Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Information:

  • Access
  • Cited by 18

Actions:

      • Send article to Kindle

        To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

        Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

        Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

        Undernutrition, fatty acid and micronutrient status in relation to cognitive performance in Indian school children: a cross-sectional study
        Available formats
        ×

        Send article to Dropbox

        To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

        Undernutrition, fatty acid and micronutrient status in relation to cognitive performance in Indian school children: a cross-sectional study
        Available formats
        ×

        Send article to Google Drive

        To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

        Undernutrition, fatty acid and micronutrient status in relation to cognitive performance in Indian school children: a cross-sectional study
        Available formats
        ×
Export citation

Abstract

While undernutrition and anaemia have previously been linked to poor development of children, relatively little is known about the role of B-vitamins and fatty acids on cognition. The present study aims to explore the associations between indicators of body size, fatty acid and micronutrient status on cognitive performance in 598 Indian school children aged 6–10 years. Baseline data of a clinical study were used to assess these associations by analyses of variance adjusting for age, sex, school, maternal education and cognitive tester. The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children II was used to measure four cognitive domains, including fluid reasoning, short-term memory, retrieval ability and cognitive speediness. Scores were combined into an overall measure, named mental processing index (MPI). Body size indicators and Hb concentrations were significantly positively related to cognitive domains and MPI, such that increases of 1 sd in height-for-age and weight-for-age z-scores would each translate into a 0·09 sd increase in MPI, P = 0·0006 and 0·002, respectively. A 10 g/l increase in Hb concentrations would translate into a 0·08 sd increase in MPI, P = 0·0008. Log-transformed vitamin B12 concentrations were significantly inversely associated with short-term memory, retrieval ability and MPI (β (95 % CI) = − 0·124 ( − 0·224, − 0·023), P = 0·02). Other indicators of Fe, iodine, folate and fatty acid status were not significantly related to cognition. Our findings for body size, fatty acids and micronutrients were in agreement with previous observational studies. The inverse association of vitamin B12 with mental development was unexpected and needed further study.

Low intakes of energy, protein and other nutrients, together with high infection rates and poor socio-economic status may lead to linear growth retardation and impaired child development(1). Cross-sectional studies have linked stunting (short stature for age) and low weight-for-age to poor development and school achievement in infants and children(2). The detrimental effects of undernutrition early in life ( < 2 years of age) on intellectual development seem irreversible and remain apparent during childhood and adolescence(3, 4).

The n-3 fatty acid DHA and the n-6 fatty acid arachidonic acid are important structural components of the human central nervous system(5) and play a role in brain functioning through their involvement in aspects of neuron function and of neurotransmitter synthesis(6). These two fatty acids can be synthesised by the human body from the α-linolenic acid (ALA) and linoleic acid. However, dietary intake of the n-3 fatty acid ALA in children is considered to be low(7, 8), which possibly limits adequate cognitive functioning. In fact, high fish intake during pregnancy has been associated with better cognitive development of infants(911) and maternal and infant DHA supplementation may benefit visual, motor and mental development of infants and young children(1218). For healthy children >2 years of age such evidence is currently limited(19).

Among the micronutrients, Fe and iodine interventions have been shown to improve intelligence scores of children(20, 21). Fe is needed for the formation of Hb for adequate oxygen transport in the human body. In the brain, Fe is required for myelination and neurotransmitter synthesis(22). Iodine is an important component of the thyroid hormones, thyroxin and tri-iodothyronine, which plays a major role in the growth and development, function and maintenance of the central and peripheral nervous system(23). For the B-vitamins, however, little research has been conducted to investigate whether these vitamins are of influence on mental development in children. Vitamin B12 (cobalamin) deficiency has been associated with lower scores on cognitive tests in Guatemalan(24) and Dutch(25) children. Folate is important for closure of the neural tube during fetal development(26), but no studies have investigated the role of folate on cognitive functioning in children after birth. In the brain, folate is required for neurotransmitter production and myelination(27, 28). Because of the interactions with folate metabolism, vitamin B12 is indirectly involved in neurotransmitter synthesis. Furthermore, the vitamin B12 cofactors adenosylcobalamin and methylcobalamin are involved in myelination of the spinal cord and the brain(29).

The primary objective of the present study is to investigate the associations between indicators of body size, fatty acid status, and Fe, iodine and B-vitamin status on overall cognitive performance in 598 Indian school-age children. Secondary, we will explore the relationships of the nutritional parameters with specific cognitive domains known to be sensitive to differences in nutritional status in children. We hypothesise that the indicators of body size, fatty acid and micronutrient status (Fe, iodine, folate and vitamin B12) will be positively related to overall cognitive performance and specific cognitive domains.

