Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

How does restricted and repetitive behavior relate to language and cognition in typical development?

  • Fionnuala Larkin (a1), Elizabeth Meins (a1), Luna C. M. Centifanti (a2), Charles Fernyhough (a2) and Susan R. Leekam (a3)...

Abstract

Relations between restricted and repetitive behavior at age 26 months and children's concurrent (N = 203) and later (n = 161) social cognition and language development were investigated. Restricted and repetitive behavior was assessed using two scales: sensory and motor repetitive behaviors and rigidity/routines/restricted interests. Language was assessed at ages 26 and 51 months; social cognition was assessed at ages 26 (symbolic play) and 51 and 61 months (theory of mind). Sensory and motor repetitive behavior was negatively related to children's (a) language performance at 26 and 51 months, (b) instructed symbolic play at 26 months, and (c) theory of mind performance at 51 and 61 months. Path analyses showed that children's sensory and motor repetitive behavior at age 26 months was related to lower receptive verbal ability and theory of mind at 51 months, which led to lower theory of mind at 61 months. Rigidity/routines/restricted interests at 26 months were unrelated to concurrent and later social cognition and language. These results are discussed in terms of the pathways via which sensory and motor repetitive behavior might impact negatively on development.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Elizabeth Meins, Department of Psychology, University of York, York, United Kingdom; E-mail: elizabeth.meins@york.ac.uk.

Footnotes

Hide All

This research was supported by Grants R000239456 and RES-000-23-1073 from the Economic and Social Research Council. We thank the families for their generous participation in this longitudinal study; Bronia Arnott, Lorna Elliott, Beth Liddle, Alexandra Hearn, Lucia Vittorini, Kathryn Parkinson, and Julia Candy for their valuable contribution to data collection and coding; and the families for their generous participation.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Arnott, B., McConachie, H., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Le Couteur, A., Turner, M., et al. (2010). The frequency of restricted and repetitive behaviors in a community sample of 15-month-old infants. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 223229.
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13111320.
Barber, A. B., Wetherby, A. M., & Chambers, N. W. (2012). Brief report: Repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorder and developmentally similar peers: A follow up to Watt et al. (2008). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 20062012.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 3746.
Bedford, R., Pickles, A., Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., et al. (2014). Additive effects of social and non-social attention during infancy relate to later autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17, 612620.
Berkson, G., & Tupa, M. (2000). Early development of stereotyped and self-injurous behaviors. Journal of Early Intervention, 23, 119.
Bishop, S. L., Richler, J., & Lord, C. (2006). Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 247267.
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behaviour in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237243.
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1997). Preschool Language Scale—3 (UK). London: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Company.
Brunsdon, V. E. A., & Happé, F. (2014). Exploring the “fractionation” of autism at the cognitive level. Autism, 18, 1730.
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 11051122.
Cevikaslan, A., Evans, D. W., Dedeoglu, C., Kalaca, S., & Yazgan, Y. (2014). A cross-sectional survey of repetitive behaviors and restricted interests in a typically-developing Turkish child population. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 45, 472482.
Charman, T., & Swettenham, J. (2001). The relationship between repetitive behaviours and social impairments in pre-school children with autism: Implications for developmental theory. In Burack, J., Charman, T., Yirmiya, N., & Zelazo, P. R. (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 325345). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale (2nd ed.). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
Dziak, J. J., Coffman, D. L., Lanza, S. T., & Li, R. (2012). Sensitivity and specificity of information criteria. State College, PA: Pennsylvania State University, Methodology Center and Department of Statistics.
Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 8187.
Evans, D. W., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, D., King, R. A., et al. (1997). Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: The prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Development, 68, 5868.
Evans, D. W., Lewis, M. D., & Iobst, E. (2004). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain and Cognition, 55, 220234.
Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The “fractionable autism triad”: A review of evidence from behavioral, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychology Review, 18, 287304.
Harrop, C., McConachie, H., Emsley, R., Leadbitter, K., Green, G., & PACT Consortium (2014). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders and typical development: Cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 12071219.
Hayes, A. F. (2013) Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.
Hobson, J. A., Hobson, R. P., Cheung, Y., & Caló, S. (2015). Symbolizing as interpersonally grounded shifts in meaning: Social play in children with and without autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 4252.
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On acquiring knowledge about people and the capacity to pretend: Response to Leslie. Psychological Review, 97, 114121.
Hobson, R. P. (1991). Against the theory of “theory of mind.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 3351.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.
Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229261.
Kravitz, H., & Boehm, J. J. (1971). Rhythmic habit patterns in infancy: Their sequence, age of onset, and frequency. Child Development, 42, 399413.
Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: A review of research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 562593.
Leekam, S. R., Tandos, J., McConachie, H., Meins, E., Le Couteur, A., Parkinson, K., et al. (2007). Repetitive behaviours in typically-developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 11311138.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 325339.
Mandy, W. P. L., & Skuse, D. H. (2008). Research review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviour and activities? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 795808.
McDonald, R., Green, G., Mansfield, R., Geckeler, A., Gardenier, N., Anderson, J., et al. (2007). Stereotypy in young children with autism and typically-developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, 266277.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & de Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, 84, 17771790.
Meins, E., & Russell, J. (1997). Security and symbolic play: The relation between security of attachment and executive capacity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 6376.
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2008–2014). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles: Author.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behaviour Research Methods, 40, 879891.
Ray-Subramanian, C. E., & Weismer, S. E. (2012). Receptive and expressive language as predictors of restricted and repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 21132120.
Rogers, S. (2009). What are infant siblings teaching us about autism in infancy? Autism Research, 2, 125137.
Symons, F. J., Sperry, L. A., Dropik, P. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2005). The early development of stereotypy and self-injury: A review of research methods. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 144158.
Thelen, E. (1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology, 17, 237257.
Tregay, J., Gilmour, J., & Charman, T. (2009). Childhood rituals and executive functions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 283296.
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523541.
Wolff, J. J., Botterton, K. N., Dager, S. R., Elison, J. T., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., et al. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behaviour in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 945953.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed