Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Allocentric Versus Egocentric Neglect in Stroke Patients: A Pilot Study Investigating the Assessment of Neglect Subtypes and Their Impacts on Functional Outcome Using Eye Tracking

  • Jennifer N. Upshaw (a1), Damian W. Leitner (a1), Barbara J. Rutherford (a1), Harry B.D. Miller (a1) and Maya R. Libben (a1)...

Abstract

Objective: Few studies have investigated the assessment and functional impact of egocentric and allocentric neglect among stroke patients. This pilot study aimed to determine (1) whether allocentric and egocentric neglect could be dissociated among a sample of stroke patients using eye tracking; (2) the specific patterns of attention associated with each subtype; and (3) the nature of the relationship between neglect subtype and functional outcome. Method: Twenty acute stroke patients were administered neuropsychological assessment batteries, a pencil-and-paper Apples Test to measure neglect subtype, and an adaptation of the Apples Test with an eye tracking measure. To test clinical discriminability, twenty age- and education-matched control participants were administered the eye tracking measure of neglect. Results: The eye tracking measure identified a greater number of individuals as having egocentric and/or allocentric neglect than the pencil-and-paper Apples Test. Classification of neglect subtype based on eye tracking performance was a significant predictor of functional outcome beyond that accounted for by the neuropsychological test performance and Apples Test neglect classification. Preliminary evidence suggests that patients with no neglect symptoms had superior functional outcomes compared with patients with neglect. Patients with combined egocentric and allocentric neglect had poorer functional outcomes than those with either subtype. Functional outcomes of patients with either allocentric or egocentric neglect did not differ significantly. The applications of our findings, to improve neglect detection, are discussed. Conclusion: Results highlight the potential clinical utility of eye tracking for the assessment and identification of neglect subtype among stroke patients to predict functional outcomes. (JINS, 2019, 25, 479–489)

Copyright

Corresponding author

Correspondence and reprint requests to: Jennifer N. Upshaw, University of British Columbia Okanagan, Psychology Department, 3187 University Way, Kelowna, BC V1V 1V7. E-mail: jen.upshaw@alumni.ubc.ca

References

Hide All
Azouvi, P., Samuel, C., Louis-Dreyfus, A., Bernati, T., Bartolomeo, P., Beis, J.M., Chokron S., Leclercq M., Marchal F., Martin Y., & De Montety G. (2002). Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 73(2), 160166.
Bartolomeo, P. & Chokron, S. (2002). Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 217234.
Baylis, G.C., Baylis, L.L., & Gore, C.L. (2004). Visual neglect can be object-based or scene-based depending on task representation. Cortex, 40(2), 237246.
Bickerton, W.L., Samson, D., Williamson, J., & Humphreys, G.W. (2011). Separating forms of neglect using the Apples Test: validation and functional prediction in chronic and acute stroke. Neuropsychology, 25(5), 567.
Bonato, M. (2012). Neglect and extinction depend greatly on task demands: A review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 195.
Buxbaum, L.J., Ferraro, M.K., Veramonti, T., Farne, A., Whyte, J.M.D.P., Ladavas, E., Frassinetti, F., & Coslett, H.B. (2004). Hemispatial neglect subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology, 62(5), 749756.
Chechlacz, M., Rotshtein, P., Bickerton, W.L., Hansen, P.C., Deb, S., & Humphreys, G.W. (2010). Separating neural correlates of allocentric and egocentric neglect: Distinct cortical sites and common white matter disconnections. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(3), 277303.
Cherney, L.R., Halper, A.S., Kwasnica, C.M., Harvey, R.L., & Zhang, M. (2001). Recovery of functional status after right hemisphere stroke: Relationship with unilateral neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 82(3), 322328.
Czernuszenko, A. (2006). Risk factors for falls in post-stroke patients treated in a neurorehabilitation ward. Neurologia i Neurochirurgia Polska, 41(1), 2835.
Dalmaijer, E.S., Van der Stigchel, S., Nijboer, T.C., Cornelissen, T.H., & Husain, M. (2015). CancellationTools: All-in-one software for administration and analysis of cancellation tasks. Behavior research methods, 47(4), 10651075.
Delazer, M., Sojer, M., Ellmerer, P., Boehme, C., & Benke, T. (2018). Eye-tracking provides a sensitive measure of exploration deficits after acute right MCA stroke. Frontiers in Neurology, 9.
Driver, J. & Pouget, A. (2000). Object-centered visual neglect, or relative egocentric neglect? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 542545.
Gainotti, G. (1996). Lateralization of brain mechanisms underlying automatic and controlled forms of spatial orienting of attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 617622.
Gainotti, G. & Tiacci, C. (1971). The relationships between disorders of visual perception and unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 9(4), 451458.
Glöckner, A. & Herbold, A.K. (2011). An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(1), 7198.
Gonzalez, R.G., Schaefer, P.W., Buonanno, F.S., Schwamm, L.H., Budzik, R.F., Rordorf, G., Wang, B., Sorensen, A.G. and Koroshetz, W.J. (1999). Diffusion-weighted MR imaging: Diagnostic accuracy in patients imaged within 6 hours of stroke symptom onset. Radiology, 210(1), 155162
Grant, E.R. & Spivey, M.J. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14(5), 462466.
Gravetter, F. & Wallnau, L. (2014). Essentials of Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 8th edition. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing.
Green, H.J., Lemaire, P., & Dufau, S. (2007). Eye movement correlates of younger and older adults’ strategies for complex addition. Acta Psychologica, 125, 257278.
Hachinski, V., Iadecola, C., Petersen, R.C., Breteler, M.M., Nyenhuis, D.L., Black, S.E., & Leblanc, G.G. (2006). National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Canadian Stroke Network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke, 37(9), 22202241.
Hannula, D.E., Althoff, R.R., Warren, D.E., Riggs, L., Cohen, N.J., & Ryan, J.D. (2010). Worth a glance: Using eye movements to investigate the cognitive neuroscience of memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 166.
Heilman, K.M. & Valenstein, E. (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Annals of Neurology, 5(2), 166170.
Hillis, A.E., Newhart, M., Heidler, J., Barker, P.B., Herskovits, E.H., & Degaonkar, M. (2005). Anatomy of spatial attention: Insights from perfusion imaging and hemispatial neglect in acute stroke. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(12), 31613167.
Ishiai, S., Sugishita, M., Mitani, K., & Ishizawa, M. (1992). Leftward search in left unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 55(1), 4044.
Jehkonen, M., Laihosalo, M., & Kettunen, J.E. (2006). Impact of neglect on functional outcome after stroke-a review of methodological issues and recent research findings. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24(4), 209215.
Karnath, H.O. & Niemeier, M. (2002). Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behavior of patients with spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 15771585.
Karnath, H.O., Mandler, A., & Clavagnier, S. (2011). Object-based neglect varies with egocentric position. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 29832993.
Khurshid, S., Trupe, L.A., Newhart, M., Davis, C., Molitoris, J.J., Medina, J., Leigh, R., & Hillis, A.E. (2012). Reperfusion of specific cortical areas is associated with improvement in distinct forms of hemispatial neglect. Cortex, 48(5), 530539.
Làdavas, E., Carletti, M., & Gori, G. (1994). Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect: Horizontal and vertical dimensions. Neuropsychologia, 32, 11951208.
Libben, M.R. & Titone, D.A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 381.
Malec, J.F., Kragnessm, , Evans, R.W., Finlay, K.L., Kent, A., & Lezak, M. (2003 ). Further psychometric evaluation and revision of the May-Portland Adaptability Inventory in a national sample. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18(6), 479492.
Malec, J.F., Kean, J., Altman, I.M. & Swick, S. (2012). Mayo-Portland Adaptability Inventory: Comparing psychometrics in cerebrovascular accident to traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 93(12), 22712275. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2012.06.013
Marsh, E.B. & Hillis, A.E. (2008). Dissociation between egocentric and allocentric visuospatial and tactile neglect in acute stroke. Cortex, 44(9), 12151220.
Marshall, J.C., & Robertson, I.H. (Eds.). (2013). Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Medina, J., Kannan, V., Pawlak, M.A., Kleinman, J.T., Newhart, M., Davis, C., Heidler-Gary, J.E., Herskovits, E.H., & Hillis, A.E. (2009). Neural substrates of visuospatial processing in distinct reference frames: Evidence from unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 20732084.
Mevorach, C., Tsal, Y., & Humphreys, G. (2014). Low level perceptual, not attentional, processes modulate distractor interference in high perceptual load displays: Evidence from neglect/extinction. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 966.
Mozer, M.C. (2002). Frames of reference in unilateral neglect and visual perception: A computational perspective. Psychological Review, 109(1), 156.
Nijboer, T.C., Kollen, B.J., & Kwakkel, G. (2013). Time course of visuospatial neglect early after stroke: A longitudinal cohort study. Cortex, 49(8), 20212027.
Ota, H., Fujii, T., Suzuki, K., Fukatsu, R., & Yamadori, A. (2001). Dissociation of body-centered and stimulus-centered representations in unilateral neglect. Neurology, 57(11), 20642069.
Parikh, R., Mathai, A., Parikh, S., Sekhar, G.C., & Thomas, R. (2008). Understanding and using sensitivity, specificity and predictive values. Indian Journal of Ophthalmology, 56(1), 45. doi:10.4103/0301-4738.37595
Parton, A., Malhotra, P., & Husain, M. (2004). Hemispatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 75(1), 1321.
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
Rorden, C., Hjaltason, H., Fillmore, P., Fridriksson, J., Kjartansson, O., Magnusdottir, S., & Karnath, H.O. (2012). Allocentric neglect strongly associated with egocentric neglect. Neuropsychologia, 50(6), 11511157.
Shepherd, M., Findlay, J.M., & Hockey, R.J. (1986). The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(3), 475491.
Shine, L.C. (1973). A multi-way analysis of variance for single-subject designs. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 33, 633636.
Tombaugh, T.N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education for Two Measures of Verbal Fluency: FAS and Animal Naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167177.
Vallar, G. & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia, 24(5), 609622.
Velichkovsky, B.M. (1999). From levels of processing to stratification of cognition: Converging evidence from three domains of research, In B.H. Challis & B.M. Velichkovsky (Eds.), Stratification in cognition and consciousness (pp. 203235). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Velichkovsky, B.M., Rothert, A., Kopf, M., Dornhofer, S.M., & Joos, M. (2002). Towards an express-diagnostics for level of processing and hazard perception. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5, 145156.
Verdon, V., Schwartz, S., Lovblad, K.O., Hauert, C.A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: A study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Brain, 133(3), 880894.
Vuilleumier, P. & Landis, T. (1998). Illusory contours and spatial neglect. NeuroReport, 9(11), 24812484. doi: http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.library.ubc.ca/10.1097/00001756-199808030-00010
Wee, J.Y. & Hopman, W.M. (2008). Comparing consequences of right and left unilateral neglect in a stroke rehabilitation population. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 87(11), 910920.
Weierich, M.R., Treat, T.A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 9851018.
Yue, Y., Song, W., Huo, S., & Wang, M. (2012). Study on the occurrence and neural bases of hemispatial neglect with different reference frames. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 93(1), 156162.

Keywords

Type Description Title
WORD
Supplementary materials

Upshaw et al. supplementary material
Upshaw et al. supplementary material 1

 Word (16 KB)
16 KB

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed