No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The technical reasoning hypothesis does not rule out the potential key roles of imitation and working memory for CTC
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 August 2020
Abstract
To support their claim for technical reasoning skills rather than imitation as the key for cumulative technological culture (CTC), Osiurak and Reynaud argue that chimpanzees can imitate mechanical actions, but do not have CTC. They also state that an increase in working memory in human evolution could not have been a key driver of CTC. We discuss why we disagree with these claims.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Clay, Z. & Tennie, C. (2017) Is over-imitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Development 89:1535–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N. (2017) The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24:1158–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64:135–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H. & Phillips, S. (1998) Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:803–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. & Tennie, C. (2017) Cultural evolution in chimpanzees and humans. In: Chimpanzees and human evolution, eds. Muller, M., Wrangham, R. & Pilbeam, D.. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J. & Whiten, A. (2008) Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 275(1636):835–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V. & Whiten, A. (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition 8(3):164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006) Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103:13878–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simms, N. K., Frausel, R. R. & Richland, L. E. (2018) Working memory predicts children's analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 166:160–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 364:2405–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2010) Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. PLoS One 5:e10544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2012) Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS One 7:e41548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Horner, V. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005) Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437:737–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, T. & Coolidge, F. L. (2007) Did a small but significant enhancement in working-memory capacity power the evolution of modern thinking? In: Rethinking the human revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans, eds. Mellars, P., Boyle, K., Bar-Yosef, O. & Stringer, C., pp. 79–90. McDonald Institute Monographs.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & Coolidge, F. L. (2014) Technical cognition, working memory and creativity. Pragmatics & Cognition 22:45–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture
Related commentaries (26)
A cognitive approach to cumulative technological culture is useful and necessary but only if it also applies to other species
A cognitive developmental approach is essential to understanding cumulative technological culture
A cognitive transition underlying both technological and social aspects of cumulative culture
A little too technical: The threat of intellectualising technical reasoning
A long view of cumulative technological culture
A theory limited in scope and evidence
Causal learning in CTC: Adaptive and collaborative
Chimpanzees' technical reasoning: Taking fieldwork and ontogeny seriously
How will we find the elephant in the room?
Human tool cognition relies on teleology
Implications for technological reserve development in advancing age, cognitive impairment, and dementia
Missing in action: Tool use is action based
New Caledonian crows afford invaluable comparative insights into human cumulative technological culture
Putting social cognitive mechanisms back into cumulative technological culture: Social interactions serve as a mechanism for children's early knowledge acquisition
Refining our understanding of the “elephant in the room”
Shared intentionality shapes humans' technical know-how
Supporting the weight of the elephant in the room: Technical intelligence propped up by social cognition and language
Taking into account the wider evolutionary context of cumulative cultural evolution
Technical reasoning alone does not take humans this far
The blind men and the elephant: What is missing cognitively in the study of cumulative technological evolution
The crow in the room: New Caledonian crows offer insight into the necessary and sufficient conditions for cumulative cultural evolution
The social side of innovation
The technical reasoning hypothesis does not rule out the potential key roles of imitation and working memory for CTC
Tools as “petrified memes”: A duality
What matters emotionally: The importance of pride for cumulative culture
Where does the elephant come from? The evolution of causal cognition is the key
Author response
The elephant in the China shop: When technical reasoning meets cumulative technological culture