Experimental methods

The Children's Health And Mental Performance Influenced by Optimal Nutrition study was designed to investigate the efficacy of foods fortified with n-3 fatty acids and micronutrients on improving intellectual performance and growth in Indian school children(30). The baseline data of the present study, collected in the period between November 2005 and February 2006, were used to assess the associations between height-for-age (HAZ) and weight-for-age z-scores (WAZ), Hb concentration and indicators of n-3 and n-6 fatty acid, Fe, iodine, folate and vitamin B12 status and cognitive performance. These nutritional parameters were selected based on their possible relationship with children's mental development.

Subjects

Two primary schools serving children from a poor socio-economic background in Bangalore city, India were selected for participation in the study. Almost all children living in the surrounding communities attended these schools, where they were taught in the local Kannada language. Before study start, parents or caretakers of all children aged 6–10 years, attending grades 2–5 of these schools were invited for a meeting during which the study procedures were explained to them. Informed, written consent from the parents and verbal assent from their children was obtained from 645 parent–child pairs. Children were included in the study if they were: (1) apparently healthy, without any chronic illness and physical/mental handicaps; (2) not severely anaemic (Hb < 80 g/l); (3) not severely undernourished ( < − 3 sd for WAZ and HAZ-scores of National Health Centre for Statistics/WHO standards(31)); (4) not intending to use micronutrients supplements during the study; (5) planning to reside in the study area during the next 12 months. Children who were frequently absent from school (>40 d during 6 months before start of the study) and children who took micronutrient supplements in the period of 3 months before the study start were excluded. A total of 598 children were enrolled in the study. Details on the enrolment, including a flow chart of children recruited in the study have been published elsewhere(30).

Socio-demographic information

Socio-demographic information on household composition, parental education, income and use of fortified foods was collected by a structured questionnaire that was administered to the mother or primary caretaker of the subjects. The age of the children was verified by the school records.

Cognitive performance

Cognitive performance was evaluated using age-appropriate, validated psychometric tests that were administered by seven masters-level psychologists in Kannada language. The psychologists were trained extensively during 3 weeks before the study to ensure standardisation in the test administration and scoring procedures. The cognitive test battery was administered to each child on a single day over three sessions of which two took place in the morning and one in the afternoon. Care was taken to ensure all the children had breakfast before testing began in the morning since omitting breakfast is known to impair cognitive performance(32). The cognitive test battery consisted of eleven subtests, including six core tests of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, second edition for children 3–18 years (pattern reasoning, triangles, rover, number recall, word order, atlantis)(33), two tests from Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-4 (picture arrangement, coding) and three additional tests from Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (auditory-verbal learning test), NEPSY (neuropsychological assessment tool, verbal fluency) and number cancellation, which was specifically designed for the study. The eleven subtests covered four cognitive domains as specified in Carroll's model as described in the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, second edition manual(33), including fluid reasoning, short-term memory, retrieval ability and cognitive speediness (Fig. 1). These domains were chosen because they have shown to be influenced by previous nutritional interventions(34). The test battery underwent an extensive adaptation process to ensure its suitability in the local cultural context(35). For each subtest, a sum score was calculated and converted into a standardised z-score. The domain score was composed by taking the average of standardised z-scores for the tests constituting a domain. The average of the domain scores named the mental processing index (MPI) was a composite measure of overall cognitive performance based on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, second edition manual(33). Our model of clustering of individual sum scores to form a composite score in four separate cognitive domains showed good validity assessed by structural equation modelling techniques(36).

Fig. 1 Clustering of cognitive tests in domain scores. Fluid reasoning involves basic processes of reasoning and other mental activities that depend only minimally on learning and acculturation; short-term memory is an ability that requires apprehending and holding information in immediate awareness briefly and then using that information within a few seconds; retrieval ability comprises the capacity to store information in long-term memory and to retrieve that information fluently and efficiently; cognitive speediness measures the ability of rapid cognitive processing of information involving attention.

Anthropometry

Anthropometric measurements were conducted in duplicate according to standard techniques(37) by trained research assistants. Height was recorded to the nearest 0·1 cm using a locally made stadiometer (BioRad, Chennai, India) that was fixed to a wall. Body weight was recorded to the nearest 0·1 kg using a digital weighing scale (Breuer, Germany). During the measurements, children wore their school uniform and no shoes, caps or hats. HAZ and WAZ were computed by data on height, weight, age and sex using the National Health Centre for Statistics/WHO growth reference data(31). Children with HAZ and WAZ < − 2 sd of this reference median were classified as stunted and underweight, respectively. We did not include weight-for-height z-scores, because National Health Centre for Statistics/WHO reference data were lacking for children >10 years of age, which concerned 57 children aged 10–11 years in the present study.

Biochemical indicators

A whole blood sample (10 ml) was collected in the morning from non-fasted children by venepuncture in an EDTA vacutainer. A spot urine sample was also collected in a sterile plastic container, and the samples were transported to the laboratory on ice. Care was taken to limit the exposure of the samples to light. Hb concentrations were determined within 4 h of collection using an AcT Diff2 Counter (Beckman Coulter Inc., Fullerton, CA, USA). One aliquot of whole blood for erythrocyte folate estimation was immediately treated with freshly prepared 1 % ascorbic acid. The remaining blood was immediately centrifuged (3000 rpm, 10 min, 4°C), and the plasma was stored in 2 ml eppendorf tubes at − 80°C until analysis. One millilitre of erythrocytes was washed with 5 ml saline containing EDTA (1 litre normal saline+0·00 324 g disodium EDTA), flushed under nitrogen and stored at − 80°C until analysis for fatty acid content. Serum ferritin was measured by an enzyme immunoassay (Access® 2 Beckman Coulter autoanalyser, Brea, CA, USA)(37) against an external 3-level control material (WHO Standard 80/578; Ramco Laboratories Inc., Houston, TX, USA). Serum soluble transferrin receptor (sTfR) was measured by using an enzyme immunoassay (Ramco Laboratories Inc.) with two-level control materials provided by the manufacturer. C-reactive protein was analysed by a turbidimetric method (Roche Hitachi 902, Indianapolis, IN, USA)(38). Plasma vitamin B12 and red blood cell folate were analysed using a chemiluminescence system (ACS:180, Bayer Diagnostics, Tarrytown, NY, USA)(39, 40). Fatty acid content of erythrocyte membrane phospholipids was analysed using GC with a flame ionization detector (Varian 3800, Palo Alto, CA, USA). The procedure involved the extraction of total lipids, isolation of phospholipid fraction by TLC and transmethylation of phospholipids(4143). The fatty acid methyl esters were separated by chain length and degree of saturation by injection onto a 50 m × 0·2 mm capillary column (Varian, Palo Alto, CA, USA) with nitrogen as the carrier gas. Urinary iodine was measured using the Sandell–Kolthoff reaction as modified by Pino et al. (44). Satisfactory agreement in urinary iodine was obtained on urine samples at four different concentrations measured and the Ensuring the Quality of Urinary Iodine Procedures, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (Atlanta, GA, USA). The following criteria were used to define micronutrient deficiencies: anaemia: Hb < 115 g/l(45); Fe deficiency: serum ferritin < 15 mg/l and/or sTfR >7·6 mg/l(46); folate deficiency: erythrocyte folate < 305 nmol/l(47); vitamin B12 deficiency: plasma vitamin B12 < 148 pmol/l(48); iodine deficiency: urinary iodine < 100 μg/l(49).

Statistical analyses

Values for serum ferritin concentrations from the subjects with elevated C-reactive protein (>10 mg/l) were excluded from statistical analyses. Body Fe stores were calculated from serum ferritin and sTfR concentrations using the formula by Cook et al. (50). Differences in mean cognitive outcomes between boys and girls, schools and different levels of education of the mother were assessed by t tests. Distributions of parameters of fatty acid status, serum ferritin and sTfR, erythrocyte folate, plasma vitamin B12 and urinary iodine were normalised by natural logarithm (ln) transformation before analysis. Associations between the nutritional parameters and the cognitive scores were analysed using ANOVA (SAS General Linear Modelling procedure) taking into account age, sex, school, maternal education level and assessor of cognitive tests as covariates. All available data were analysed, missing values were not replaced. All analyses were performed using Statistical Analysis Software version 9.1 statistical software package (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the ethics committees at St John's National Academy of Health Sciences, Bangalore, India and Wageningen University, The Netherlands. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all subjects and verbal assent from all the subjects. Verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded.

Results

Five hundred and ninety eight children completed the baseline measurements on cognitive performance, anthropometry and Hb concentrations. Data on biochemical indicators of micronutrient and fatty acid status were available for at least 529 and 541 children, respectively. The socio-demographic characteristics and nutritional status of the subjects are presented in Table 1. Mean age of the children was 8·7 (sd 1·2) years and 49 % of them were boys. Nearly, half of the mothers were uneducated and median family income was 2700 Indian rupees per month, which is close to the poverty line of US$2 per d. Twenty-two percent of the children were stunted and 30 % were underweight. The prevalence of anaemia was 9 %, while that of Fe, folate, vitamin B12 and iodine deficiencies were 31, 17, 23 and 47 %, respectively.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population

(Mean values and standard deviations; median and percentile values)

Associations of covariates with cognitive performance

Age was significantly positively related with all cognitive outcomes (β = 0·31 (95 % CI 0·27, 0·35), P < 0·0001 for MPI). Mean cognitive scores for boys and girls are presented in Table 2. Scores on the domains of retrieval ability and cognitive speediness and MPI were significantly lower in boys compared with girls (P < 0·001). These findings did not change when scores were corrected for age (data not shown). There was a significant difference in performance on short-term memory and retrieval ability between the two schools (data not shown). Children of mothers with < 6 years of education had significantly lower MPI scores compared with children of mothers with ≥ 6 years of education (means were − 0·05 (sd 0·66) v. 0·07 (sd 0·63), P = 0·03, respectively).

Table 2 Cognitive domain scores for boys and girls*

(Mean values and standard deviations)

* Domain scores are expressed in z-scores.

Scores between boys and girls were significantly different, t test (P < 0·001).

Associations of nutritional parameters with cognitive performance

Table 3 provides an overview of the associations between the nutritional parameters and the indicators of cognitive performance. Scatter plots of the correlations between the MPI and HAZ, WAZ, Hb and vitamin B12 concentrations are shown in Fig. 2. HAZ scores were significantly positively related to all cognitive domains and MPI. WAZ were significantly positively associated with all cognitive parameters, except cognitive speediness. The associations of HAZ and WAZ would in theory mean that an increase of 1 sd in HAZ and WAZ would correspond with 0·09 sd increase in MPI, P = 0·0006 and P = 0·002, respectively.

Table 3 Overview of associations of nutritional factors with cognitive performance*

(β coefficients and 95 % confidence intervals)

* Using a model adjusted for age, sex, school, maternal education and assessor of cognitive tests. The R 2 of the models with the different nutritional indicators ranged from 0·34 to 0·37 for MPI, 0·37 to 0·39 for fluid reasoning, 0·08 to 0·10 for short-term memory, 0·25 to 0·27 for retrieval ability and 0·34 to 0·36 for cognitive speediness.

Variables were normalised by natural logarithm transformation.

Fatty acids were measured in the erythrocyte membranes in the phospholipid fraction.

Fig. 2 Scatter plots of correlations between the mental processing index and height-for-age z-score (r 0·10, P = 0·012), weight-for-age z-score (r 0·11, P = 0·007), Hb concentration (r 0·21, P < 0·0001) and log-transformed vitamin B12 (r − 0·09, P = 0·046).

No significant relationships were detected between linoleic acid, arachidonic acid, EPA and DHA and any of the cognitive parameters. ALA was significantly inversely related to the MPI, but no significant associations were observed with the separate cognitive domains.

Hb concentrations were significantly positively related to all cognitive domains and MPI. Our findings suggest that an increase of 10 g/l in Hb concentration would translate into a 0·08 sd increase in MPI, P = 0·0008. There was a significant inverse association between sTfR concentrations and retrieval ability. Other indicators of Fe status were not significantly related to cognitive performance. Similarly, there were no significant associations between urinary iodine concentrations and cognitive parameters. In contrast, significantly inverse relationships were found between erythrocyte folate concentrations and fluid reasoning (β = − 0·10 (95 % CI − 0·19, − 0·01) and short-term memory ( − 0·11 (95 % CI − 0·23, 0·02)). However, when vitamin B12 status was added to the model, these inverse associations were not significant anymore for fluid reasoning ( − 0·07 (95 % CI − 0·17, 0·02) and for short-term memory ( − 0·08 (95 % CI − 0·21, 0·05)). Vitamin B12 concentrations were significantly inverse related to short-term memory and retrieval ability and the MPI. These associations remained significant after further adjusting for Hb and folate status and HAZ (β (95 % CI) were − 0·19 (95 % CI − 0·36, − 0·03) for short-term memory; − 0·20 (95 % CI − 0·33, − 0·08) for retrieval ability; − 0·12 (95 % CI − 0·22, − 0·02), P = 0·02 for MPI).

Discussion

The present study shows that indicators of body size, HAZ and WAZ and Hb concentrations were significantly positively related to various cognitive domain scores and MPI, while plasma vitamin B12 concentrations were significantly inversely associated with short-term memory and retrieval ability and MPI. Other indicators of Fe, folate, iodine and fatty acid status were not significantly related to cognitive performance.

Strengths of this cross-sectional study were the availability of biochemical parameters of micronutrient and fatty acid status in a relatively large sample of >500 children from a low socio-economic background. The sample is the representative for school children aged 6–10 years from poor socio-economic classes in Bangalore city and the surrounding peri-urban areas, based on a similar prevalence of anaemia measured and similar average heights and weights in our studies and other studies conducted in children in Bangalore(51). The cognitive test battery was thoroughly adapted to local language and culture and showed good internal and external validity, which is essential to detect any associations between cognitive functioning and nutritional status(52). In addition, we chose to assess the cognitive abilities that have been shown to be influenced by nutritional interventions before(53).

A limitation of a cross-sectional study design is the inability for causal inference. Furthermore, the high number of comparisons made between nutritional and cognitive variables may have yielded false-positive findings (type I error). However, we tried to limit the number of comparisons by the use of composite scores for the cognitive tests. In addition, we aimed to look for patterns among our findings, such as the consistent positive association of HAZ with all cognitive parameters. Another limitation of the study was the finding that our overall model explained only 10–40 % of the variation in cognitive parameters. Genetic variation and environmental factors such as socio-emotional stimulation at home may account for this unexplained variation. In additional analyses we explored whether the interactions of age and sex with the nutritional indicators could explain any variation in cognitive test scores, but the results of these analyses did not yield further insights.

In agreement with our findings, lower HAZ, reflecting longer term undernutrition, has previously been associated with poorer cognitive performance in younger (1–3 years)(5456) and school children(5759). Moreover, intervention studies have demonstrated that protein–energy supplementation in young children benefits cognitive development on the longer term(4, 60, 61) and therefore an adequate intake of energy and protein is required for optimal development.

Erythrocyte fatty acid status was unrelated to cognitive performance, which is in line with findings from a cohort study in children aged 7 years(62). Possibly, the range in fatty acid status among the children was too narrow to determine effects on cognition. It may also be that erythrocyte or plasma fatty acid status does not resemble brain fatty acid status at school age when most brain growth has been completed. A study in human subjects estimated that DHA requirements of the brain are rather low and the authors suggested that the liver may synthesise sufficient amounts of DHA to maintain brain DHA concentrations, provided that dietary intake of the precursor ALA is adequate(63). Moreover, animal studies indicated that synthesis of DHA in the liver is enhanced and the turnover of DHA in the brain is reduced when diets were low in ALA and free of DHA(63). Thus, intake and erythrocyte concentrations of n-3 fatty acids may not be related to brain function. Besides, there is some evidence that children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorders have lower plasma–erythrocyte DHA and higher linoleic and arachidonic acid concentrations than control children(6467), which could be attributed to differences in fatty acid metabolism(68). Therefore, more research is needed to investigate whether specific subgroups of children may be sensitive to fatty acid interventions and whether fatty acids may predominantly influence certain aspects of behaviour, such as attention.

For Hb, we showed a very small but significant positive relationship with mental performance. However, for the other parameters of Fe status, no such relationships could be detected. Possibly, this relationship becomes only apparent when Fe deficiency has caused anaemia, which was the case in only 6 % of the present study population. This has also been reported in a review of literature of observational studies showing that (Fe deficient) anaemic children have poorer cognitive development and school performance than non-anaemic children, and it was concluded that it is unclear whether Fe deficiency without anaemia impairs mental performance(69). In contrast, Fe supplementation has been shown to improve mental performance in children >2 years of age in (Fe deficient) anaemic as well as in non-anaemic children(20, 21), indicating that extra Fe may also be beneficial for development of non-anaemic children. The higher cognitive scores with increasing Hb concentrations found in the present study, suggest that the Hb level for optimal mental performance may be higher than the current definition of anaemia ( < 115 g/l).

Against our expectations, both folate and vitamin B12 were inversely associated with some of the cognitive domain scores. For folate these inverse relationships disappeared after controlling for vitamin B12 status, while for vitamin B12 the inverse associations with memory remained significant even after controlling for folate, Hb and height-for-age. Our findings are in contrast with two earlier observational studies indicating that children with lower plasma vitamin B12 concentrations had poorer cognitive test scores(24, 25) and could be due to chance. In elderly, however, eight studies did not show significant associations between plasma vitamin B12 and cognitive test performance(70) and one study showed an inverse relationship(71). Our finding and the observations in elderly contradict to the overt clinical signs of vitamin B12 deficiency of neurological damage. Therefore, it has been questioned whether plasma vitamin B12 is a suitable indicator to study effects on cognition(70, 72). It is of interest to investigate whether higher plasma homocysteine concentrations are related to poorer mental performance in children, as has been observed in elderly(70, 71). In both children and adults, plasma homocysteine concentrations are increasing when folate and vitamin B12 intake are low(73) and elevated homocysteine may impair cognitive functioning through neurotoxic and vasotoxic effects(74). Also other indicators of vitamin B12 status, such as holotranscobalamin and methylmalonic acid may be worth evaluating in future research(72).

In addition, we could speculate on other confounding factors that influence the relationship between higher plasma vitamin B12 concentrations and poorer cognitive performance. Possibly, consumption of animal products infected with pathogens or vegetables contaminated with vitamin B12-producing bacteria from manure may improve vitamin B12 status(75, 76) and simultaneously increase the risk of disease, resulting in poor school attendance and impaired cognition. However, no literature is available to support this hypothesis.

Despite the evidence in literature that iodine deficiency is detrimental to cognitive development(77) and that iodine supplementation improves cognitive functioning in children(78), we failed to detect any association between urinary iodine concentrations and cognition, which may be due to day-to-day within subject variation in iodine excretion in urine(49).

In conclusion, findings of the present study are in agreement with other observational studies showing that undernutrition (lower HAZ and WAZ) and lower Hb concentrations adversely influence cognitive performance in school-age children, while serum ferritin and sTfR concentrations, and indicators of iodine, folate and fatty acid status were unrelated and an inverse association was found for vitamin B12 and memory. Future research is needed to elucidate the role of B-vitamins and homocysteine in cognitive development of children and to investigate whether fatty acid status at school age may be of influence on specific cognitive functions not measured in the present study, such as attention.

Acknowledgements

We are most grateful to the principals and teachers of the schools, the children and their parents for their participation in the study. We want to express our thanks to all our colleagues from St Johns Research Institute and Unilever who were involved in data collection and biochemical analyses, and to Maike Malda and Prof Fons van de Vijver, University of Tilburg, The Netherlands for the adaptation of the cognitive test battery and training of psychologists. Funding. The study was supported by Unilever Netherlands BV. Conflict of interest. A. E., H. v. d. K. and S. J. M. O. are employees of Unilever. All other authors do not have any conflict of interest. Author's responsibilities. All authors were involved in the design of the study and statistical analysis plan. A. E. and S. M. were responsible for data collection and overall study management. A. E. and H. v. d. K. performed the statistical analysis. A. E. wrote the manuscript. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript.

References

1Martorell, R, Mendoza, F & Castillo, R (1988) Poverty and stature in children. In Linear Growth Retardation in Less Developed Countries, pp. 5773 [Waterlow, JC, editor]. New York: Raven Press.
2Grantham-McGregor, S & Baker-Henningham, H (2005) Review of the evidence linking protein and energy to mental development. Public Health Nutr 8, 11911201.
3Chang, SM, Walker, SP, Grantham-McGregor, S, et al. (2002) Early childhood stunting and later behaviour and school achievement. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43, 775783.
4Walker, SP, Chang, SM, Powell, CA, et al. (2005) Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: Prospective Cohort Study. Lancet 366, 18041807.
5Martinez, M (1992) Tissue levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids during early human development. J Pediatr 120, S129S138.
6Yehuda, S (2003) Omega-6/omega-3 ratio and brain-related functions. World Rev Nutr Diet 92, 3756.
7Elmadfa, I, Weichselbaum, E, Konig, J, et al. (2005) European nutrition and health report 2004. Forum Nutr 1220.
8Kris-Etherton, PM, Innis, S, American Dietetic Associations, et al. (2007) Position of the American Dietetic Association and Dietitians of Canada: dietary fatty acids. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 15991611.
9Daniels, JL, Longnecker, MP, Rowland, AS, et al. (2004) Fish intake during pregnancy and early cognitive development of offspring. Epidemiology 15, 394402.
10Oken, E, Wright, RO, Kleinman, KP, et al. (2005) Maternal fish consumption, hair mercury, and infant cognition in a U.S. Cohort. Environ Health Perspect 113, 13761380.
11Hibbeln, JR, Davis, JM, Steer, C, et al. (2007) Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): an observational cohort study. Lancet 369, 578585.
12Helland, IB, Smith, L, Saarem, K, et al. (2003) Maternal supplementation with very-long-chain n-3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children's IQ at 4 years of age. Pediatrics 111, e39e44.
13Hoffman, DR, Theuer, RC, Castaneda, YS, et al. (2004) Maturation of visual acuity is accelerated in breast-fed term infants fed baby food containing DHA-enriched egg yolk. J Nutr 134, 23072313.
14Lauritzen, L, Jorgensen, MH, Olsen, SF, et al. (2005) Maternal fish oil supplementation in lactation: effect on developmental outcome in breast-fed infants. Reprod Nutr Dev 45, 535547.
15Jensen, CL, Voigt, RG, Prager, TC, et al. (2005) Effects of maternal docosahexaenoic acid intake on visual function and neurodevelopment in breastfed term infants. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 125132.
16Judge, MP, Harel, O & Lammi-Keefe, CJ (2007) Maternal consumption of a docosahexaenoic acid-containing functional food during pregnancy: benefit for infant performance on problem-solving but not on recognition memory tasks at age 9 mo. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 15721577.
17Judge, MP, Harel, O & Lammi-Keefe, CJ (2007) A docosahexaenoic acid-functional food during pregnancy benefits infant visual acuity at four but not six months of age. Lipids 42, 117122.
18Agostoni, C, Zuccotti, CV, Radaelli, G, et al. (2009) Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation and time at achievement of gross motor milestones in healthy infants: a randomized, prospective, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 6470.
19Eilander, A, Hundscheid, DC, Osendarp, SJ, et al. (2007) Effects of n-3 long chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on visual and cognitive development throughout childhood: a review of human studies. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 76, 189203.
20Grantham-McGregor, S & Ani, C (2001) A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr 131, 649S666S.
21Sachdev, HPS, Gera, T & Nestel, P (2005) Effect of iron supplementation on mental and motor development in children: systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr 8, 117132.
22Beard, JL & Connor, JR (2003) Iron status and neural functioning. Annu Rev Nutr 23, 4158.
23Delange, F (2000) The role of iodine in brain development. Proc Nutr Soc 59, 7579.
24Louwman, MWJ, van Dusseldorp, M, van de Vijver, FJR, et al. (2000) Signs of impaired cognitive function in adolescents with marginal cobalamin status. Am J Clin Nutr 72, 762769.
25Allen, LH, Penland, JG, Boy, E, et al. (1999) Cognitive and neuromotor performance of Guatemalan schoolers with deficient, marginal, and normal plasma vitamin B-12. FASEB J 13, A544.
26Butterworth, CE Jr & Bendich, A (1996) Folic acid and the prevention of birth defects. Annu Rev Nutr 16, 7397.
27Sugden, C (2006) One-carbon metabolism in psychiatric illness. Nutr Res Rev 19, 117136.
28Hutto, BR (1997) Folate and cobalamin in psychiatric illness. Compr Psychiatry 38, 305314.
29Dror, DK & Allen, LH (2008) Effect of vitamin B12 deficiency on neurodevelopment in infants: current knowledge and possible mechanisms. Nutr Rev 66, 250255.
30Muthayya, S, Eilander, A, Transler, C, et al. (2009) Effect of fortification with multiple micronutrients and n-3 fatty acids on growth and cognitive performance in Indian schoolchildren: the CHAMPION (Children's Health and Mental Performance Influenced by Optimal Nutrition) Study. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 17661775.
31World Health Organization Expert Committee on Physical Status (1995) Physical Status: The Use and Interpretation of Anthropometry. WHO Technical Report Series no. 854. Geneva: World Health Organization.
32Grantham-McGregor, S (2005) Can the provision of breakfast benefit school performance? Food Nutr Bull 26, S144S158.
33Kaufman, AS & Kaufman, LN (2004) Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children: Manual, 2nd ed.Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
34Hughes, D & Bryan, J (2003) The assessment of cognitive performance in children: considerations for detecting nutritional influences. Nutr Rev 61, 413422.
35Malda, M, van de Vijver, FJR, Srinivasan, K, et al. (2008) Adapting a cognitive test for a different culture: an illustration of qualitative procedures. Psychol Sci Q 50, 451468.
36Malda, M, van de Vijver, FJR, Srinivasan, K, et al. (2009) Traveling with cognitive tests: testing the validity of a KABC-II adaptation in India. Assessment (In the Press).
37Lohman, TG, Roche, AF & Martorell, R (1988) Anthropometric Standardization Reference Manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers.
38Burtis, CA & Ashwood, ER (1996) Fundamentals of Clinical Chemistry. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders Company.
39Miale, JB (1982) Laboratory Medicine: Hematology. St Louis, MO: CV Mosby.
40Chen, W, Sperling, MI & Heminger, LA (1987) Vitamin B12. In Methods in Clinical Chemistry, pp. 569573 [Pesce, AJ and Kaplan, LA, editors]. St Louis, MO: CV Mosby.
41Rose, HG & Oaklander, M (1965) Improved procedure for the extraction of lipids from human erythrocytes. J Lipids Res 6, 428431.
42Gentner, PR, Bauer, M & Dieterich, I (1981) Separation of major phospholipids classes of milk without previous isolation from total lipid extracts. J Chromatogr 13, 200204.
43Morrison, WR & Smith, LM (1964) Preparation of fatty acid methyl esters and dimethylacetals from lipids with boron fluoride–methanol. J Lipids Res 5, 600608.
44Pino, S, Fang, SL & Braverman, LE (1996) Ammonium persulfate: a safe alternative oxidizing reagent for measuring urinary iodine. Clin Chem 42, 239243.
45World Health Organization, United Nations Children's Fund & United Nations University (2001) Iron Deficiency Anemia. Assessment, Prevention and Control. A Guide for Programme Managers. Geneva: World Health Organization.
46Zimmermann, MB, Molinari, L & Staubli-Asobayire, F (2005) Serum transferrin receptor and zinc protoporphyrin as indicators of iron status in African children. Am J Clin Nutr 81, 615623.
47Institute of Medicine (IOM) (1998) Dietary Reference Intakes for Thiamin, Riboflavin, Niacin, Vitamin B6, Folate, Vitamin B12, Pantothenic Acid, Biotin, and Choline. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
48Jones, KM, Ramirez-Zea, M, Zuleta, C, et al. (2007) Prevalent vitamin B-12 deficiency in twelve-month-old Guatemalan infants is predicted by maternal B-12 deficiency and infant diet. J Nutr 137, 13071313.
49World Health Organization, United Nations Children's Fund & International Council for Control of Iodine Deficiency Disorders (2001) Assessment of Iodine Deficiency Disorders and Monitoring Their Elimination. A Guide for Programme Managers. Geneva: World Health Organization.
50Cook, JD, Flowers, CH & Skikne, BS (2003) The quantitative assessment of body iron. Blood 101, 33593364.
51Muthayya, S, Thankachan, P, Zimmermann, MB, et al. (2007) Low anemia prevalence in school-aged children in Bangalore, South India: possible effect of school health initiatives. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 865869.
52Isaacs, E & Oates, J (2008) Nutrition and cognition: assessing cognitive abilities in children and young people. Eur J Nutr 47, Suppl. 3, 424.
53Hughes, D & Bryan, J (2003) The assessment of cognitive performance in children: considerations for detecting nutritional influences. Nutr Rev 61, 413422.
54Sigman, M, Neumann, C, Baksh, M, et al. (1989) Relationship between nutrition and development in Kenyan toddlers. J Pediatr 115, 357364.
55Freeman, HE, Klein, RE, Kagan, J, et al. (1977) Relations between nutrition and cognition in rural Guatemala. Am J Public Health 67, 233239.
56Whaley, SE, Sigman, M, Espinosa, MP, et al. (1998) Infant predictors of cognitive development in an undernourished Kenyan population. J Dev Behav Pediatr 19, 169177.
57Powell, C & Grantham-McGregor, SM (1980) The associations between nutritional status, school achievement and school attendance in twelve-year-old children at a Jamaican school. West Indian Med J 29, 247253.
58Sigman, M, Neumann, C, Jansen, AA, et al. (1989) Cognitive abilities of Kenyan children in relation to nutrition, family characteristics, and education. Child Dev 60, 14631474.
59Johnston, FE, Low, SM, de Baessa, Y, et al. (1987) Interaction of nutritional and socioeconomic status as determinants of cognitive development in disadvantaged urban Guatemalan children. Am J Phys Anthropol 73, 501506.
60Grantham-McGregor, SM, Walker, SP, Chang, SM, et al. (1997) Effects of early childhood supplementation with and without stimulation on later development in stunted Jamaican children. Am J Clin Nutr 66, 247253.
61Pollitt, E, Watkins, WE & Husaini, MA (1997) Three-month nutritional supplementation in Indonesian infants and toddlers benefits memory function 8 y later. Am J Clin Nutr 66, 13571363.
62Bakker, EC, Ghys, AJA, Kester, ADM, et al. (2003) Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids at birth and cognitive function at 7y of age. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 8995.
63Rapoport, SI, Rao, JS & Igarashi, M (2007) Brain metabolism of nutritionally essential polyunsaturated fatty acids depends on both the diet and the liver. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 77, 251261.
64Mitchell, EA, Aman, MG, Turbott, SH, et al. (1987) Clinical characteristics and serum essential fatty acid levels in hyperactive children. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 26, 406411.
65Stevens, L, Zhang, W, Peck, L, et al. (2003) EFA supplementation in children with inattention, hyperactivity, and other disruptive behaviors. Lipids 38, 10071021.
66Chen, JR, Hsu, SF, Hsu, CD, et al. (2004) Dietary patterns and blood fatty acid composition in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in Taiwan. J Nutr Biochem 15, 467472.
67Colter, AL, Cutler, C & Meckling, KA (2008) Fatty acid status and behavioural symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adolescents: a case-control study. Nutr J 7, 8.
68Burgess, JR, Stevens, L, Zhang, W, et al. (2000) Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Clin Nutr 71, 327S330S.
69Nokes, C, van den Bosch, C & Bundy, DAP (1998) The Effects of Iron Deficiency and Anemia on Mental and Motor Performance, Educational Achievement, and Behavior in Children. An Annotated Bibliography. Washington, DC: International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group.
70Raman, G, Tatsioni, A, Chung, M, et al. (2007) Heterogeneity and lack of good quality studies limit association between folate, vitamins B-6 and B-12, and cognitive function. J Nutr 137, 17891794.
71Durga, J, van Boxtel, MP, Schouten, EG, et al. (2006) Folate and the methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase 677C → T mutation correlate with cognitive performance. Neurobiol Aging 27, 334343.
72Miller, JW (2006) Assessing the association between vitamin B-12 status and cognitive function in older adults. Am J Clin Nutr 84, 12591260.
73Bjorke Monsen, AL & Ueland, PM (2003) Homocysteine and methylmalonic acid in diagnosis and risk assessment from infancy to adolescence. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 721.
74Obeid, R & Herrmann, W (2006) Mechanisms of homocysteine neurotoxicity in neurodegenerative diseases with special reference to dementia. FEBS Lett 580, 29943005.
75Allen, LH (2008) Causes of vitamin B12 and folate deficiency. Food Nutr Bull 29, S20S34.
76Antony, AC (2003) Vegetarianism and vitamin B-12 (cobalamin) deficiency. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 36.
77Bleichrodt, N & Born, MP (1994) A meta-analysis of research on iodine and its relationship to cognitive development. In The Damaged Brain of Iodine Deficiency, pp. 195200 [Stanbury, JB, editor]. New York: Cognizant Communication Corporation.
78Zimmermann, MB, Connolly, K, Bozo, M, et al. (2006) Iodine supplementation improves cognition in iodine-deficient schoolchildren in Albania: a randomized, controlled, double-blind study. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 108114.