Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-nptnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T23:37:24.016Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Proactive and reactive aggression: Developmental trajectories and longitudinal associations with callous–unemotional traits, impulsivity, and internalizing emotions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 April 2023

Erin P. Vaughan*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, 236 Audubon Hall, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Julianne S. Speck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, 236 Audubon Hall, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Paul J. Frick
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, 236 Audubon Hall, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Toni M. Walker
Affiliation:
Harris County Juvenile Probation Department, Houston, USA
Emily L. Robertson
Affiliation:
Florida International University, Miami, USA
James V. Ray
Affiliation:
University of Central Florida, Orlando, USA
Tina D. Wall Myers
Affiliation:
Louisiana Department of Health, Baton Rouge, USA
Laura C. Thornton
Affiliation:
ABT Associates, Cambridge, MA, USA
Laurence Steinberg
Affiliation:
Temple University & King Abdulaziz University, Philadelphia, USA
Elizabeth Cauffman
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Erin P. Vaughan, email: evaugh7@lsu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Research on proactive and reactive aggression has identified covariates unique to each function of aggression, but hypothesized correlates have often not been tested with consideration of developmental changes in or the overlap between the types of aggression. The present study examines the unique developmental trajectories of proactive and reactive aggression over adolescence and young adulthood and tests these trajectories’ associations with key covariates: callous–unemotional (CU) traits, impulsivity, and internalizing emotions. In a sample of 1,211 justice-involved males (ages 15–22), quadratic growth models (i.e., intercepts, linear slopes, and quadratic slopes) of each type of aggression were regressed onto quadratic growth models of the covariates while controlling for the other type of aggression. After accounting for the level of reactive aggression, the level of proactive aggression was predicted by the level of CU traits. However, change in proactive aggression over time was not related to the change in any covariates. After accounting for proactive aggression, reactive aggression was predicted by impulsivity, both at the initial level and in change over time. Results support that proactive and reactive aggression are unique constructs with separate developmental trajectories and distinct covariates.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Aggressive behavior is defined as actions resulting in harm to another person (Anderson & Bushman, Reference Anderson and Bushman2002). This construct has been the topic of decades of research in an effort to understand which individuals are at risk for exhibiting high levels of aggressive behavior, the development and trajectory of aggressive behavior over the life span, and the social, emotional, and interpersonal consequences of high levels of aggression. A key aspect of aggression that has emerged from this research is the differentiation between different functions of aggression, which include proactive aggression (i.e., aggressive behavior without antagonism, used to achieve specific goals) and reactive aggression (i.e., aggressive behavior in reaction to a perceived threat; Dodge, Reference Dodge, Pepler and Rubin1991; Dodge & Coie, Reference Dodge and Coie1987). Importantly, research has supported different correlates of the two functions, which could inform causal theory and guide intervention (Dodge, Reference Dodge, Pepler and Rubin1991; Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates and Pettit1997; Dodge & Coie, Reference Dodge and Coie1987; Merk et al., Reference Merk, Orobio de Castro and Koops2005; Poulin & Boivin, Reference Poulin and Boivin2000).

Specifically, reactive aggression is consistently correlated with measures of poor impulse control and high rates of negative affect. Many studies have found that reactive aggression is more closely linked to impulsivity than proactive aggression (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; Duan et al., Reference Duan, Yang, Zhang, Zhou and Yin2021; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009b; Latzman & Vaidya, Reference Latzman and Vaidya2013; Urben et al., Reference Urben, Habersaat, Pihet, Suter, Ridder and Stéphan2018). For example, a study of 242 elementary school-age children reported that a group of children characterized by reactive aggression showed higher levels of impulsivity compared to a group characterized by proactive aggression (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, McCarthy, Houghton, O’Connor and Zadow2018). Similar associations have also been found later in adolescence, with trajectories of hyperactive and impulsive symptoms between the ages of 7 and 15 being more strongly associated with trajectories of reactive aggression than proactive aggression (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Obsuth, Zirk-Sadowski, Ribeaud and Eisner2020). Several studies have also investigated the differential associations between the two functions of aggression and negative affect (i.e., depression, anxiety, and anger) and have reported that reactive aggression is also more strongly related to various forms of negative affect (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009a; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Hartley et al., Reference Hartley, Pettit and Castellanos2018; McAuliffe et al., Reference McAuliffe, Hubbard, Rubin, Morrow and Dearing2006; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Hubbard, Bookhout and Mlawer2019). While many studies in this domain focus on reactive aggression’s relationship with anger (McAuliffe et al., Reference McAuliffe, Hubbard, Rubin, Morrow and Dearing2006; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Hubbard, Bookhout and Mlawer2019), several studies have also linked reactive aggression to increased internalizing emotions such as depression, suicidal behavior, and anxiety (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009a, Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Hartley et al., Reference Hartley, Pettit and Castellanos2018; McAuliffe et al., Reference McAuliffe, Hubbard, Rubin, Morrow and Dearing2006). These findings are consistent with the frustration-aggression theory which suggests that heightened negative emotions in reaction to provocation or an inability to modulate such emotions places an individual at greater risk for making aggressive responses to provocation (Berkowitz, Reference Berkowitz1993; Finkel & Hall, Reference Finkel and Hall2018).

In contrast to these findings on reactive aggression, proactive aggression has been associated with reduced emotional responses to distress and expectations of positive outcomes of aggressive behavior (Aggensteiner et al., Reference Aggensteiner, Holz, Böttinger, Baumeister, Hohmann, Werhahn, Naiijen, Ilbegi, Glennon, Hoekstra, Dietrich, Deters, Saam, Schulze, Lythgoe, Sethi, Craig, Mastroianni, Sagar-Ouriaghli and Brandeis2022; Hubbard et al., Reference Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, Coie and Schwartz2001; Lozier et al., Reference Lozier, Cardinale, VanMeter and Marsh2014; Marsee & Frick, Reference Marsee and Frick2007; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Hubbard, Bookhout and Mlawer2019; Smithmyer et al., Reference Smithmyer, Hubbard and Simons2000). Additionally, callous–unemotional (CU) traits (i.e., lack of guilt, lack of empathy or concern for others, lack of concern over performance in important activities, and shallow or deficient affect; Frick et al., Reference Frick, Ray, Thornton and Kahn2014) have been linked to proactive aggression in several studies (Fanti et al., Reference Fanti, Frick and Georgiou2009; Frick et al., Reference Frick, Cornell, Barry, Bodin and Dane2003; Marsee & Frick, Reference Marsee and Frick2007; Urben et al., Reference Urben, Habersaat, Pihet, Suter, Ridder and Stéphan2018). While several studies have also found support for both functions of aggression being related to CU traits (Elowsky et al., Reference Elowsky, Bajaj, Bashford-Largo, Zhang, Mathur, Schwartz, Dobbertin, Blair, Leibenluft, Pardini and Blair2022; Frick et al., Reference Frick, Cornell, Barry, Bodin and Dane2003), evidence indicates that the associations are stronger and more consistent for proactive aggression (Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011). For example, Fite et al. (Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010) examined the longitudinal associations between the two functions of aggression and CU traits and found that reactive aggression (measured at age 16) was concurrently but not longitudinally associated with CU traits (measured at ages 16 and 26), but that proactive aggression was both concurrently and longitudinally associated with CU traits. In addition, those high on proactive aggression also show a cognitive style in which they expect their aggressive behavior to result in positive outcomes (Arsenio et al., Reference Arsenio, Gold and Adams2004; Hubbard et al., Reference Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, Coie and Schwartz2001; Smithmyer et al., Reference Smithmyer, Hubbard and Simons2000). It is important to note, however, that this cognitive style is also associated with CU traits (Frick et al., Reference Frick, Ray, Thornton and Kahn2014) and may contribute to the higher rate of proactive aggression shown by individuals high on these traits. Importantly, these correlates are consistent with social learning theory, which proposes that individuals who have experienced positive consequences from past aggressive acts (i.e., accomplishing their intended goal) will be more likely to use this method to accomplish their goals in the future (Bandura, Reference Bandura1973).

Thus, research on the two functions of aggression has great potential for advancing theories for the development of aggressive behavior and for guiding interventions that target mechanisms unique to each type of aggression. However, this research has been limited by a number of factors. First, behavior associated with the two functions of aggression are often highly correlated, ranging from .40 to .90 across samples of youth with the typical estimate being approximately .70 (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; Little et al., Reference Little, Henrich, Jones and Hawley2003; Poulin & Boivin, Reference Poulin and Boivin2000). Further, research has consistently shown an asymmetry in the overlap between the two types of aggression. Specifically, there appears to be a significant number of children who only show reactive aggression, whereas most children who show high levels of proactive aggression also show high rates of reactive aggression (Brown, et al., Reference Brown, Atkins, Osborne and Milnamow1996; Dodge & Coie, Reference Dodge and Coie1987; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Frick, Barry, Kimonis, Centifanti and Aucoin2014). The fact that the combined aggressive group is typically more aggressive overall has led some researchers to question whether the two functions of aggression reflect different patterns of behavior with unique causal mechanisms or whether proactive aggression is simply an indicator of severity of aggressive behavior (Bushman & Anderson, Reference Bushman and Anderson2001; Walters, Reference Walters and Morgan2005). Also, this pattern of overlap may obscure differential correlates to the two functions of aggression, since those high on proactive aggression may also show elevated rates of reactive aggression. Failure to consider this correlation among the aggressive subtypes may explain some of the inconsistent findings in past research on the differential correlates (Latzman & Vaidya, Reference Latzman and Vaidya2013; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Obsuth, Zirk-Sadowski, Ribeaud and Eisner2020; Pérez Fuentes et al., Reference Pérez Fuentes, Molero Jurado del, Carrión Martínez, Mercader Rubio and Gázquez2016). Thus, theories to explain the different functions of aggression need to not only explain the different correlates to the two types of aggression but also explain the high correlations. For example, there is evidence to suggest that persons high on proactive aggression may appear to show anger in response to provocation but lack the emotional arousal typically associated with such responses (Hubbard et al., Reference Hubbard, Smithmyer, Ramsden, Parker, Flanagan, Dearing, Relyea and Simons2002; Jambon et al., Reference Jambon, Colasante, Peplak and Malti2019; Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Frick, Kimonis and Aucoin2008; Song et al., Reference Song, Colasante and Malti2020). Thus, those high on proactive aggression could appear to show reactive aggression but fail to show the same emotional correlates as other persons who show reactive aggression. Further, the overlap in the types of aggressive behavior means that research studying the correlates to aggressive behavior need to consider ways of controlling for this overlap, either by using person-centered analyses that group persons into aggressive typologies (e.g., non-aggressive, reactive aggressive only, or combined proactive and reactive aggressive; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Frick, Barry, Kimonis, Centifanti and Aucoin2014) or by studying the variance unique to each function of aggression (i.e., controlling for the other function of aggression; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Evans, Pederson and Tampke2017; Paré-Ruel et al., Reference Paré-Ruel, Brendgen, Ouellet-Morin, Lupien, Vitaro, Dionne and Boivin2022).

A second limitation in existing research on the unique correlates to proactive and reactive aggression is that much of the research has been correlational (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, McCarthy, Houghton, O’Connor and Zadow2018; Duan et al., Reference Duan, Yang, Zhang, Zhou and Yin2021; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009a; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009b; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011; Marsee & Frick, Reference Marsee and Frick2007; Urben et al., Reference Urben, Habersaat, Pihet, Suter, Ridder and Stéphan2018) or, if longitudinal, it has not used a design that separates predictors of the severity of aggression over time from predictors of changes in the level of aggression over time (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Frick et al., Reference Frick, Cornell, Barry, Bodin and Dane2003; McAuliffe et al., Reference McAuliffe, Hubbard, Rubin, Morrow and Dearing2006). This is a critical consideration because predictors of severity of aggression (i.e., the intercept of the trajectory) may not be the same as predictors of changes in this severity over time (i.e., the slope of the trajectory). Further, consideration of developmental changes is particularly important when studying aggression in adolescence, given that past research has found that aggressive behavior displays significant change over the course of development. While studies of the trajectories of aggression over time often find that at least some individuals have aggression which is largely stable or increases slightly over time, most findings support that aggression generally decreases from childhood to young adulthood (Bongers et al., Reference Bongers, Koot, van der Ende and Verhulst2004; Fite et al., 2008; Jennings & Reingle, Reference Jennings and Reingle2012; Maldonado-Molina et al., Reference Maldonado-Molina, Reingle, Tobler, Jennings and Komro2010; Nagin & Tremblay, Reference Nagin and Tremblay1999; Storvall & Wichstrøm, Reference Storvall and Wichstrøm2003; Xie et al., Reference Xie, Drabick and Chen2011). However, much of the literature on changes in aggression over time have focused on the childhood or adolescent period, with little research focusing on the trajectory of aggression during the transition to adulthood (for an exception, see Fite et al., Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010 which studied aggression at ages 16 and 26).

Unfortunately, much of the research on the developmental trajectories of aggressive behavior has not considered the two functions (i.e., proactive and reactive aggression) separately. When research has separated the different functions, it has usually found that both functions of aggression follow generally similar patterns to those found in the broader aggression literature (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Vitaro, Lacourse, Fontaine, Carbonneau and Tremblay2010; Cui et al., Reference Cui, Colasante, Malti, Ribeaud and Eisner2016; Fite et al., 2008; Ojanen & Kiefer, Reference Ojanen and Kiefer2013). However, while studies are largely consistent in finding that reactive aggression tends to be more common across development than proactive aggression (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Vitaro, Lacourse, Fontaine, Carbonneau and Tremblay2010; Cui et al., Reference Cui, Colasante, Malti, Ribeaud and Eisner2016; Fite et al., 2008), findings have been mixed regarding the degree of change in proactive aggression over time compared to reactive aggression (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Vitaro, Lacourse, Fontaine, Carbonneau and Tremblay2010; Ojanen & Kiefer, Reference Ojanen and Kiefer2013). These conflicting results highlight the need for greater research in this area; in addition, the possibility of different trajectories suggests that when trying to predict changes in aggressive behavior over time, it is important to not only account for the severity of the other function of aggression but also to account for the change in the other function of aggression over time. In the only study to do this, results indicated that, after accounting for the other function of aggression, the trajectories of both functions of aggression did not change (Fite et al., 2008). However, this study did not investigate how change in the two types of aggression was related to hypothesized correlates, leaving it unknown how key variables are uniquely related to the trajectories of aggression over time.

Current study

Thus, in the present study, we study several variables that have consistently shown differential correlations with the two functions of aggression in past research: impulsivity, internalizing emotions, and CU traits. Impulsivity and internalizing emotions have been more consistently associated with reactive aggression, and CU traits have been associated with proactive aggression. While we hypothesized that the differential associations would be similar to those found in past work, we employed several major advances in our research design. First, we controlled for the shared variance in the two functions of aggression in studying these associations. Second, we utilized a longitudinal design in which the two functions of aggression, as well as the hypothesized correlates, were assessed multiple times. Using an accelerated multiple cohort design, we were able to model the trajectories of the two functions of aggression and their correlates over a developmental period from ages 15 to 22, allowing us to test the predictors of both the level of aggression over time and changes in this level across a developmental period that has not been the focus of a great deal of past research (i.e., the transition to adulthood). Finally, we extended past research to include a high-risk, justice-involved sample, which is in contrast to most past work that has focused on community samples. This design feature is important because it led to a sample with higher levels of aggression and, in particular, proactive aggression, which tends to show a fairly low base rate in community samples (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; Poulin & Boivin, Reference Poulin and Boivin2000). When studying trajectories over time, it is important to have significant variability in these variables of interest in order to detect clear developmental trends and to identify predictors of these trends.

Method

Participants

The current study utilized data collected as a part of the Crossroads Study, a multisite longitudinal study of male youths involved in the juvenile justice system. Eligible participants for the Crossroads Study were male English-speaking adolescents who were arrested for the first time between the ages of 13 and 17 (Mage = 15.29) in three jurisdictions in the United States (Orange County, California; Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; and Jefferson Parish, Louisiana). All participants were arrested for offenses of mild to moderate severity. Qualifying offenses included property offenses such as vandalism and theft (48%), drug offenses such as possession of a controlled substance (23%), and person offenses such as assault and battery (20%). The total sample consisted of 1,216 male adolescents. Forty-six percent of the sample self-identified as Latino, 37% identified as Black, 15% identified as White, and 2% identified as another race or ethnicity. The sample represented a range of socioeconomic statuses, measured through parental educational attainment; approximately 27% of the sample did not have a parent who completed high school, 35% had at least one parent who completed high school, and 38% had at least one parent who completed education beyond high school. Further information about the demographic characteristics of the sample and the primary aims of the Crossroads study can be found elsewhere (Cauffman et al., Reference Cauffman, Beardslee, Fine, Frick and Steinberg2021).

Procedure

Institutional review board approval was obtained at sponsoring institutions at each site. Contact information for eligible youths were obtained for all youth who met the inclusionary criteria. As a part of the informed consent and assent procedures, youths and their parent or guardian were informed that their participation in the study would not influence their treatment in the justice system. In addition, they were informed that all information was protected from involuntary disclosure by a Privacy Certificate obtained from the Department of Justice. Within six weeks of their arrest, participants were interviewed in convenient locations (i.e., home, library, fast food restaurant, detention center, jail, etc.) by a trained interviewer. Interviews were conducted over the phone if the youth moved outside of the study area, if the facility in which they lived did not allow in-person interviews, or due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Structured interviews were administered using a standardized protocol with laptop computers equipped with all study questionnaires.

Follow up interviews were conducted every 6 months for the first three years, then yearly (with the exception of Year 6) for another 4 years. Six-month intervals were not used in current analyses. Data for the current study was therefore collected at seven time points (baseline, Year 1, Year 2, Year 3, Year 4, Year 5, and Year 7). Compensation started at $50 for the baseline interview and increased by $15 for each interview, up to $140 which was provided at Year 3 through Year 7. Retention rates were high across time points; 94% of the sample were retained at Year 1, 93% at Year 2, 91% at Year 3, 87% at Year 4, 84% at Year 5, and 76% at Year 7. Participants who dropped out of the study by Year 7 did not differ on any baseline study variables (i.e., proactive aggression, reactive aggression, CU traits, impulsivity, or internalizing emotions) when compared to participants who persisted throughout the study.

Measures

Proactive and reactive aggression

The proactive and reactive overt aggression scales from the Peer Conflict Scale (PCS; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011; Marsee & Frick, Reference Marsee and Frick2007) were used to measure the two functions of aggression. Although the PCS also includes relational aggression scales, these scales were not used in current analyses due to past findings that relational aggression may be more important in female samples (Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Frick, Barry, Kimonis, Centifanti and Aucoin2014). All items were rated on a scale from 0 (“not at all true”) to 3 (“definitely true”). The proactive overt aggression scale consisted of 10 items (i.e., “I start fights to get what I want”, “I carefully plan out how to hurt others”) which were summed to form a total score reflecting greater proactive aggression. The reactive overt aggression scale also consisted of 10 items (i.e., “I threaten others when they do something wrong to me”, “If others make me mad, I hurt them”) which were summed to form a total score reflecting greater reactive aggression. These scales of the PCS have been correlated with laboratory measures of aggression and other indicators of violence (Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Frick, Kimonis and Aucoin2008) and the two subscales have been found to be associated with differences in several hypothesized correlates. Specifically, the reactive aggression scale has been uniquely associated with reaction to provocation and poor emotion regulation, while the proactive aggression scale has been uniquely associated with CU traits and biased outcome expectations for aggressive behavior in other samples of adolescents and young adults (Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011; Marsee & Frick, Reference Marsee and Frick2007; Muñoz et al., Reference Muñoz, Frick, Kimonis and Aucoin2008; Vagos et al., Reference Vagos, Marinho, Pandeirada, Rodrigues and Marsee2021). In the present sample, both scales showed acceptable to good internal consistency across time points (Cronbach’s αs = .72 – .82 for proactive aggression; Cronbach’s αs = .82 – .86 for reactive aggression).

CU traits

CU traits were measured using the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits (ICU; Kimonis et al., Reference Kimonis, Frick, Skeem, Marsee, Cruise, Munoz, Aucoin and Morris2008). The ICU consists of 12 positively-worded items reflecting greater CU traits (i.e., “I do not feel remorseful when I do something wrong”) and 12 negatively-worded items reflecting lower CU traits (i.e., “I am concerned about the feelings of others”), each rated on a scale from 0 (“not at all true”) to 3 (“definitely true”). After reverse-coding negatively-worded items, items were summed to create a total score that reflected greater CU traits. Although past work has found that this measure of CU traits can be broken into subscales, the total score was used in the current study due to consistent findings of a general factor that accounts for a large portion of the variance of subscales and that is associated negatively with empathy and positively with aggression in a variety of child, adolescent, and adult samples (Cardinale & Marsh, Reference Cardinale and Marsh2020; Ray & Frick, Reference Ray and Frick2020). Across time points, Cronbach’s α ranged from .76 to .80 for this scale.

Impulsivity

Impulsivity was measured using the Weinberger Adjustment Inventory (WAI) Impulse Control Scale (Weinberger & Schwartz, Reference Weinberger and Schwartz1990). Items measure level of impulsive control (i.e., “I should try harder to control myself when I’m having fun”, “I do things without giving them enough thought”) that are rated on a scale from “false” (1) to “true” (5). For the current analyses, items were inversely scored and summed, so that higher scores indicated greater levels of impulsive responding. This scale has been related to antisocial behavior and poor self-control in adolescent and young adult samples (Jones, Reference Jones2017; Monahan et al., Reference Monahan, Steinberg, Cauffman and Mulvey2009). Internal consistency was acceptable across time points (Cronbach’s α = .74 – .79).

Internalizing emotions

A composite score from an abridged version of the Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scale (RCADS; Chorpita et al., Reference Chorpita, Yim, Moffitt, Umemoto and Francis2000) was used to measure internalizing emotions. The RCADS is a well-validated measure of internalizing symptomatology, as indicated by correlations with other measures of depression and anxiety in child and adolescent samples (Chorpita et al., Reference Chorpita, Moffitt and Gray2005), as well as in young adult samples (McKenzie et al., Reference McKenzie, Murray, Freeston, Whelan and Rodgers2019). While the full RCADS includes several subscales reflecting multiple internalizing disorders, only the major depressive disorder (MDD; i.e., “I feel sad or empty”) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; i.e., “I worry about things”) subscales were administered during the Crossroads Study. One item which was conceptualized as a part of the separation anxiety disorder (SAD) scale (“I worry when I go to bed at night”) has shown split loadings with the GAD factor (Chorpita et al., Reference Chorpita, Yim, Moffitt, Umemoto and Francis2000); as such, this item was included in the current version of the RCADS. For current analyses, all depression and anxiety items were summed to provide a composite measure of internalizing emotions, such that higher scores indicated greater depression and anxiety. The composite internalizing emotions scale showed good internal consistency across time points (Cronbach’s α = .87–.92).

Data analysis

For data analyses, we used an accelerated multiple cohort design (Galbraith et al., Reference Galbraith, Bowden and Mander2017). Individuals were placed into six cohorts based on their age at baseline: age 13 (n = 136), age 14 (n = 210), age 15 (n = 300), age 16 (n = 310), age 17 (n = 259), and age 18 (n = 1; one participant who was 17 at the time of their arrest had turned 18 by the time of the baseline interview). While individuals in the age 13 cohort had data collected at ages 13–20, individuals in the age 18 cohort had data collected at ages 18–25. All data collected at each age was collapsed into a single variable regardless of cohort (e.g., age 17 data consisted of baseline data from the age 17 cohort, Year 1 data from the age 16 cohort, etc.), resulting in a single dataset with data collected between ages 13 and 25. Although no age had complete data, ages with valid data for less than one-third of the sample were excluded; therefore, data collected at ages 13 (n = 136), 14 (n = 337), 23 (n = 253), 24 (n = 204), and 25 (n = 1) were not included in model estimation. Five participants who only had data available at ages 13 and/or 14 were therefore excluded from analyses. As such, the final dataset consisted of 1,211 individuals with data collected between ages 15 and 22, though participants did not have data collected at all of these yearly intervals (age 15 [n = 625], age 16 [n = 908], age 17 [n = 1137], age 18 [n = 1109], age 19 [n = 960], age 20 [n = 838], age 21 [n = 651], age 22 [n = 433]). Missing data due to attrition/age gaps were estimated using the MLR estimator, a full information maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors, in Mplus 8.4. If individual items were missing for any questionnaires, a prorated total score was calculated if at least 80% of the questionnaire was valid.

First, to determine the shape of the trajectory of all study variables, a linear growth model was fit to each type of aggression (i.e., proactive and reactive aggression) and each covariate (i.e., CU traits, impulsivity, and internalizing emotions). Past research has detected some non-linear trends in aggression over development (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Vitaro, Lacourse, Fontaine, Carbonneau and Tremblay2010; Cui et al., Reference Cui, Colasante, Malti, Ribeaud and Eisner2016; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Colder, Lochman and Wells2008; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Obsuth, Zirk-Sadowski, Ribeaud and Eisner2020). For example, Fite et al. (Reference Fite, Colder, Lochman and Wells2008) found that proactive and reactive aggression reached a peak in early adolescence and decreased into later adolescence. Thus, to determine if a non-linear trajectory better fit the data, a quadratic slope factor was then added to each model, and improvement in model fit was tested using chi-squared difference tests, RMSEA (root mean square error of estimation), CFI (comparative fit index), TLI (Tucker-Lewis index), and SRMR (standardized root mean square residual; Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999).

The best-fitting univariate models (i.e., linear or quadratic) for each variable were then combined into multivariate directional growth models (i.e., one for proactive aggression and one for reactive aggression; Bollen & Curran, Reference Bollen and Curran2006). In order to test hypotheses regarding associations between covariates and the two types of aggression while controlling for the other type of aggression, intercepts and slopes (both linear and quadratic, where relevant) of aggression were regressed onto the intercepts and slopes of covariates and the other type of aggression. For example, the intercept of proactive aggression was regressed onto intercepts of CU traits, impulsivity, internalizing emotions, and reactive aggression. Further, the linear slope of proactive aggression was regressed onto linear slopes of CU traits, impulsivity, internalizing emotions, and reactive aggression. Finally, the quadratic slope of proactive aggression regressed onto quadratic slopes of CU traits, impulsivity, internalizing emotions, and reactive aggression. These analyses were repeated for reactive aggression.

Results

Single variable growth models

Descriptive statistics for all study variables across age are shown in Table 1. As shown in Table 2, the addition of a quadratic slope factor improved model fit for all variables. Therefore, all variables were assumed to have a quadratic growth structure, and quadratic slopes were included for all variables in multivariate growth models. Proactive aggression had a mean intercept of 1.62 (p < .001), a mean linear slope of −0.32 (p < .001), and a mean quadratic slope of 0.03 (p < .001). Reactive aggression had a mean intercept of 5.30 (p < .001), a mean linear slope of −0.77 (p < .001), and a mean quadratic slope of 0.05 (p < .001). The intercepts, linear slopes, and quadratic slopes for both proactive and reactive aggression all had significant variances (p < .05), indicating that investigation of covariates that can predict this variance is warranted. Change in proactive and reactive aggression between ages 15 and 22 is depicted in Figure 1. This figure shows first that, as expected, the level of reactive aggression was substantially higher than proactive aggression across the entire developmental period. Further, also as predicted, both types of aggression decreased over time, with this decrease being greatest during the earlier (i.e., adolescent) ages. Proactive aggression reached a minimum at approximately age 20 followed by a very slight increase after this age.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics across ages

Table 2. Comparison of linear and quadratic growth models for all study variables

Note: Because MLR estimator was used, change in χ2 and significance values represent corrected χ2 values after using scaling formulas provided by Muthén and Muthén (Reference Muthén and Muthén2005).

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

Figure 1. Quadratic trajectories of proactive and reactive aggression in adolescence & young adulthood.

Proactive aggression multivariate growth

The proactive multivariate growth model with quadratic slopes for all variables displayed good model fit (RMSEA = .02, CFI = .99, TLI = .98, SRMR = .04). Regression parameters for the multivariate quadratic growth model of proactive aggression are reported in Table 3. The intercept of proactive aggression was positively predicted by the intercepts of reactive aggression (β = .69, p < .001) and CU traits (β = .15, p < .01), but the linear and quadratic slopes of proactive aggression were not significantly related to the linear and quadratic slopes of reactive aggression or CU traits. Impulsivity and internalizing emotions were not related to the intercept, linear slope, or quadratic slope of proactive aggression.

Table 3. Multivariate quadratic growth model results for proactive and reactive aggression

Note: Standardized results are reported in text.

* p < .01.

** p < .001.

Reactive aggression multivariate growth

The reactive multivariate growth model also displayed good model fit (RMSEA = .02, CFI = .99, TLI = .98, SRMR = .04). Results with reactive aggression were consistent across the intercepts, linear slopes, and quadratic slopes. As shown in Table 3, at all three levels of analysis, reactive aggression was positively predicted by proactive aggression (βintercept = .74, p < .001; βlinear = .73, p < .001; βquadratic = .78, p < .001) and impulsivity (βintercept = .16, p < .001; βlinear = .19, p < .001, βquadratic = .24, p < .01), but CU traits and internalizing emotions were not significantly related to reactive aggression.Footnote 1

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the distinct correlates to proactive and reactive aggression, using a longitudinal design that can test for predictors of severity and predictors of change over time. Further, the longitudinal design allowed us to control for the severity and level of change in the other function of aggression, which is critical given the pattern of overlap between the two functions of aggression and the potential differences in trajectories over time.

Consistent with a great deal of past research, this study found that reactive aggression was more common across the entire developmental range studied (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Atkins, Osborne and Milnamow1996; Dodge & Coie, Reference Dodge and Coie1987; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Frick, Barry, Kimonis, Centifanti and Aucoin2014) and that both forms of aggression decreased across adolescence and into young adulthood (Bongers et al., Reference Bongers, Koot, van der Ende and Verhulst2004; Fite et al., Reference Fite, Colder, Lochman and Wells2008; Nagin & Tremblay, Reference Nagin and Tremblay1999; Storvall & Wichstrøm, Reference Storvall and Wichstrøm2003; Xie et al., Reference Xie, Drabick and Chen2011). However, our finding of a quadratic trend for both forms of aggression suggests that this decrease in aggression was less later in adolescence and into young adulthood (Loeber & Hay, Reference Loeber and Hay1997). Importantly, our findings suggested that this quadratic trend was similar for both proactive and reactive aggression.

Some of these findings could be consistent with suggestions that the less frequent proactive aggression is better considered an indicator of severity, rather than an indicator of another type of aggression (Bushman & Anderson, Reference Bushman and Anderson2001; Walters, Reference Walters and Morgan2005). Also consistent with such a contention, multivariate growth models for proactive aggression revealed that the intercept (or starting value at age 15) of proactive aggression was positively related to the intercept of reactive aggression, indicating that individuals with higher levels of proactive aggression also tended to have higher levels of reactive aggression. However, contrary to a model of proactive aggression only being an indicator of severity, we found that, after controlling for the association between proactive and reactive aggression, the remaining variance in the intercept of proactive aggression was predicted by the intercept of CU traits. That is, individuals with higher levels of proactive aggression also had higher levels of CU traits. This finding is consistent with past research showing that CU traits are more consistently related to proactive aggression, particularly when controlling for reactive aggression (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009b, Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011; Urben et al., Reference Urben, Habersaat, Pihet, Suter, Ridder and Stéphan2018). Although not tested in this study, this link between proactive aggression and CU traits could be due to the tendency of individuals with elevated CU traits to overestimate the positive outcomes of aggression and, as a result, make aggression more likely to occur in anticipation of instrumental gain (Frick et al., Reference Frick, Ray, Thornton and Kahn2014).

Another unique aspect of our study design is that our use of growth models allowed us to separate predictors of the severity of aggression from predictors of the change in aggression over time. Interestingly, none of our predictors, including CU traits and reactive aggression, significantly predicted changes in proactive aggression over time. This finding could be due to our choice of risk factors that were largely dispositional characteristics of the youth (e.g., CU traits, impulse control, internalizing emotions), whereas changes in proactive aggression may be more related to contextual factors (e.g., parenting practices) that could determine whether or not the aggressive behavior is reinforced over time. This possibility needs to be tested in future research, but our results highlight the importance of separating potential predictors of the severity of aggression with predictors of change in the levels of aggressive behavior over time.

As expected, the intercept, linear slope, and quadratic slope of proactive aggression predicted the intercept, linear slope, and quadratic slope of reactive aggression. Once variance due to this overlap was accounted for in analyses, impulsivity provided additional predictive utility for the intercept, linear slope, and quadratic slope of reactive aggression, which is consistent with a great deal of past research (Card & Little, Reference Card and Little2006; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Obsuth, Zirk-Sadowski, Ribeaud and Eisner2020; Urben et al., Reference Urben, Habersaat, Pihet, Suter, Ridder and Stéphan2018). Thus, for reactive aggression, greater overall levels of impulsivity and greater change in impulsivity over time predicted both greater levels of reactive aggression and greater change in reactive aggression over time. Consistent with our hypotheses and past research, CU traits did not predict the severity and degree of change in reactive aggression once the severity and degree of change in proactive aggression was controlled (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010; Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Barry, Childs, Frick, Kimonis, Muñoz, Aucoin, Fassnacht, Kunimatsu and Lau2011). These findings support the theory that reactive aggression is closely tied to an individual’s reduced ability to inhibit their impulses, causing them to react strongly to perceived provocation (Bertsch et al., Reference Bertsch, Florange and Herpertz2020; Finkel & Hall, Reference Finkel and Hall2018).

Contrary to predictions and past research (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009a, Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010), neither severity nor level of change in reactive aggression was predicted by our measure of internalizing emotions. This is inconsistent with theoretical predictions that reactive aggression often occurs in the context of high emotional arousal (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates and Pettit1997; Dodge & Coie, Reference Dodge and Coie1987). It should be noted, however, that past research linking internalizing emotions to reactive aggression did not control for more general problems with impulse control (Fite et al., Reference Fite, Stoppelbein and Greening2009a, Reference Fite, Raine, Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber and Pardini2010). Thus, controlling for more general difficulties in behavioral and emotional regulation may eliminate any predictive power related to the expression of internalizing emotions.

All interpretations of our current findings must take into account several limitations in the study design. While the current high-risk and justice-involved sample was helpful for studying aggressive behavior by resulting in a sample with greater variability in such behaviors when compared to community samples, this sample limits our ability to generalize our results to other types of samples. Also, by studying justice-involved adolescents, we were forced to limit our data collection to only boys, which means that our findings need to be tested in samples of girls. There is evidence that aggression may be expressed differently in girls (Marsee et al., Reference Marsee, Frick, Barry, Kimonis, Centifanti and Aucoin2014), making such tests critical for determining the generalizability of our results. Finally, our results relied on self-report measures for assessing both aggression and our hypothesized predictors. While this could not account for differences in which predictors were related to aggression, it could have inflated the overall level of associations due to shared method variance.

Despite these limitations, our findings have important implications for the conceptualization, measurement, and treatment of aggressive behaviors in adolescents and young adults. Most importantly, these findings provide additional evidence that, despite significant overlap, proactive and reactive aggression are distinct constructs with unique developmental influences that need to be studied in a way that controls for the variance accounted for by the other function of aggression. Further, these findings suggest that aggression needs to be assessed in a way that separately studies proactive and reactive aggression and allows for aggressive individuals to have tailored treatments. For example, an individual with a largely reactive style of aggression, impulse control may be an important target of intervention; for an individual who also shows proactive types of aggression, increasing empathy and emotion recognition may be a more important target of treatment (Frick, 2012). In addition to supporting the importance of separate consideration of proactive and reactive aggression in research and practice, these findings also have important implications for the constructs of CU traits and impulsivity. For example, both CU traits and impulsivity have been considered a part of the larger construct of psychopathy (Frick, Reference Frick2022). However, current findings suggest that these two constructs show unique associations with separate forms of aggression, indicating that considering these together in a composite measure of psychopathy may reduce predictive utility of the separate constructs, especially as they help to explain different patterns of aggressive behavior.

Acknowledgments

None.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention (2005-JK-FX-K001), the County of Orange, and the William T. Grant Foundation.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Footnotes

1 Analyses were also conducted including IQ (measured at baseline by a proxy variable consisting of the Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subscales from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) as a time-invariant covariate in multivariate directional growth models. The addition of IQ as a covariate did not change pattern of study results.

References

Aggensteiner, P. M., Holz, N. E., Böttinger, B. W., Baumeister, S., Hohmann, S., Werhahn, J. E., Naiijen, J., Ilbegi, S., Glennon, J. C., Hoekstra, P. J., Dietrich, A., Deters, R. K., Saam, M. C., Schulze, U. M. E., Lythgoe, D. J., Sethi, A., Craig, M. C., Mastroianni, M., Sagar-Ouriaghli, I., … Brandeis, D. (2022). The effects of callous-unemotional traits and aggression subtypes on amygdala activity in response to negative faces. Psychological Medicine, 52, 476484. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002111 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arsenio, W. F., Gold, J., & Adams, E. (2004). Adolescents’ emotion expectancies regarding aggressive and nonaggressive events: Connections with behavior problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(4), 338355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning theory analysis. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Barker, E. D., Vitaro, F., Lacourse, E., Fontaine, N. M., Carbonneau, R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Testing the developmental distinctiveness of male proactive and reactive aggression with a nested longitudinal experimental intervention. Aggressive Behavior, 36(2), 127140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Bertsch, K., Florange, J., & Herpertz, S. C. (2020). Understanding brain mechanisms of reactive aggression. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(12), 81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-01208-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2006). Latent curve models: A structural equation perspective. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 75(5), 15231537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, K., Atkins, M. S., Osborne, M. L., & Milnamow, M. (1996). A revised teacher rating scale for reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24(4), 473480. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01441569 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychological Review, 108(1), 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(5), 466480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071904 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardinale, E. M., & Marsh, A. A. (2020). The reliability and validity of the inventory of callous unemotional traits: A meta-analytic review. Assessment, 27(1), 5771. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191117747392 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, A., McCarthy, M., Houghton, S., O’Connor, E. S., & Zadow, C. (2018). Reactive and proactive aggression as meaningful distinctions at the variable and person level in primary school-aged children. Aggressive Behavior, 44(5), 431441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cauffman, E., Beardslee, J., Fine, A., Frick, P., & Steinberg, L. (2021). Crossroads in juvenile justice: The impact of initial processing decision on youth 5 years after first arrest. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 700713. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942000200X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chorpita, B. F., Moffitt, C. E., & Gray, J. (2005). Psychometric properties of the revised child anxiety and depression scale in a clinical sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(3), 309322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chorpita, B. F., Yim, L., Moffitt, C., Umemoto, L. A., & Francis, S. E. (2000). Assessment of symptoms of DSM-IV anxiety and depression in children: A revised child anxiety and depression scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(8), 835855. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00130-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cui, L., Colasante, T., Malti, T., Ribeaud, D., & Eisner, M. P. (2016). Dual trajectories of reactive and proactive aggression from mid-childhood to early adolescence: Relations to sensation seeking, risk taking, and moral reasoning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(4), 663675. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0079-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1991). T he structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In Pepler, D. J. & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.), The Development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 201-218). L. Erlbaum Associates. Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 11461158. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 3751. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.37 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duan, J., Yang, Z., Zhang, F., Zhou, Y., & Yin, J. (2021). Aggressive behaviors in highly sadistic and highly impulsive individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 178, 110875. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110875 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elowsky, J., Bajaj, S., Bashford-Largo, J., Zhang, R., Mathur, A., Schwartz, A., Dobbertin, M., Blair, K. S., Leibenluft, E., Pardini, D., & Blair, R. J. R. (2022). Differential associations of conduct disorder, callous-unemotional traits, and irritability with outcome expectations and values regarding the consequences of aggression. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 16(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-022-00466-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanti, K. A., Frick, P. J., & Georgiou, S. (2009). Linking callous-unemotional traits to instrumental and non-instrumental forms of aggression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31(4), 285. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-008-9111-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, E. J., & Hall, A. N. (2018). The I3 Model: A metatheoretical framework for understanding aggression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 125130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fite, P. J., Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2008). Developmental trajectories of proactive and reactive aggression from fifth to ninth grade. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(2), 412421. https://doi.org/15374410801955920 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fite, P. J., Evans, S. C., Pederson, C. A., & Tampke, E. C. (2017). Functions of aggression and disciplinary actions among elementary school-age youth. Child & Youth Care Forum, 46, 825839. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-017-9410-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fite, P. J., Raine, A., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., & Pardini, D. A. (2010). Reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent males: Examining differential outcomes 10 years later in early adulthood. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 37(2), 141157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854809353051 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fite, P. J., Stoppelbein, L., & Greening, L. (2009a). Proactive and reactive aggression in a child psychiatric inpatient population. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38(2), 199205. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410802698461 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fite, P. J., Stoppelbein, L., & Greening, L. (2009b). Proactive and reactive aggression in a child psychiatric inpatient population: Relations to psychopathic characteristics. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36(5), 481493. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854809332706 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, P. J. (2022). Some critical considerations in applying the construct of psychopathy to research and classification of childhood disruptive behavior disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 96, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102188 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., & Dane, H. E. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(4), 457470. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023899703866 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 157. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galbraith, S., Bowden, J., & Mander, A. (2017). Accelerated longitudinal designs: An overview of modelling, power, costs and handling missing data. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(1), 374398. https://doi.org/10.1177/0962280214547150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, C. M., Pettit, J. W., & Castellanos, D. (2018). Reactive aggression and suicide-related behaviors in children and adolescents: A review and preliminary meta-analysis. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48(1), 3851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, J. A., Dodge, K A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Coie, J. D., & Schwartz, D. (2001). The dyadic nature of social information processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 268280. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.2.268 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., Dearing, K. F., Relyea, N., & Simons, R. F. (2002). Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger: Relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Development, 73(4), 11011118. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00460 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jambon, M., Colasante, T., Peplak, J., & Malti, T. (2019). Anger, sympathy, and children’s reactive and proactive aggression: Testing a differential correlation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 10131024. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0498-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, W. G., & Reingle, J. M. (2012). On the number and shape of developmental/life-course violence, aggression, and delinquency trajectories: A state-of-the-art review. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40(6), 472489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2012.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. (2017). Does choice of measure matter? Assessing the similarities and differences among self-control scales. Journal of Criminal Justice, 50, 7885. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2017.04.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Skeem, J. L., Marsee, M. A., Cruise, K., Munoz, L. C., Aucoin, K. J., & Morris, A. S. (2008). Assessing callous-unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: Validation of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 31(3), 241252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latzman, R. D., & Vaidya, J. G. (2013). Common and distinct associations between aggression and alcohol problems with trait disinhibition. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 35(2), 186196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-012-9330-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, T. D., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 122. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 371410. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozier, L. M., Cardinale, E. M., VanMeter, J. W., & Marsh, A. A. (2014). Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(6), 627636. https://doi.org/10.1001/Jamapsychlatry.2013.4540 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldonado-Molina, M. M., Reingle, J. M., Tobler, A. L., Jennings, W. G., & Komro, K. A. (2010). Trajectories of physical aggression among Hispanic urban adolescents and young adults: An application of latent trajectory modeling from ages 12 to 18. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 35(3), 121133. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-010-9074-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsee, M. A., Barry, C. T., Childs, K. K., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Muñoz, L. C., Aucoin, K. J., Fassnacht, G. M., Kunimatsu, M. M., & Lau, K. S. L. (2011). Assessing the forms and functions of aggression using self-report: Factor structure and invariance of the Peer Conflict Scale in youths. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 792804. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023369 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsee, M. A., & Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(6), 969981. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9147-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsee, M. A., Frick, P. J., Barry, C. T., Kimonis, E. R., Centifanti, L. C. M., & Aucoin, K. J. (2014). Profiles of the forms and functions of self-reported aggression in three adolescent samples. Development and Psychopathology, 26(3), 705720. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000339 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAuliffe, M. D., Hubbard, J. A., Rubin, R. M., Morrow, M. T., & Dearing, K. F. (2006). Reactive and proactive aggression: Stability of constructs and relations to correlates. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167(4), 365382. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.167.4.365-382 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenzie, K., Murray, A., Freeston, M., Whelan, K., & Rodgers, J. (2019). Validation of the Revised Children’s Anxiety and Depression Scales (RCADS) and RCADS short forms adapted for adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 245, 200204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.10.362 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merk, W., Orobio de Castro, B., & Koops, W. (2005). The distinction between reactive and proactive aggression: Utility for theo…: Discovery. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2(2), 197220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monahan, K. C., Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., & Mulvey, E. P. (2009). Trajectories of antisocial behavior and psychosocial maturity from adolescence to young adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 16541668. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015862 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C. C., Hubbard, J. A., Bookhout, M. K., & Mlawer, F. (2019). Relations between reactive and proactive aggression and daily emotions in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(9), 14951507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00533-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz, L. C., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008). Types of aggression, responsiveness to provocation, and callous-unemotional traits in detained adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(1), 1528. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9137-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., Obsuth, I., Zirk-Sadowski, J., Ribeaud, D., & Eisner, M. (2020). Developmental relations between ADHD symptoms and reactive versus proactive aggression across childhood and adolescence. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(12), 17011710. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054716666323 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2005). Chi-square difference testing using the Satorra–Bentler scaled chi-square. Retrieved from https://www.statmodel.com/chidiff.shtml Google Scholar
Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of Boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent Juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70(5), 11811196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ojanen, T., & Kiefer, S. (2013). Instrumental and reactive functions and overt and relational forms of aggression: Developmental trajectories and prospective associations during middle school. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(6), 514517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413503423 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paré-Ruel, M., Brendgen, M., Ouellet-Morin, I., Lupien, S., Vitaro, F., Dionne, G., & Boivin, M. (2022). Unique and interactive associations of proactive and reactive aggression with cortisol secretion. Hormones and Behavior, 137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez Fuentes, M. D. C., Molero Jurado del, M. M., Carrión Martínez, J. J., Mercader Rubio, I., & Gázquez, J. J. (2016). Sensation-seeking and impulsivity as predictors of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 12(2), 115122. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ray, J. V., & Frick, P. J. (2020). Assessing callous-unemotional traits using the total score from the inventory of callous-unemotional traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 49(2), 190199. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1504297 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome expectancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(1), 8693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, J., Colasante, T., & Malti, T. (2020). Taming anger and trusting others: Roles of skin conductance, anger regulation, and trust in children’s aggression. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38, 4258. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12304 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storvall, E. E., & Wichstrøm, L. (2003). Gender differences in changes in and stability of conduct problems from early adolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 26(4), 413429. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1971(03)00028-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urben, S., Habersaat, S., Pihet, S., Suter, M., Ridder, J., & Stéphan, P. (2018). Specific contributions of age of onset, callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity to reactive and proactive aggression in youths with conduct disorders. Psychiatric Quarterly, 89(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-017-9506-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vagos, P., Marinho, P. I., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Rodrigues, P. F. S., & Marsee, M. (2021). Measuring forms and functions of aggression in Portuguese young adults: Validation of the Peer Conflict Scale. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 39(7), 902908. https://doi.org/10.1177/07342829211018106 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walters, G. D. (2005). Proactive and reactive aggression: A lifestyle view. In Morgan, J. P. (Ed.), Psychology of aggression. (pp. 2943). Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weinberger, D. A., & Schwartz, G. E. (1990). Distress and restraint as superordinate dimensions of self-reported adjustment: A typological perspective. Journal of Personality, 58(2), 381417. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00235.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, H., Drabick, D. A., & Chen, D. (2011). Developmental trajectories of aggression from late childhood through adolescence: Similarities and differences across gender. Aggressive Behavior, 37(5), 387404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics across ages

Figure 1

Table 2. Comparison of linear and quadratic growth models for all study variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Quadratic trajectories of proactive and reactive aggression in adolescence & young adulthood.

Figure 3

Table 3. Multivariate quadratic growth model results for proactive and reactive aggression