Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T01:59:13.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Orienting in normal and pathological development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Mary K. Rothbart*
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
Michael I. Posner
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
John Rosicky
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Mary K. Rothbart, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403.

Abstract

The term plasticity is often confined to changes due to learning. We believe, however, that the rapid lability of human behavior as different mental contents receive attention represents an important form of plasticity. Recent research in temperament and cognitive neuroscience provides an account of the development of a specific neural system involved in attentional self-regulation. In this paper, we concentrate on a network involved in orienting. We describe what is currently known about this network and its development during the first years of life. This network interacts with other attentional networks that come to control orienting to remembered events as well as those currently present. Orienting reflects processes that are both deeply biological and also open to cultural influences through learning. Attention is influenced by the current emotional state and is also important in achieving control over the emotional state. We present evidence on how learning influences eye position between 4 and 18 months of life and indicate influences of orienting on important aspects of infant behavior. Like any biological system, problems can arise in these mechanisms. We speculate on how individual differences in distress proneness and orienting may relate to the development of psychopathology.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3.5 and 4.5 month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655664.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875890.Google Scholar
Bates, J. E. (1987). Temperament in infancy. In Osofsky, J. D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 11011149). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 4976). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bronson, G. (1974). The postnatal growth of visual capacity. Child Development, 4, 873890.Google Scholar
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Annals of Neurology, 22, 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clohessy, A. B. (1993). Anticipatory eye movements in infants and adults: Using visual cues to predict event locations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Clohessy, A. B., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Vecera, S. (1991). The development of inhibition of return in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 345350.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R. (1994). Temperament and personality. Current Opinion in Neurobiology: Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 266273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M., Meizin, F. M., Shulman, G. L., & Petersen, S. E. (1993). A PET study of visuospatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 12021226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 189202.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 11281139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal, affect and attention as components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 958966.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1990). Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural basis of inhibiting control of reaching. In Diamond, A. (Ed.), The development and neural basis of higher cognitive functions (pp. 637669). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Drevets, W. C., Videen, T. O., Price, J. L., Preskorn, S. H., Carmichael, S. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1992). A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 36283641.Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183201.Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakie, P. S. (1988). Topography of cognition: Parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 137156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1987). The Oxford companion to the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. G., Fisch, R. O., & Moline, S. (1984). The effects of a pacifying stimulus on behavioral adrenocortical responses to circumcision in the new born. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 3438.Google Scholar
Haith, M. (1980). Rules that babies look by. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Haith, M., Hazan, C., & Goodman, G. S. (1988). Expectation and anticipation of dynamic visual events by 3.5-month-old babies. Child Development, 59, 467479.Google Scholar
Halsted, N. (1990). Developmental mechanisms of soothing and orienting in infancy. Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Harman, C. (1994). The interaction of distress and attention in early infancy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Harman, C., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Thomas-Thrapp, L. (1994). Development of orienting to objects and locations in human infants. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 301318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, A. B., & Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. (1986). Personality, life events and subclinical depression in students. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 469478.Google Scholar
Hinchliffe, M. K., Lancashire, M., & Roberts, F. J. (1971). A study of eye-contact changes in depressed and recovered psychiatric patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 119, 213215.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, R. M., Klerman, G. L., Keller, M. B., & Andreason, N. C. (1986). Personality of recovered patients with bipolar affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 11, 8189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hood, B. M. (1993). Inhibition of return produced by covert shifts of visual attention in 6 month old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 245254.Google Scholar
Hood, B. M., & Atkinson, J. (1993). Disengaging visual attention in infant and adult. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 405422.Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. (1981). Eye, brain and vision. New York: Scientific American Library W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (1990). Cortical maturation and the development of attention in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 8195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1993). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 335344.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Facilitation of saccades toward a covertly attended location in early infancy. Psychological Science, 5, 9093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, P. R., Mulder, R. T., & Cloninger, C. R. (1994). Temperament predicts clomipramine and desipramine responses to major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 30, 3546.Google Scholar
Kenofer, B. (1982). Sustained visual orienting in 13-month old infants. Masters' thesis, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Kety, S. S. (1970). The biogenic amines in the central nervous system: Their possible role in arousal, emotion and learning. In Schmidt, F. O. (Ed.), The neurosciences: Second study program (pp. 334336). New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. H. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
McHaffie, J. G., Kao, C.-O., & Stein, B. E. (1989). Nociceptive neurons in rat superior colliculus: Response properties, topography and functional implications. Journal of Neurophysiology, 63, 510523.Google Scholar
Macleod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorder.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 1520.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1991). Towards a developmental cognitive science. In Diamond, A. (Ed.), The development and neural basis of higher cognitive functions (pp. 131). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Merzenich, M. M., & Sameshima, K. (1993). Cortical plasticity and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3, 187196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 132.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In Boll, T. & Bryant, B. (Eds.), Master lectures in clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 171202). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1990). Hierarchical distributed networks in the neuropsychology of selective attention. In Carramaza, A. (Ed.), Cognitive neuropsychology and neurolinguistics: Advances in models of cognitive function and impairment (pp. 187210). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1980). Attention and the control of movements. In Stelmach, G. E. & Requin, J. (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 243258). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends in Neuroscience, 17, 7579.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1977). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set from spatial location. In Pick, H. J. (Ed.), Modes of perception (pp. 137157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Books.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1980). The development of attentional mechanisms. In Flowers, J. H. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 149). Lincoln: Nebraska University Press.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Attention and conscious experience. In Milner, A. D. & Rugg, M. D. (Eds.), The neuropsychology of consciousness (pp. 91112). London: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Constructing neuronal theories of mind. In Koch, C. & Davis, J. (Eds.), High level neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 183199). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rafal, R., Calaabresi, P., Brennan, C., & Scioloto, T. (1989). Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 673685.Google Scholar
Rafal, R., Smith, W., Krantz, J., Cohen, A., & Brennan, C. (1990). Extrageniculate vision in hemianopic humans: Saccade inhibition by signals in the blind field. Science, 250, 118121.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569578.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament and development. In Kohnstam, G., Bates, J., & Rothbart, M. K. (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 187248). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 2139.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. (in press). Temperament, attention and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 1. Theory and method. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & O'Boyle, C. G. (1992). Self regulation and emotion in infancy. New Directions for Child Development, 55, 724.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Talbot, J. D., Marrett, A., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., Bushnell, M. C., & Duncan, G. H. (1991). Multiple representation of pain in human cerebral cortex. Science, 251, 13551357.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. (1992). Perceiving and reperceiving objects. American Psychologist, 47, 862875.Google Scholar
Valenza, E., Simione, F., & Umilta, C. (in press). Inhibition of return in newborn infants. Infant Behavior and Development.Google Scholar
Vecera, S. P., & Johnson, M. H. (in press). Eye gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition.Google Scholar
Vecera, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1991). Development of spontaneous alternation in infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 351354.Google Scholar
Vogt, B. A., Finch, D. M., & Olson, C. R. (1992). Overview: Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: The anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 435443.Google Scholar
Waxer, P. (1974). Nonverbal cues for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 319322.Google Scholar
Witte, E. A. (1994). The effects of pharmacological changes in the catecholaminergic and cholinergic systems on arousal and covert orienting. Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar