Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

How does consciousness for action relate to attention for action?

  • Elizabeth A. Franz (a1)

Abstract

The interesting target article by Morsella et al. addresses critical issues that impact our understanding of consciousness. It is surprising, however, to see no treatment of the relationship between attention and consciousness, particularly given available models. Whether olfaction is most suitable as a model system to study consciousness for action also seems questionable. These issues are elaborated in the present commentary.

Copyright

References

Hide All
Bednark, J. G. & Franz, E. A. (2014) Agency attribution: Event-related potentials and outcome monitoring. Experimental Brain Research 232:1117–26.
Bednark, J. G., Reynolds, J. N. J, Stafford, T., Redgrave, P. & Franz, E. A. (2013) Creating a movement heuristic for voluntary action: Electrophysiological correlates of movement-outcome learning. Cortex 49(3):771–80.
Franz, E. A. (1997) Spatial coupling in the coordination of complex actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 50A:684704.
Franz, E. A. (2003) Bimanual action representation: A window to human evolution. In: Taking action: Cognitive neuroscience perspectives on intentional acts, ed. Johnston-Frey, S. H., pp. 259–88. MIT Press.
Franz, E. A. (2006) Converging evidence of the basal ganglia's role in focused action selection and inhibition of competing information. In: Recent breakthroughs in basal ganglia research, ed. Bezard, E., Ch. 18, pp. 227–39. Nova Science.
Franz, E. A. (2010) A framework for conceptual binding of bimanual actions: Possible applications to neurology and neuro-rehabilitative therapies. Current Trends in Neurology 4:122.
Franz, E. A. (2012) The allocation of attention to learning of goal-directed actions: A cognitive neuroscience framework focusing on the basal ganglia. Frontiers in Psychology 3, article 535:115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00535.
Franz, E. A., Eliassen, J., Ivry, R. B. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1996) Dissociation of spatial and temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of callosotomy patients. Psychological Science 7:306–10.
Franz, E. A. & Gillett, G. (2011) John Hughlings Jackson's evolutionary neurology: A unifying framework for cognitive neuroscience. Brain 134(10):3114–20.
Franz, E. A. & McCormick, R. (2010) Conceptual unifying constraints on anticipatory bimanual control. Experimental Brain Research 205(2):273–82.
Franz, E. A. & Ramachandran, V. S. (1998) Bimanual coupling in amputees with phantom limbs. Nature Neuroscience 1:443–44. doi:10.1038/2161.
Franz, E. A., Waldie, K. & Smith, M. (2000) The effect of callosotomy on novel versus familiar bimanual actions: A neural dissociation between controlled and automatic processes? Psychological Science 11(1):8285.
Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N. & McCabe, G. (1991) Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. Acta Psychologica 77(2):137–51.
Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., Swinnen, S. & Walter, C. (2001) Spatial conceptual influences on the coordination of bimanual actions: When a dual task becomes a single task. Journal of Motor Behavior 33(1):103–12.
Gillett, G. & Franz, E. A. (2013) John Hughlings Jackson: Bridging theory and clinical observation. The Lancet 381(9866):528–29.
Gottfried, J. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) The nose smells what the eye sees: Crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 39(2):375–86.
Greenwald, A. G. (1972) On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology 94(1):5257.
Grossberg, S. (1999) The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8(1):144.
Hommel, B. (2010) Grounding attention in action control: The intentional control of selection. In: Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action, ed. Bruya, B. J., pp. 121–40. MIT Press.
Hughes, C. M. L. & Franz, E. A. (2007) Experience-dependent effects in unimanual and bimanual reaction time tasks in musicians. Journal of Motor Behavior 39(1):38.
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1884) On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (Part 2). British Medical Journal 12:703707.
James, W. (1890/1918) The principles of psychology, vols. 1 and 11. Dover. (Original work published in 1890).
Keller, A. (2011) Attention and olfactory consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology 2, article 380. (Online journal). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00380.
Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. L. & Goodman, D. (1979) On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science 203(4384):1029–31.
Laing, D. G. (1983) Natural sniffing gives optimum odour perception for humans. Perception 12:99117.
Mainland, J. D. & Sobel, N. (2006) The sniff is part of the olfactory percept. Chemical Senses 31:181–96.
Mink, J. W. (1996) The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Progress in Neurobiology 50:381425.
Shen, Y. C. & Franz, E. A. (2005) Hemispheric competition in left-handers on bimanual reaction time tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior 37:39.
Sperry, R. W. (1952) Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40:291312.
Tassinari, G. & Berlucchi, G. (1993) Sensory and attentional components of slowing of manual reaction time to non-fixated visual targets by ipsilateral primes. Vision Research 33(11):1525–34.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed