Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Online processing of Verb–Argument Constructions: lexical decision and meaningfulness

  • NICK C. ELLIS (a1)

Abstract

This paper discusses how patterns of construction usage, implicitly learned over lifelong experience, tune the language processing system for fluent interactive lexical, syntactic, and semantic access. It reports on three experiments that investigate online processing of Verb–Argument Constructions (VACs) and the degree to which this is effected by (i) verb frequency in the language, (ii) verb frequency in the VAC, (iii) VAC-verb contingency, and (iv) verb prototypicality in terms of centrality within the VAC semantic network. Experiment 1 tested lexical decision of VAC exemplars presented as successive verb–preposition pairs. Experiment 2 tested lexical decision of VAC exemplars presented as arbitrarily interrupted verb–adverb–preposition pairs. Experiment 3 had participants judge whether two-word utterances were meaningful or not. All of the experiments show effects of Verb Frequency and Verb-VAC frequency: learners have rich implicit statistical knowledge of verb-VAC type–token frequency that guides processing. Lexical decision is additionally driven by semantic prototypicality (but not VAC-verb contingency ΔPcw), whereas meaning judgment is affected by VAC-verb contingency ΔPcw (but not semantic prototypicality). These findings, I argue, index the spreading activation of unconscious meaning representation in lexical decision in comparison to the election of a unitary interpretation in conscious comprehension. I conclude that speeded automatic VAC processing involves rich associations, tuned by verb type and token frequencies, their contingencies of usage, and their histories of prototypical and specific interpretations which interface syntax, lexis, and semantics.

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Online processing of Verb–Argument Constructions: lexical decision and meaningfulness
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Online processing of Verb–Argument Constructions: lexical decision and meaningfulness
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Online processing of Verb–Argument Constructions: lexical decision and meaningfulness
      Available formats
      ×

Copyright

Corresponding author

e-mail: ncellis@umich.edu

References

Hide All
Allan, L. G. (1980). A note on measurement of contingency between two binary variables in judgment tasks. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 147149.
Ambridge, B., & Lieven, E. (2011). Child language acquisition: contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ambridge, B., & Rowland, C. F. (2013). Experimental methods in studying child language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(2), 149168.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 22(3), 261295.
Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 6782.
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theatre of consciousness: Global Workspace Theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4, 292309.
Balota, D., Cortese, M., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 283316.
Balota, D., Ferraro, F., & Connor, L. (1991). On the early influence of meaning in word recognition: a review of the literature. In Schwanenflugel, P. J. (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 187222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., & Cortese, M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: the journey from features to meaning (a travel update). In Traxler, M. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, 2nd ed. (pp. 285375). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Ad hoc categories. In Hogan, P. C. (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of the language sciences (pp. 8788). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148.
Beckner, C., Blythe, R., Bybee, J., Christiansen, M. H., Croft, W., Ellis, N. C., … Schoenemann, T. (2009). Language is a complex adaptive system. Position paper. Language Learning, 59 Supplement 1, 126.
Biber, D., & Reppen, D. (Eds.) (2015). The Cambridge handbook of English corpus linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
BNC. (2007). BNC XML Edition. Online: <http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/corpus/>.
Bod, R. (2001). Sentence memory: storage vs. computation of frequent sentences. Paper presented at the CUNY-2001, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
Clark, E. V. (1978). Discovering what words can do. In Farkas, D., Jacobsen, W. M., & Todrys, K. W. (Eds.), Papers from the parasession on the lexicon, Chicago Linguistics Society April 14–15, 1978 (pp. 3457). Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.
Cortese, M., & Balota, D. (2012). Visual word recognition in skilled adult readers. In Spivey, Michael J., McRae, Ken, & Joanisse, Marc F. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 159185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
de Nooy, W., Mrvar, A., & Batagelj, V. (2010). Exploratory social network analysis with Pajek. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York: Penguin/Random House.
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (2004). Neural mechanisms for access to consciousness. In Gazzaniga, M. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences, 3rd ed. (pp. 11451158). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., & Changeaux, J.-P. (2003). A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 100, 85208525.
Durrant, P., & Doherty, A. (2010). Are high-frequency collocations psychologically real? Investigating the thesis of collocational priming. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 6, 125155.
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: a review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143188.
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 124.
Ellis, N. C. (2012). Formulaic language and second language acquisition: Zipf and the phrasal teddy bear. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 1744.
Ellis, N. C. (2016). On-line processing of Verb–Argument Constructions: visual recognition thresholds and naming. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 14(1), 105135.
Ellis, N. C., & Ferreira-Junior, F. (2009). Constructions and their acquisition: islands and the distinctiveness of their occupancy. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 111139.
Ellis, N. C., Frey, E., & Jalkanen, I. (2009). The psycholinguistic reality of collocation and semantic prosody (1): lexical access. In Römer, U. & Schulze, R. (Eds.), Exploring the lexis–grammar interface (pp. 89114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ellis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (Eds.) (2006). Language emergence: implications for applied linguistics. Special Issue of Applied Linguistics, 27(4).
Ellis, N. C., & O’Donnell, M. B. (2011). Robust language acquisition – an emergent consequence of language as a complex adaptive system. In Carlson, L., Hölscher, C., & Shipley, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 35123517). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Ellis, N. C., & O’Donnell, M. B. (2012). Statistical construction learning: Does a Zipfian problem space ensure robust language learning? In Rebuschat, J. & Williams, J. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 265304). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Ellis, N. C., O’Donnell, M. B., & Römer, U. (2012). Usage-based language: investigating the latent structures that underpin acquisition. Currents in Language Learning, 1, 2551.
Ellis, N. C., O’Donnell, M. B., & Römer, U. (2014). The processing of Verb–Argument constructions is sensitive to form, function, frequency, contingency, and prototypicality. Cognitive Linguistics, 25, 5598.
Ellis, N. C., & Ogden, D. C. (2015). Language cognition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 182186.
Ellis, N. C., Römer, U., & O’Donnell, M. B. (2016). Usage-based approaches to language acquisition and processing: cognitive and corpus investigations of construction grammar. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 127.
Francis, G., Hunston, S., & Manning, E. (Eds.) (1996). Grammar patterns 1: verbs (The COBUILD Series). London: Harper Collins.
Freeman, L. (1977). A set of measures of centrality based upon betweenness. Sociometry, 40, 3541.
Gaskell, G. (Ed.) (2007). Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gass, S., & Mackey, A. (2011). Handbook of second language acquisition. London: Routledge / Taylor Francis.
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1994). A handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: a construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Granger, S., Gilquin, G., & Meunier, F. (Eds.) (2015). The Cambridge handbook of learner corpus research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 427.
Gries, S. Th., & Ellis, N. C. (2015). Statistical measures for usage-based linguistics. Currents in Language Learning, 2, 228255.
Harnad, S. (Ed.) (1987). Categorical perception: the groundwork of cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: a neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts and Company.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Marcel, A. J. (1980). Conscious and preconscious recognition of polysemous words: locating the selective effects of prior verbal context. In Nickerson, Raymond S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 435457). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197237.
Masson, M. E. J. (1991). A distributed memory model of context effects in word identification. In Besner, D. & Humphreus, G. W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: visual word recognition (pp. 233263). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375407.
McDonald, S. A., & Shillcock, R. C. (2003). Eye movements reveal the on-line computation of lexical probabilities during reading. Psychological Science, 14(6), 648652.
McDonough, K., & De Vleeschauwer, J. (2012). Prompt type frequency, auditory pattern discrimination, and EFL learners’ production of wh-questions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 355377.
McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: perspectives from memory and word recognition: Hove: Psychology Press.
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227234.
Miller, G. A. (2009). WordNet – about us. Online: <http://wordnet.princeton.edu> (last accessed 1 March 2010).
Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: a selective review of current findings and theories. In Besner, D. & Humphreus, G. W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: visual word recognition (pp. 264336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ninio, A. (1999). Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal of Child Language, 26, 619653.
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy–psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1), 813.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.
Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534 nonwords: the ARC Nonword Database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 13391362.
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (Eds.) (2012). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 15.
Römer, U., O’Donnell, M. B., & Ellis, N. C. (2015). Using COBUILD grammar patterns for a large-scale analysis of verb–argument constructions: exploring corpus data and speaker knowledge. In Charles, M., Groom, N., & John, S. (Eds.), Corpora, grammar, text and discourse: in honour of Susan Hunston (pp. 4371). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192233.
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.
Rubenstein, H., & Pollack, I. (1963). Word predictability and intelligibility. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2(2), 147158.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: II. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89(1), 6094.
Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(1), 117.
Schvaneveldt, R. W., & Meyer, D. E. (1973). Retrieval and comparison processes in semantic memory. Attention and Performance, IV, 395409.
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523568.
Shanks, D. R. (1995). The psychology of associative learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29(1), 4178.
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31, 6199.
Toglia, M. P., & Battig, W. F. (1978). Handbook of semantic word norms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Traxler, M., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.) (2011). Handbook of psycholinguistics. New York: Academic Press.
Trousdale, G., & Hoffmann, T. (Eds.) (2013). Oxford handbook of Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Zipf, G. K. (1935). The psycho-biology of language: an introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Keywords

Type Description Title
PDF
Supplementary materials

Ellis supplementary material
Appendix A

 PDF (366 KB)
366 KB

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed