Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Getting the ball rolling: the cross-linguistic conceptualization of caused motion*

  • GUILLERMO MONTERO-MELIS (a1) and EMANUEL BYLUND (a2)

Abstract

Does the way we talk about events correspond to how we conceptualize them? Three experiments (N = 135) examined how Spanish and Swedish native speakers judge event similarity in the domain of caused motion (‘He rolled the tyre into the barn’). Spanish and Swedish motion descriptions regularly encode path (‘into’), but differ in how systematically they include manner information (‘roll’). We designed a similarity arrangement task which allowed participants to give varying weights to different dimensions when gauging event similarity. The three experiments progressively reduced the likelihood that speakers were using language to solve the task. We found that, as long as the use of language was possible (Experiments 1 and 2), Swedish speakers were more likely than Spanish speakers to base their similarity arrangements on object manner (rolling/sliding). However, when recruitment of language was hindered through verbal interference, cross-linguistic differences disappeared (Experiment 3). A compound analysis of all experiments further showed that (i) cross-linguistic differences were played out against a backdrop of commonly represented event components, and (ii) describing vs. not describing the events did not augment cross-linguistic differences, but instead had similar effects across languages. We interpret these findings as suggesting a dynamic role of language in event conceptualization.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Guillermo Montero-Melis, Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Stockholm University, SE-106 91 Stockholm. e-mail: montero-melis@biling.su.se

Footnotes

Hide All
*

We thank Maya Hickmann and Henriette Hendriks for sharing their stimuli, Johannes Bjerva and Jaime Melis for programming help, and Maria Donoso and Arantxa Hurtado for data collection. We are grateful to Florian Jaeger, Guillaume Thierry, and an anonymous reviewer for valuable feedback on a previous version of this paper. Any remaining errors are entirely our own. The work for this paper was supported by the Swedish Research Council (grant no. 2015-01317).

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Athanasopoulos, P., & Bylund, E. (2013). Does grammatical aspect affect motion event cognition? A cross-linguistic comparison of English and Swedish speakers. Cognitive Science, 37(2), 286309.
Athanasopoulos, P., Bylund, E., Montero-Melis, G., Damjanovic, L., Schartner, A., Kibbe, A., … Thierry, G. (2015). Two languages, two minds: flexible cognitive processing driven by language of operation. Psychological Science, 26(4), 518526.
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390412.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255278.
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 211227.
Bates, D. M., Maechler, D., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (Version R package version 1.1-7). Online: <http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4>.
Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2014). Language and thought in a multilingual context: the case of isiXhosa. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 431441.
Bylund, E., Athanasopoulos, P., & Oostendorp, M. (2013). Motion event cognition and grammatical aspect: evidence from Afrikaans. Linguistics, 51, 929955.
Cadierno, T., & Ruiz, L. (2006). Motion events in Spanish L2 acquisition. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4(1), 183216.
Casasanto, D., & Lupyan, G. (2015). All concepts are ad hoc concepts. In Margolis, E. & Laurence, S. (Eds.), The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts (pp. 543566). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Choi, S., & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41(1/3), 83121.
Flecken, M., Gerwien, J., Carroll, M., & von Stutterheim, C. (2015). Analyzing gaze allocation during language planning: a cross-linguistic study on dynamic events. Language and Cognition, 7(1), 138166.
Flecken, M., von Stutterheim, C., & Carroll, M. (2014). Grammatical aspect influences motion event perception: findings from a cross- linguistic non-verbal recognition task. Language and Cognition, 6(1), 4578.
Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (n.d.). The garden of forking paths: why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no ‘fishing expedition’ or ‘p-hacking’ and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. Online: <http://www.stat.columbia.edu/∼gelman/research/unpublished/>.
Gelman, A., & Su, Y.-S. (2014). arm: data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models (Version R package version 1.7-07). Online: <http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=arm>.
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83(1), 4979.
Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2012). New perspectives on language and thought. In Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 543568). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). An efficient method for obtaining similarity data. Behavior Research Methods, 26(4), 381386.
Hendriks, H., Hickmann, M., & Demagny, A.-C. (2008). How adult English learners of French express caused motion: a comparison with English and French natives. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 27, 1541.
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (2010). Typological constraints on the acquisition of spatial language in French and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 21(2), 189215.
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434446.
Ji, Y., Hendriks, H., & Hickmann, M. (2011). How children express caused motion events in Chinese and English: universal and language-specific influences. Lingua, 121(12), 17961819.
Kersten, A. W., Meissner, C. A., Lechuga, J., Schwartz, B. L., Albrechtsen, J. S., & Iglesias, A. (2010). English speakers attend more strongly than Spanish speakers to manner of motion when classifying novel objects and events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 638653.
Lai, V. T., Garrido Rodriguez, G., & Narasimhan, B. (2014). Thinking-for-speaking in early and late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 139152.
Loucks, J., & Pederson, E. (2011). Linguistic and non-linguistic categorization of complex motion events. In Bohnemeyer, J. & Pederson, E. (Eds.), Event representation in language and cognition (Vol. 11, pp. 108133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 54. Online: <http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054>.
Lupyan, G., & Clark, A. (2015). Words and the World Predictive Coding and the Language-Perception-Cognition Interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 279284.
Malt, B. C., Ameel, E., Imai, M., Gennari, S. P., Saji, N., & Majid, A. (2014). Human locomotion in languages: constraints on moving and meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 74, 107123.
Papafragou, A. (2008). Space and the language-cognition interface. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. P. (Eds.), The innate mind. Volume 3: foundations and the future (pp. 272289). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155184.
Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: the representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84(2), 189219.
Papafragou, A., & Selimis, S. (2010). Event categorisation and language: a cross-linguistic study of motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 224260.
R Development Core Team. (2013). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Online: <http://www.R-project.org/>.
Ragnarsdóttir, H., & Strömqvist, S. (2004). Time, space, and manner in Swedish and Icelandic: narrative construction in two closely related languages. In Strömqvist, S. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative. Vol. 2: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 113141). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Regier, T., & Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 439446.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
Slobin, D. I. (1996). From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking’. In Gumperz, J. J. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 7096). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 157191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Slobin, D. I., Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I., Kopecka, A., & Majid, A. (2014). Manners of human gait: a crosslinguistic event-naming study. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(4), 701741.
Stringer, D. (2005). Paths in first language acquisition: motion through space in English, French and Japanese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Durham.
Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, T. (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 57149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a cognitive semantics: concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Talmy, L. (2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics: typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Trueswell, J. C., & Papafragou, A. (2010). Perceiving and remembering events cross-linguistically: evidence from dual-task paradigms. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(1), 6482.
von Stutterheim, C., Andermann, M., Carroll, M., Flecken, M., & Schmiedtová, B. (2012). How grammaticized concepts shape event conceptualization in language production: insights from linguistic analysis, eye tracking data, and memory performance. Linguistics, 50(4), 833867.
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: selected writings, ed. Carroll, J. B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wolff, P. (2003). Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events. Cognition, 88(1), 148.
Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253265.
Wolff, P., Jeon, Ga-hyun, & Li, Yu (2009). Causers in English, Korean, and Chinese and the individuation of events. Language and Cognition, 1(2), 167196.

Keywords

Type Description Title
PDF
Supplementary materials

Montero-Melis and Bylund supplementary material
Montero-Melis and Bylund supplementary material 1

 PDF (518 KB)
518 KB

Getting the ball rolling: the cross-linguistic conceptualization of caused motion*

  • GUILLERMO MONTERO-MELIS (a1) and EMANUEL BYLUND (a2)

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed