acquisition: islands and the distinctiveness of their occupancy. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 187220.
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1979). Discourse constraints on dative movement. In Laberge, S. & Sankoff, G. (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (pp. 441467). New York: Academic Press.
Foraker, Stephani, Regier, Terry, Khetarpal, Naveen, Perfors, Amy, & Tenenbaum, Joshua B. (2007). Indirect evidence and the poverty of the stimulus: the case of anaphoric one . In McNamara, D. S. & Trafton, J. G. (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 275280). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Givón, T. (1979). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.
Givón, T. (1984). Syntax: A functional-typological introduction. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Goldberg, A. E. (1993). Another look at some learnability paradoxes. Paper presented to the Proceedings of the 25th Annual Stanford Child Language Research Forum, Stanford, 1993.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: a construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (2011a). Corpus evidence of the viability of statistical preemption. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(1), 131153.
Goldberg, A. E. (2011b). Are a-adjectives like afraid prepositional phrases underlying and does it matter from a learnability perspective? Unpublished ms, Princeton University.
Goldberg, A. E., and Ambridge, Ben (forthcoming). Explain me this. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Goldberg, A. E., & Boyd, J. K. (2015). A-adjectives, statistical preemption, and the evidence: reply to Yang (2015). Language, e184e197.
Goldberg, A. E., Casenhiser, D., & Sethuraman, N. (2004). Learning argument structure generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics, 15, 289316.
Green, G. M. (1974). Semantics and syntactic regularity. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., & Goldberg, R. (1991). Syntax and semantics in the acquisition of locative verbs. Journal of Child Language, 18, 115151.
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., & Wilson, R. (1989). The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language, 65(2), 203257.
Hilpert, M. (2013). Constructional change in English: developments in allomorphy, word formation, and syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Janda, R. D. (1990). Frequency, markedness and morphological change: on predicting the spread of noun-plural -s in Modern High Germanic and West Germanic. Proceedings of the 7th Eastern States Conference on Linguistics, 136153. Online: <http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED333749.pdf#page=145>.
Johnson, M. A., Turk-Browne, N., & Goldberg, A. E. (2013). Prediction plays a key role in language development as well as processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 360361.
Kalyan, S. (2012). Similarity in linguistic categorization: the importance of necessary properties. Cognitive Linguistics, 23(3), 539554.
Kim, G., Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Norman, K. A., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2014). Pruning of memories by context-based prediction error. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(24), 89979002.
Kiparsky, P. (1993). Blocking in non-derived environments. In Hargus, S. & Kaisse, Ellen (Eds.), Phonetics and Phonology 4: Studies in Lexical Phonology (pp. 277313). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category inference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.
Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11850–1857.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161163.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire and dangerous things. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Landauer, T. K. (2006). Latent semantic analysis. Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Online: <http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/0470018860.s00561/abstract>.
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: theoretical prerequisites (Vol. 1). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Madlener, Karin. (2015). Frequency effects in instructed second language acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Marcotte, J. (2005). Causative alternation errors in child language acquisition. Unpublished PhD thesis, Stanford.
Marcus, G. F. (1993). Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition, 46, 5385.
McDonough, K., & Nekrasova-Becker, T. (2014). Comparing the effect of skewed and balanced input on English as a foreign language learners’ comprehension of the double-object dative construction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 419442.
McRae, K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 283312.
Newman, E. L., & Norman, K. A. (2010). Moderate excitation leads to weakening of perceptual representations. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 27602770.
Norman, K., Newman, E. L., & Detre, G. (2007). A neural network model of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Review, 114(4), 887953.
Oehrle, R. (1975). The grammatical status of the English dative alternation. Unpublished PhD dissertation, MIT.
Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., Lopez, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97(2), 185200.
Perek, F. (2016). Using distributional semantics to study syntactic productivity in diachrony: a case study. Linguistics, 54(1), 149188.
Perek, F., & Goldberg, A. E. (2015). Generalizing beyond the input: the functions of the constructions matter. Journal of Memory and Language, 84, 108127.
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105110.
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329347.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rainer, S. (1988). A short story of down. In Hüllen, W., & Schulze, R. (Eds.), Understanding the lexicon: meaning sense and world knowledge in lexical semantics (pp. 394410). Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Reeder, P, Newport, Elissa, & Aslin, Richard N. (2013). From shared contexts to syntactic categories: the role of distributional information in learning linguistic form-classes. Cognitive Psychology, 66(1), 3054.
Robenalt, C., & Goldberg, A. E. (2015). Judgment and frequency evidence for statistical preemption: it is relatively better to vanish than to disappear a rabbit, but a lifeguard can equally well backstroke or swim children to shore. Cognitive Linguistics, 26(3), 467503.
Robenalt, C., & Goldberg, A. E. (2016). Nonnative speakers do not take competing alternative expressions into account the way native speakers do. Language Learning, 66(1), 6093.
Stefanowitsch, A. (2008). Negative entrenchment: a usage-based approach to negative evidence. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(3), 513531.
Stephens, G. J., Silbert, L. J., & Hasson, U. (2010). Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(32), 1442514430.
Storm, B. C., & Levy, B. J. (2012). A progress report on the inhibitory account of retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 40(6), 827843.
Suttle, L., & Goldberg, A. E. (2011). The partial productivity of constructions as induction. Linguistics, 49(6), 12371269.
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 16321634.
Taylor, J. R. (2003). Linguistic categorization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Theakston, A. L. (2004). The role of entrenchment in constraining children’s verb argument structure overgeneralisations: a grammatical judgment study. Cognitive Development, 19, 1534.
Thompson, S. A. (1990). Information flow and ‘dative shift’ in English. In Edmondson, J., Feagin, K., & Mühlhäusler, P. (Eds.), Development and diversity: linguistic variation across time and space (pp. 239253). Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Thompson, S. A. (1995). The iconicity of ‘dative shift’ in English: considerations from information flow in discourse. In Landsberg, M. E. (Ed.), Syntactic iconicity and linguistic freezes: the human dimension (Studies in Anthropological Linguistics 9) (pp. 155175). Berlin / New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wasow, T. (2002). Postverbal behavior. Stanford, CA: CSLI.
Wonnacott, E., Boyd, J. K., Thompson, J., & Goldberg, A. E. (2012). Novel construction learning in five year olds. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 458478.
Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review, 114(2), 245272.
Yang, Charles (2015). Negative knowledge from positive evidence. Language, 91(4), 938953.
Zeschel, A., & Bildhauer, F. (2009). Islands of acceptability. Paper presented at the AfLiCO, Paris.

Keywords

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed