Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2012
  • Online publication date: September 2012

Section 1 - Basic Mechanisms: Cognitive Performance and Sleep

Related content

Powered by UNSILO

References

[1] DurmerJS, DingesDFNeurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol 2005; 25(1): 117–29.
[2] BalkinTJ, ThorneD, SingH, et al. Effects of Sleep Schedules on Commercial Motor Vehicle Driver Performance. FMCSA Technical Report No. DOT-MC-00–133. Washington, D.C: U.S. Department of Transportation; 2000.
[3] LandriganCP, RothschildJM, CroninJW, et al. Effect of reducing interns’ work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. N Engl J Med 2004; 351(18): 1838–48.
[4] Citizen.org (Internet). Washington, DC: Public Citizen. c2010 (updated 2010 Sep 2). Available from: http://www.citizen.org/Page.aspx?pid=4294
[5] NaitohP.Sleep deprivation in human subjects: A reappraisal. Waking and Sleeping 1976; 1: 53–60.
[6] AchesonA, RichardsJB, de WitH. Effects of sleep deprivation on impulsive behaviors in men and women. Physiol Behav 2007; 91(5): 579–87. Epub 2007 Mar 31.
[7] AndersonC, PlattenCR. Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behav Brain Res 2010; Oct 1 [Epub ahead of print].
[8] KillgoreWD. Effects of sleep deprivation and morningness-eveningness traits on risk-taking. Psychol Rep 2007; 100(2): 613–26.
[9] OlsenOK, PallesenS, EidJ. The impact of partial sleep deprivation on moral reasoning in military officers. Sleep 2010; 33(8): 1086–90.
[10] KillgoreWD, McBrideSA, KillgoreDB, BalkinTJ. The effects of caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil on humor appreciation during sleep deprivation. Sleep 2006; 29(6): 841–7.
[11] GoharA, AdamsA, GertnerE, et al. Working memory capacity is decreased in sleep-deprived internal medicine residents. J Clin Sleep Med 2009; 5(3): 191–7.
[12] KillgoreWD, Kahn-GreeneET, LipizziEL, et al. Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med 2008; 9(5): 517–26.
[13] van der HelmE, GujarN, WalkerMP. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep 2010; 33(3): 335–42.
[14] Benjamin, AC. Operationalism. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas; 1955.
[15] MarxMH, HillixWA. Systems and Theories in Psychology, 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company; 1973.
[16] WilkinsonRT. Sleep deprivation. In: EdholmOG, BacaharachAL, editors. The Physiology of Human Survival. London: Academic Press; 1965: 399–430.
[17] BalkinTJ, BliesePD, BelenkyG, et al. Comparative utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness-related performance decrements in the operational environment. J Sleep Res 2004; 13: 219–27.
[18] WarmJS, Vigilance and target detection. In: Wickens CD, Huey BM, editors. Workload Transition: Implications for Individual and Team Performance.Washingdon, DC: National Research Council; 1993: 139–170.

References

[1] OgawaS, MenonRS, KimSG, UgurbilK. On the characteristics of functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain. Annu Rev Biophys Biomol Struct 1998; 27: 447–74.
[2] McKiernanKA, KaufmanJN, Kucera-ThompsonJ, BinderJR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci 2003; 15(3): 394–408.
[3] RaichleME, MacLeodAM, SnyderAZ, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001; 98(2): 676–82.
[4] MaquetP. Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission tomography. J Sleep Res 2000; 9(3): 207–31.
[5] LimJ, WuWC, WangJ, et al. Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: an ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. Neuroimage. 2010; 49(4): 3426–35.
[6] BuxtonRB. Quantifying CBF with arterial spin labeling. J Magn Reson Imaging 2005; 22(6): 723–6.
[7] AguirreGK, DetreJA, ZarahnE, AlsopDC. Experimental design and the relative sensitivity of BOLD and perfusion fMRI. Neuroimage 2002; 15(3): 488–500.
[8] FristonKJ, BuechelC, FinkGR, et al. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 1997; 6(3): 218–29.
[9] BiswalB, YetkinFZ, HaughtonVM, HydeJS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 1995; 34(4): 537–41.
[10] BeckmannCF, DeLucaM, DevlinJT, SmithSM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2005; 360(1457): 1001–13.
[11] HorovitzSG, BraunAR, CarrWS, et al. Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106(27): 11 376–81.
[12] SamannPG, TullyC, SpoormakerVI, et al. Increased sleep pressure reduces resting state functional connectivity. MAGMA 2010; May 15.
[13] ColeDM, SmithSM, BeckmannCF. Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front Syst Neurosci 2010; 4: 8.
[14] MatthewsPM, HoneyGD, BullmoreET. Applications of fMRI in translational medicine and clinical practice. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006; 7(9): 732–44.
[15] CheeMW, ChooWC. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci 2004; 24(19): 4560–7.
[16] CheeMW, ChuahYM. Functional neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of capacity decline in visual short-term memory after sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104(22): 9487–92.
[17] ChuahLY, CheeMW. Cholinergic augmentation modulates visual task performance in sleep-deprived young adults. J Neurosci 2008; 28(44): 11 369–77.
[18] DrummondSP, BrownGG, SalamatJS, GillinJC. Increasing task difficulty facilitates the cerebral compensatory response to total sleep deprivation. Sleep 2004; 27(3): 445–51.
[19] ChooWC, LeeWW, VenkatramanV, SheuFS, CheeMW. Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. Neuroimage 2005; 25(2): 579–87.
[20] LeproultR, ColecchiaEF, BerardiAM, et al. Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. Am J Physiol 2003; 284(2): R280–90.
[21] Van DongenHP, BaynardMD, MaislinG, DingesDF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 2004; 27(3): 423–33.
[22] KillgoreWD, LipizziEL, KamimoriGH, BalkinTJ. Caffeine effects on risky decision making after 75 hours of sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med 2007; 78(10): 957–62.
[23] WesenstenNJ, KillgoreWD, BalkinTJ. Performance and alertness effects of caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res 2005; 14(3): 255–66.
[24] YooSS, GujarN, HuP, JoleszFA, WalkerMP. The human emotional brain without sleep – a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biol. 2007; 17(20): R877–8.
[25] CheeMW, TanJC, ParimalS, ZagorodnovV. Sleep deprivation and its effects on object-selective attention. Neuroimage 2010; 49(2): 1903–10.
[26] RamseyJD, HansonSJ, HansonC, et al. Six problems for causal inference from fMRI. Neuroimage 2010; 49(2): 1545–58.
[27] GoelN, RaoH, DurmerJS, DingesDF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol 2009; 29(4): 320–39.
[28] LimJ, DingesDF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull 2010; 136(3): 375–89.
[29] CheeMW, TanJC. Lapsing when sleep deprived: neural activation characteristics of resistant and vulnerable individuals. Neuroimage 2010; In Press.
[30] CabezaR, AndersonND, LocantoreJK, McIntoshAR. Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 2002; 17(3): 1394–402.
[31] LoganJM, SandersAL, SnyderAZ, MorrisJC, BucknerRL. Under-recruitment and non-selective recruitment: dissociable neural mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron 2002; 33(5): 827–40.
[32] NagelIE, PreuschhofC, LiSC, et al. Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009; 106(52): 22 552–7.
[33] CheeMW, ChuahLY. Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol 2008; 21(4): 417–23.
[34] CorbettaM, ShulmanGL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3(3): 201–15.
[35] KastnerS, UngerleiderLG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 2000; 23: 315–41.
[36] SturmW, WillmesK. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 2001; 14(1 Pt 2): S76–84.
[37] PardoJV, FoxPT, RaichleME. Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 1991; 349(6304): 61–4.
[38] SturmW, de SimoneA, KrauseBJ, et al. Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 1999;37(7): 797–805.
[39] HarrisonY, HorneJA, RothwellA. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults: a model for healthy aging? Sleep 2000; 23(8): 1067–73.
[40] MuzurA, Pace-SchottEF, HobsonJA. The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends Cogn Sci 2002; 6(11): 475–81.
[41] ThomasM, SingH, BelenkyG, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J Sleep Res 2000; 9(4): 335–52.
[42] DrummondSP, BrownGG, GillinJC, et al. Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature 2000; 403(6770): 655–7.
[43] PortasCM, ReesG, HowsemanAM, et al. A specific role for the thalamus in mediating the interaction of attention and arousal in humans. J Neurosci 1998; 18(21): 8979–89.
[44] Bell-McGintyS, HabeckC, HiltonHJ, et al. Identification and differential vulnerability of a neural network in sleep deprivation. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14(5): 496–502.
[45] DrummondSP, BrownGG, StrickerJL, et al. Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport 1999; 10(18): 3745–8.
[46] MuQ, NahasZ, JohnsonKA, et al. Decreased cortical response to verbal working memory following sleep deprivation. Sleep 2005; 28(1): 55–67.
[47] CajochenC, BrunnerDP, KrauchiK, GrawP, Wirz-JusticeA. Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep 1995; 18(10): 890–4.
[48] FinelliLA, BaumannH, BorbelyAA, AchermannP. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience 2000; 101(3): 523–9.
[49] CheeMW, ChuahYML, VenkatramanV, et al. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 hours of sleep deprivation: correlations of frontoparietal activation with performance. Neuroimage 2006; 31(1): 419–28.
[50] HabeckC, RakitinBC, MoellerJ, et al. An event-related fMRI study of the neurobehavioral impact of sleep deprivation on performance of a delayed-match-to-sample task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004; 18(3): 306–21.
[51] LimJ, ChooWC, CheeMW. Reproducibility of changes in behavior and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep 2007; 30(1): 61–70.
[52] CheeMWL, TanJC, ZhengH, et al. Lapsing during sleep deprivation is associated with distributed changes in brain activation. J Neurosci 2008; 28(21): 5519–28.
[53] LimJ, TanJC, ParimalS, DingesDF, CheeMW. Sleep deprivation impairs object-selective attention: a view from the ventral visual cortex. PLoS One 2010; 5(2): e9087.
[54] TomasiD, WangRL, TelangF, et al. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cereb Cortex 2009; 19(1): 233–40.
[55] AlvarezGA, CavanaghP. The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. Psychol Sci 2004; 15(2): 106–11.
[56] CowanN. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci 2000; 24(1): 87–114; discussion 114–185.
[57] EverittBJ, RobbinsTW. Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Ann Rev Psychol 1997; 48: 649–84.
[58] DoranSM, Van DongenHP, DingesDF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol 2001; 139(3): 253–67.
[59] WeissmanDH, RobertsKC, VisscherKM, WoldorffMG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neurosci 2006; 9(7): 971–8.
[60] KruegerJM, RectorDM, RoyS, et al. Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9(12): 910–19.
[61] PigarevIN, NothdurftHC, KastnerS. Evidence for asynchronous development of sleep in cortical areas. Neuroreport 1997; 8(11): 2557–60.
[62] MaroisR, ChunMM, GoreJC. A common parieto-frontal network is recruited under both low visibility and high perceptual interference conditions. J Neurophysiol 2004; 92(5): 2985–92.
[63] SerencesJT, SchwarzbachJ, CourtneySM, GolayX, YantisS. Control of object-based attention in human cortex. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14(12): 1346–57.
[64] WojciulikE, KanwisherN. The generality of parietal involvement in visual attentionNeuron 1999; 23(4): 747–64.
[65] GazzaleyA, CooneyJW, McEvoyK, KnightRT, D’EspositoM. Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J Cogn Neurosci 2005; 17(3): 507–17.
[66] YiD-J, ChunMM. Attentional modulation of learning-related repetition attenuation effects in human parahippocampal cortex. J Neurosci 2005; 25(14): 3593–600.
[67] EicheleT, DebenerS, CalhounVD, et al. Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105(16): 6173–8.
[68] HarrisonY, HorneJA. One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 1999; 78(2): 128–45.
[69] LindeL, EdlandA, BergstromM. Auditory attention and multiattribute decision-making during a 33 h sleep-deprivation period: mean performance and between-subject dispersions. Ergonomics 1999; 33(5): 696–713.
[70] KillgoreWD, BalkinTJ, WesenstenNJ. Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res 2006; 15(1): 7–13.
[71] McKennaBS, DicjinsonDL, OrffHJ, DrummondSP. The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. J Sleep Res 2007; 16(3): 245–52.
[72] HarrisonY, HorneJA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl 2000; 6(3): 236–49.
[73] BecharaA, DamasioH, DamasioAR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10(3): 295–307.
[74] VenkatramanV, ChuahYM, HuettelSA, CheeMW. Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep 2007; 30(5): 603–9.
[75] DickinsonDL, DrummondSP. The effects of total sleep deprivation on bayesian updating. Judgement and Decision Making 2008; 3(2): 181–90.
[76] KillgoreWD, GrugleNL, KillgoreDB, et al. Restoration of risk-propensity during sleep deprivation: caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil. Aviat Space Environ Med 2008; 79(9): 867–74.
[77] KillgoreWD, KillgoreDB, DayLM, et al. The effects of 53 hours of sleep deprivation on moral judgment. Sleep 2007; 30(3): 345–52.
[78] VenkatramanV, PayneJW, BettmanJR, LuceMF, HuettelSA. Separate neural mechanisms underlie choices and strategic preferences in risky decision making. Neuron 2009; 62(4): 593–602.
[79] MuQ, MishoryA, JohnsonKA, et al. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep 2005; 28(4): 433–46.
[80] SternY, MoellerJR, AndersonKE, et al. Different brain networks mediate task performance in normal aging and AD: defining compensation. Neurology 2000; 55(9): 1291–7.
[81] ChuahYM, VenkatramanV, DingesDF, CheeMW. The neural basis of interindividual variability in inhibitory efficiency after sleep deprivation. J Neurosci 2006; 26(27): 7156–62.
[82] ChuahLY, ChongDL, ChenAK, et al. Donepezil improves episodic memory in young individuals vulnerable to the effects of sleep deprivation. Sleep 2009; 32(8): 999–1010.
[83] VandewalleG, ArcherSN, WuillaumeC, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J Neurosci 2009; 29(25): 7948–56.
[84] SchmidtC, ColletteF, LeclercqY, et al. Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the suprachiasmatic area. Science 2009; 324(5926): 516–19.
[85] GrawP, KrauchiK, KnoblauchV, Wirz-JusticeA, CajochenC. Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiol Behav 2004; 80(5): 695–701.
[86] RéteyJV, AdamM, KhatamiR, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2007; 81(5): 692–8.
[87] RéteyJV, AdamM, GottseligJM, et al. Adenosinergic mechanisms contribute to individual differences in sleep deprivation-induced changes in neurobehavioral function and brain rhythmic activity. J Neurosci 2006; 26(41): 10 472–9.
[88] ChenY, ParrishTB. Caffeine dose effect on activation-induced BOLD and CBF responses. Neuroimage 2009; 46(3): 577–83.
[89] VandewalleG, MaquetP, DijkDJ. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13(10): 429–38.
[90] CaldwellJA, PrazinkoB, CaldwellJL. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2003; 114(1): 23–31.
[91] DickersonBC, SperlingRA. Neuroimaging biomarkers for clinical trials of disease-modifying therapies in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroRx 2005; 2(2): 348–60.
[92] JackCR Jr, ShiungMM, GunterJL, et al. Comparison of different MRI brain atrophy rate measures with clinical disease progression in AD. Neurology 2004;62(4): 591–600.
[93] FilippiniN, MacIntoshBJ, HoughMG, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106(17): 7209–14.
[94] GroegerJA, ViolaAU, LoJC, et al. Early morning executive functioning during sleep deprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism. Sleep 2008; 31(8): 1159–67.
[95] GiacobiniE. Cholinesterase inhibitors do more than inhibit cholinesterase. In: BeckerR, GiacobiniE, editors. Alzheimer Disease: From Molecular Biology to Therapy. Boston: Birkhauser; 1996: 187–204.
[96] FureyML, PietriniP, HaxbyJV. Cholinergic enhancement and increased selectivity of perceptual processing during working memory. Science 2000; 290(5500): 2315–19.
[97] LawrenceNS, RossTJ, SteinEA. Cognitive mechanisms of nicotine on visual attention. Neuron 2002; 36(3): 539–48.
[98] BentleyP, DriverJ, DolanRJ. Cholinesterase inhibition modulates visual and attentional brain responses in Alzheimer’s disease and health. Brain 2008; 131(Pt 2): 409–24.
[99] SperlingR, GreveD, DaleA, et al. Functional MRI detection of pharmacologically induced memory impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002; 99(1): 455–60.
[100] SchonK, AtriA, HasselmoME, et al. Scopolamine reduces persistent activity related to long-term encoding in the parahippocampal gyrus during delayed matching in humans. J Neurosci 2005; 25(40): 9112–23.
[101] FanJ, McCandlissBD, FossellaJ, FlombaumJI, PosnerMI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 2005; 26(2): 471–9.
[102] JackCR Jr, LoweVJ, WeigandSD, et al. Serial PIB and MRI in normal, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: implications for sequence of pathological events in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2009; 132(Pt 5): 1355–65.

References

[1] KillgoreWD. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog Brain Res 2010; 185: 105–29.
[2] CzeislerCA. Medical and genetic differences in the adverse impact of sleep loss on performance: ethical considerations for the medical profession. Trans Am Clin Climatol Assoc 2009; 120: 249–85.
[3] PhilipP, AkerstedtT. Transport and industrial safety, how are they affected by sleepiness and sleep restriction? Sleep Med Rev 2006; 10:347–56.
[4] CappuccioFP, D’EliaL, StrazzulloP, MillerMA. Sleep duration and all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep 2010; 33: 585–92.
[5] MontiJM, Pandi-PerumalSR, SintonCM. Neurochemistry of Sleep and Wakefulness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008.
[6] SteriadeM, McCarleyRW. Brain Control of Wakefulness and Sleep. New York: Kluwer Academic; 2005.
[7] BrownRE, BasheerR, McKennaJT, StreckerRE, McCarleyRW. Control of sleep and wakefulness. Physiol Rev 2012; In press.
[8] McCoyJG, StreckerRE. The cognitive cost of sleep lost. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2011; In press.
[9] KesslerRC, BerglundPA, CoulouvratC, et al. Insomnia and the performance of US workers: Results from the America Insomnia Survey. Sleep 2011; 34: 1161–71.
[10] AserinskyE, KleitmanN. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science 1953; 118: 273–4.
[11] BorbélyAA, AchermannP. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J Biol Rhythms 1999; 14: 557–68.
[12] McKennaJT, BrownRE, McCarleyRW. Neuroanatomy and neurobiology of sleep and wakefulness. In: WinkelmanJW, PlanteDT, editors. Foundations of Psychiatric Sleep Medicine. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2010: 13–35.
[13] BalkinTJ, BliesePD, BelenkyG, et al. Comparative utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness-related performance decrements in the operational environment. J Sleep Res 2004; 13: 219–27.
[14] BalkinTJ, RuppT, PicchioniD, WesenstenNJ. Sleep loss and sleepiness: current issues. Chest 2008; 134: 653–60.
[15] McKennaJT, CordeiraJW, ChristieMA, et al. Assessing sleepiness in the rat: a multiple sleep latencies test compared to polysomnographic measures of sleepiness. J Sleep Res 2008; 17: 365–75.
[16] RechtschaffenA, BergmannBM. Sleep deprivation in the rat: an update of the 1989 paper. Sleep 2002; 25: 18–24.
[17] KimY, LaposkyAD, BergmannBM, TurekFW. Repeated sleep restriction in rats leads to homeostatic and allostatic responses during recovery sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104: 10 697–702.
[18] LeemburgS, VyazovskiyVV, OlceseU, et al. Sleep homeostasis in the rat is preserved during chronic sleep restriction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107: 15 939–44.
[19] AkerstedtT, KecklundG, IngreM, LekanderM, AxelssonJ. Sleep homeostasis during repeated sleep restriction and recovery: support from EEG dynamics. Sleep 2009; 32: 217–22.
[20] ChristieMA, McKennaJT, ConnollyNP, McCarleyRW, StreckerRE. 24 hours of sleep deprivation in the rat increases sleepiness and decreases vigilance: introduction of the rat-psychomotor vigilance task. J Sleep Res 2008; 17: 376–84.
[21] ChristieMA, BolortuyaY, ChenLC, et al. Microdialysis elevation of adenosine in the basal forebrain produces vigilance impairments in the rat psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep 2008; 31: 1393–8.
[22] Blanco-CenturionC, GerashchenkoD, ShiromaniPJ. Effects of saporin-induced lesions of three arousal populations on daily levels of sleep and wake. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 14 041–8.
[23] ThakkarMM, StreckerRE, McCarleyRW. Behavioral state control through differential serotonergic inhibition in the mesopontine cholinergic nuclei: a simultaneous unit recording and microdialysis study. J Neurosci 1998; 18: 5490–7.
[24] BremerF. Cerveau “isole” et physiologie du sommeil. C R Soc Biol Paris 1935; 118: 1235–42.
[25] MoruzziG, MagounHW. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1949; 1: 455–73.
[26] LindsleyDB, BowdenJW, MagounHW. Effect upon the EEG of acute injury to the brain stem activating system. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1949; 1: 475–86.
[27] JonesBE. From waking to sleeping: neuronal and chemical substrates. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2005; 26: 578–86.
[28] RustedJM, CaulfieldD, KingL, GoodeA. Moving out of the laboratory: does nicotine improve everyday attention? Behav Pharmacol 2000; 11: 621–9.
[29] PhillipsBA, DannerFJ. Cigarette smoking and sleep disturbance. Arch Intern Med 1995; 155: 734–7.
[30] UchidaS, HottaH, MisawaH, KawashimaK. Sustained subcutaneous infusion of nicotine enhances cholinergic vasodilation in the cerebral cortex induced by stimulation of the nucleus basalis of Meynert in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 2011; 654: 235–40.
[31] BoutrelB, KoobGF. What keeps us awake: the neuropharmacology of stimulants and wakefulness-promoting medications. Sleep 2004; 27: 1181–94.
[32] LeeMG, HassaniOK, AlonsoA, JonesBE. Cholinergic basal forebrain neurons burst with theta during waking and paradoxical sleep. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 4365–9.
[33] WilliamsJA, ComisarowJ, DayJ, FibigerHC, ReinerPB. State-dependent release of acetylcholine in rat thalamus measured by in vivo microdialysis. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 5236–42.
[34] StreckerRE, ThakkarMM, Porkka-HeiskanenT, et al. Behavioral state-related changes of extracellular serotonin concentration in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus: a microdialysis study in freely moving animals. Sleep Res Online 1999; 2: 21–7.
[35] GrayR, RajanAS, RadcliffeKA, YakehiroM, DaniJA. Hippocampal synaptic transmission enhanced by low concentrations of nicotine. Nature 1996; 383: 713–16.
[36] AlkondonM, PereiraEF, EisenbergHM, AlbuquerqueEX. Nicotinic receptor activation in human cerebral cortical interneurons: a mechanism for inhibition and disinhibition of neuronal networks. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 66–75.
[37] AlonsoA, FaureMP, BeaudetA. Neurotensin promotes oscillatory bursting behavior and is internalized in basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 5778–92.
[38] LeeMG, ChrobakJJ, SikA, WileyRG, BuzsákiG. Hippocampal theta activity following selective lesion of the septal cholinergic system. Neuroscience 1994; 62: 1033–47.
[39] HentschkeH, PerkinsMG, PearceRA, BanksMI. Muscarinic blockade weakens interaction of gamma with theta rhythms in mouse hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 26: 1642–56.
[40] KaurS, JunekA, BlackMA, SembaK. Effects of ibotenate and 192IgG-saporin lesions of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis/substantia innominata on spontaneous sleep and wake states and on recovery sleep after sleep deprivation in rats. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 491–504.
[41] KalinchukAV, McCarleyRW, StenbergD, Porkka-HeiskanenT, BasheerR. The role of cholinergic basal forebrain neurons in adenosine-mediated homeostatic control of sleep: lessons from 192 IgG-saporin lesions. Neuroscience 2008; 157: 238–53.
[42] WebsterHH, JonesBE. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsolateral pontomesencephalic tegmentum-cholinergic cell area in the cat. II. Effects upon sleep-waking states. Brain Res 1988; 458: 285–302.
[43] CarterME, YizharO, ChikahisaS, et al. Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat Neurosci 2010; 13: 1526–33.
[44] TullyK, BolshakovVY. Emotional enhancement of memory: how norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Mol Brain 2010; 3: 15.
[45] RasmussenK, MorilakDA, JacobsBL. Single unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons in the freely moving cat. I. During naturalistic behaviors and in response to simple and complex stimuli. Brain Res 1986; 371: 324–34.
[46] BerridgeCW, AbercrombieED. Relationship between locus coeruleus discharge rates and rates of norepinephrine release within neocortex as assessed by in vivo microdialysis. Neuroscience 1999; 93: 1263–70.
[47] GallopinT, FortP, EggermannE, et al. Identification of sleep-promoting neurons in vitro. Nature 2000; 404: 992–5.
[48] SteriadeM, AmzicaF, NuñezA. Cholinergic and noradrenergic modulation of the slow (approximately 0.3 Hz) oscillation in neocortical cells. J Neurophysiol 1993; 70: 1385–400.
[49] Blanco-CenturionC, GerashchenkoD, Salin-PascualRJ, ShiromaniPJ. Effects of hypocretin2-saporin and antidopamine-beta-hydroxylase-saporin neurotoxic lesions of the dorsolateral pons on sleep and muscle tone. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 19: 2741–52.
[50] StantonPK, SarveyJM. Depletion of norepinephrine, but not serotonin, reduces long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus of rat hippocampal slices. J Neurosci 1985; 5: 2169–76.
[51] JacobsBL, FornalCA. Activity of brain serotonergic neurons in the behaving animal. Pharmacol Rev 1991; 43: 563–78.
[52] MouretJ, BobillierP, JouvetM. Insomnia following parachlorophenylalanine in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1968; 5: 17–22.
[53] BuchananGF, RichersonGB. Central serotonin neurons are required for arousal to CO[2]. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107: 16 354–9.
[54] Vázquez-PalaciosG, Hernández-GonzálezM, Guevara PérezMA, Bonilla-JaimeH. Nicotine and fluoxetine induce arousing effects on sleep-wake cycle in antidepressive doses: a possible mechanism of antidepressant-like effects of nicotine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2010; 94: 503–9.
[55] SchultzW. Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007; 30: 259–88.
[56] DahanL, AstierB, VautrelleN, et al. Prominent burst firing of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area during paradoxical sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007; 32: 1232–41.
[57] ZantJC, LeenaarsCH, KostinA, Van SomerenEJ, Porkka-HeiskanenT. Increases in extracellular serotonin and dopamine metabolite levels in the basal forebrain during sleep deprivation. Brain Res 2011; 1399: 40–8.
[58] EspañaRA, ScammellTE. Sleep neurobiology from a clinical perspective. Sleep 2011; 34: 845–58.
[59] StreckerRE, NalwalkJ, DauphinLJ, et al. Extracellular histamine levels in the feline preoptic/anterior hypothalamic area during natural sleep-wakefulness and prolonged wakefulness: an in vivo microdialysis study. Neuroscience 2002; 113: 663–70.
[60] ThakkarMM. Histamine in the regulation of wakefulness. Sleep Med Rev 2011; 15: 65–74.
[61] Van RuitenbeekP, VermeerenA, RiedelWJ. Cognitive domains affected by histamine H(1)-antagonism in humans: a literature review. Brain Res Rev 2010; 64: 263–82.
[62] SakaiK, el MansariM, LinJS, ZhangZG, Vanni-MercierG. The posterior hypothalamus in the regulation of wakefulness and paradoxical sleep. In: ManciaM, MariniM, editors. The Diencephalon and Sleep. New York: Raven Press; 1990: 171–98.
[63] AnacletC, ParmentierR, OukK, et al. Orexin/hypocretin and histamine: distinct roles in the control of wakefulness demonstrated using knock-out mouse models. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 14 423–38.
[64] ValdésJL, FaríasP, Ocampo-GarcésA, et al. Arousal and differential Fos expression in histaminergic neurons of the ascending arousal system during a feeding-related motivated behaviour. Eur J Neurosci 2005; 21: 1931–42.
[65] CaoM, GuilleminaultC. Hypocretin and its emerging role as a target for treatment of sleep disorders. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2011; 11: 227–34.
[66] LeeMG, HassaniOK, JonesBE. Discharge of identified orexin/hypocretin neurons across the sleep-waking cycle. J Neurosci 2005; 25: 6716–20.
[67] AdamantidisAR, ZhangF, AravanisAM, DeisserothK, de LeceaL. Neural substrates of awakening probed with optogenetic control of hypocretin neurons. Nature 2007; 450: 420–4.
[68] MochizukiT, CrockerA, McCormackS, et al. Behavioral state instability in orexin knock-out mice. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 6291–300.
[69] YamanakaA, BeuckmannCT, WillieJT, et al. Hypothalamic orexin neurons regulate arousal according to energy balance in mice. Neuron 2003; 38: 701–13.
[70] HassaniOK, LeeMG, HennyP, JonesBE. Discharge profiles of identified GABAergic in comparison to cholinergic and putative glutamatergic basal forebrain neurons across the sleep-wake cycle. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 11 828–40.
[71] HurEE, ZaborszkyL. Vglut2 afferents to the medial prefrontal and primary somatosensory cortices: a combined retrograde tracing in situ hybridization study. J Comp Neurol 2005; 483: 351–73.
[72] HennyP, JonesBE. Projections from basal forebrain to prefrontal cortex comprise cholinergic, GABAergic and glutamatergic inputs to pyramidal cells or interneurons. Eur J Neurosci 2008; 27: 654–70.
[73] LynchG, PalmerLC, GallCM. The likelihood of cognitive enhancement. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2011; 99: 116–29.
[74] PorrinoLJ, DaunaisJB, RogersGA, HampsonRE, DeadwylerSA. Facilitation of task performance and removal of the effects of sleep deprivation by an ampakine (CX717) in nonhuman primates. PLoS Biol 2005; 3(9): e299.
[75] WesenstenNJ, ReichardtRM, BalkinTJ. Ampakine (CX717) effects on performance and alertness during simulated night shift work. Aviat Space Environ Med 2007; 78(10): 937–43
[76] BoyleJ, StanleyN, JamesLM, et al. Acute sleep deprivation: the effects of the AMPAKINE compound CX717 on human cognitive performance, alertness and recovery sleep. Psychopharmacology 2011; Sept 22 [Epub ahead of print].
[77] PoeGR, WalshCM, BjornessTE. Cognitive neuroscience of sleep. Prog Brain Res 2010; 185: 1–19.
[78] RauchsG, DesgrangesB, ForetJ, EustacheF. The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. J Sleep Res 2005; 14: 123–40.
[79] StickgoldR, WalkerMP. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Med 2007; 8: 331–43.
[80] Von EconomoC. Sleep as a problem of localization. J Nervous Mental Disorders 1930; 71: 249–259.
[81] SzymusiakR, McGintyD. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and arousal. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1129: 275–86.
[82] SherinJE, ShiromaniPJ, McCarleyRW, SaperCB. Activation of ventrolateral preoptic neurons during sleep. Science 1996; 271: 216–19.
[83] SaperCB, ChouTC, ScammellTE. The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci 2001; 24: 726–31.
[84] LuJ, BjorkumAA, XuM, et al. Selective activation of the extended ventrolateral preoptic nucleus during rapid eye movement sleep. J Neurosci 2002; 22: 4568–76.
[85] LuJ, GrecoMA, ShiromaniP, SaperCB. Effect of lesions of the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus on NREM and REM sleep. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 3830–42.
[86] FeldbergW, SherwoodSL. Injections of drugs into the lateral ventricle of the cat. J Physiol 1954; 123: 148–67.
[87] SnyderSH, KatimsJJ, AnnauZ, BrunsRF, DalyJW. Adenosine receptors and behavioral actions of methylxanthines. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1981; 78: 3260–4.
[88] RadulovackiM, VirusRM, RapozaD, CraneRA. A comparison of the dose response effects of pyrimidine ribonucleosides and adenosine on sleep in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985; 87: 136–40.
[89] Porkka-HeiskanenT, StreckerRE, ThakkarM, et al. Adenosine: a mediator of the sleep-inducing effects of prolonged wakefulness. Science 1997; 276: 1265–8.
[90] Porkka-HeiskanenT, StreckerRE, McCarleyRW. Brain site-specificity of extracellular adenosine concentration changes during sleep deprivation and spontaneous sleep: an in vivo microdialysis study. Neuroscience 2000; 99: 507–17.
[91] McKennaJT, DauphinLJ, MulkernKJ, et al. Nocturnal elevation of extracellular adenosine in the rat basal forebrain. Sleep Res Online 2003; 5: 155–60.
[92] Murillo-RodriguezE, Blanco-CenturionC, GerashchenkoD, Salin-PascualRJ, ShiromaniPJ. The diurnal rhythm of adenosine levels in the basal forebrain of young and old rats. Neuroscience 2004; 123: 361–70.
[93] ThakkarMM, WinstonS, McCarleyRW. A1 receptor and adenosinergic homeostatic regulation of sleep-wakefulness: effects of antisense to the A1 receptor in the cholinergic basal forebrain. J Neurosci 2003; 23: 4278–87.
[94] LazarusM, ShenHY, CherasseY, et al. Arousal effect of caffeine depends on adenosine A2A receptors in the shell of the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 2011; 31(27): 10 067–75.
[95] StreckerRE, MorairtyS, ThakkarMM, et al. Adenosinergic modulation of basal forebrain and preoptic/anterior hypothalamic neuronal activity in the control of behavioral state. Behav Brain Res 2000; 115: 183–204.
[96] AlamMN, SzymusiakR, GongH, KingJ, McGintyD. Adenosinergic modulation of rat basal forebrain neurons during sleep and waking: neuronal recording with microdialysis. J Physiol 1999; 521: 679–90.
[97] ArrigoniE, ChamberlinNL, SaperCB, McCarleyRW. Adenosine inhibits basal forebrain cholinergic and noncholinergic neurons in vitro. Neuroscience 2006; 140: 403–13.
[98] ThakkarMM, DelgiaccoRA, StreckerRE, McCarleyRW. Adenosinergic inhibition of basal forebrain wakefulness-active neurons: a simultaneous unit recording and microdialysis study in freely behaving cats. Neuroscience 2003; 122: 1107–13.
[99] ScammellTE, GerashchenkoDY, MochizukiT, et al. An adenosine A2a agonist increases sleep and induces Fos in ventrolateral preoptic neurons. Neuroscience 2001; 107: 653–63.
[100] HuangZL, QuWM, EguchiN, et al. Adenosine A2A, but not A1, receptors mediate the arousal effect of caffeine. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 858–19.
[101] HalassaMM, FlorianC, FellinT, et al. Astrocytic modulation of sleep homeostasis and cognitive consequences of sleep loss. Neuron 2009; 61: 213–9.
[102] BasheerR, BauerA, ElmenhorstD, RameshV, McCarleyRW. Sleep deprivation upregulates A1 adenosine receptors in the rat basal forebrain. Neuroreport 2007; 18: 1895–9.
[103] ElmenhorstD, BasheerR, McCarleyRW, BauerA. Sleep deprivation increases A(1) adenosine receptor density in the rat brain. Brain Res 2009; 1258: 53–8.
[104] RosenbergPA, LiY, LeM, ZhangY. Nitric oxide-stimulated increase in extracellular adenosine accumulation in rat forebrain neurons in culture is associated with ATP hydrolysis and inhibition of adenosine kinase activity. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 6294–301.
[105] BurletS, LegerL, CespuglioR. Nitric oxide and sleep in the rat: a puzzling relationship. Neuroscience 1999; 92: 627–39.
[106] KalinchukAV, LuY, StenbergD, RosenbergPA, Porkka-HeiskanenT. Nitric oxide production in the basal forebrain is required for recovery sleep. J Neurochem 2006; 99: 483–98.
[107] KalinchukAV, StenbergD, RosenbergPA, Porkka-HeiskanenT. Inducible and neuronal nitric oxide synthases (NOS) have complementary roles in recovery sleep induction. Eur J Neurosci 2006; 24: 1443–56.
[108] KalinchukAV, McCarleyRW, Porkka-HeiskanenT, BasheerR. Sleep deprivation triggers inducible nitric oxide-dependent nitric oxide production in wake-active basal forebrain neurons. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 13254–64.
[109] CalabreseV, MancusoC, CalvaniM, et al. Nitric oxide in the central nervous system: neuroprotection versus neurotoxicity. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8: 766–75.
[110] SmallCI, LylesGA, BreenKC. Inducible form of nitric oxide synthase expression in rat cortical neuronal cells in vitro. Neurobiol Dis 2004; 17: 70–6.
[111] MahoneyCR, TaylorHA, KanarekRB. Effect of an afternoon confectionery snack on cognitive processes critical to learning. Physiol Behav 2007; 90: 344–52.
[112] BeningtonJH, HellerHC. Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of sleep. Prog Neurobiol 1995; 45: 347–60.
[113] DworakM, McCarleyRW, KimT, KalinchukAV, BasheerR. Sleep and brain energy levels: ATP changes during sleep. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 9007–16.
[114] ImeriL, OppMR. How (and why) the immune system makes us sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 199–210.
[115] AlamMN, McGintyD, BashirT, et al. Interleukin-1beta modulates state-dependent discharge activity of preoptic area and basal forebrain neurons: role in sleep regulation. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 20: 207–16.
[116] HuangZL, UradeY, HayaishiO. Prostaglandins and adenosine in the regulation of sleep and wakefulness. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2007; 7: 33–8.
[117] RamA, PandeyHP, MatsumuraH, et al. CSF levels of prostaglandins, especially the level of prostaglandin D2, are correlated with increasing propensity towards sleep in rats. Brain Res 1997; 751: 81–9.
[118] ScammellT, GerashchenkoD, UradeY, et al. Activation of ventrolateral preoptic neurons by the somnogen prostaglandin D2. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998; 95: 7754–9.
[119] PentreathVW, ReesK, OwolabiOA, PhilipKA, DouaF. The somnogenic T lymphocyte suppressor prostaglandin D2 is selectively elevated in cerebrospinal fluid of advanced sleeping sickness patients. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg 1990; 84: 795–9.
[120] EllenbogenJM. Cognitive benefits of sleep and their loss due to sleep deprivation. Neurology 2005; 64: E25–7.
[121] DesseillesM, Dang-VuT, SchabusM, et al. Neuroimaging insights into the pathophysiology of sleep disorders. Sleep 2008; 31: 777–94.
[122] DauvilliersY, MorinC, CervenaK, et al. Family studies in insomnia. J Psychosom Res 2005; 58: 271–8.
[123] TaftiM, FrankenP. Molecular analysis of sleep. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 2007; 72: 573–8.
[124] CrockerA, SehgalA. Genetic analysis of sleep. Genes Dev 2010; 24: 1220–35.
[125] Van DongenHP, BaynardMD, MaislinG, DingesDF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 2004; 27: 423–33.
[126] CórdovaCA, SaidBO, McCarleyRW, et al. Sleep deprivation in rats produces attentional impairments on a 5-choice serial reaction time task. Sleep 2006; 29: 69–76.
[127] GuanZ, PengX, FangJ. Sleep deprivation impairs spatial memory and decreases extracellular signal-regulated kinase phosphorylation in the hippocampus. Brain Res 2004; 1018: 38–47.
[128] WardCP, McCarleyRW, StreckerRE. Experimental sleep fragmentation impairs spatial reference but not working memory in Fischer/Brown Norway rats. J Sleep Res 2009; 18: 238–44.
[129] SiapasAG, WilsonMA. Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 1998; 21: 1123–8.
[130] LouieK, WilsonMA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 2001; 29: 145–56.
[131] TartarJL, WardCP, McKennaJT, et al. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. Eur J Neurosci 2006; 23: 2739–48.
[132] PaxinosG, WatsonC. The Rat Brain in Stereotoxic Coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press, 1998.

References

[1] PhilipP, AkerstedtT. Transport and industrial safety, how are they affected by sleepiness and sleep restriction? Sleep Med Rev 2006;10(5): 347–56.
[2] BorbélyAA, AchermannP. In: KrygerMH, RothT, DementWC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders; 2005: 405–17.
[3] LandoltHP. Genotype-dependent differences in sleep, vigilance, and response to stimulants. Curr Pharm Des 2008; 14(32): 3396–407.
[4] Van DongenHP, BaynardMD, MaislinG, DingesDF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 2004; 27(3): 423–33.
[5] AmbrosiusU, LietzenmaierS, WehrleR, et al. Heritability of sleep electroencephalogram. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64(4): 344–8.
[6] De GennaroL, MarzanoC, FratelloF, et al. The electroencephalographic fingerprint of sleep is genetically determined: a twin study. Ann Neurol 2008; 64(4): 455–60.
[7] RaizenDM, WuMN. Genome-wide association studies of sleep disorders. Chest 2011; 139(2): 446–52.
[8] CrockerA, SehgalA. Genetic analysis of sleep. Genes Dev 2010; 24(12): 1220–35.
[9] PosthumaD, de GeusEJ, MulderEJ, et al. Genetic components of functional connectivity in the brain: the heritability of synchronization likelihood. Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 26(3): 191–8.
[10] VolkowND, FowlerJS, LoganJ, et al. Effects of modafinil on dopamine and dopamine transporters in the male human brain: clinical implications. JAMA 2009; 301(11): 1148–54.
[11] VolkowND, WangGJ, TelangF, et al. Sleep deprivation decreases binding of [11C]Raclopride to dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the human brain. J Neurosci 2008; 28(34): 8454–61.
[12] WaterhouseEG, XuB. New insights into the role of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in synaptic plasticity. Mol Cell Neurosci 2009; 42(2): 81–9. [Epub 2009 Jul 3. Review].
[13] PezawasL, VerchinskiBA, MattayVS, et al. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology. J Neurosci 2004; 24(45): 10 099–102.
[14] van BeijsterveldtCE, MolenaarPC, de GeusEJ, BoomsmaDI. Heritability of human brain functioning as assessed by electroencephalography. Am J Hum Genet 1996; 58(3): 562–73.
[15] van BeijsterveldtCE, van BaalGC. Twin and family studies of the human electroencephalogram: a review and a meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 2002; 61(1–2): 111–38.
[16] KoskenvuoM, HublinC, PartinenM, HeikkiläK, KaprioJ. Heritability of diurnal type: a nationwide study of 8753 adult twin pairs. J Sleep Res 2007; 16(2): 156–62.
[17] HeathAC, KendlerKS, EavesLJ, MartinNG. Evidence for genetic influences on sleep disturbance and sleep pattern in twins. Sleep 1990; 13(4): 318–35.
[18] RéteyJV, AdamM, KhatamiR, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2007; 81(5): 692–8.
[19] TanX, CampbellIG, PalaginiL, FeinbergI. High internight reliability of computer-measured NREM delta, sigma, and beta: biological implications. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48(10): 1010–19.
[20] TanX, CampbellIG, FeinbergI. Internight reliability and benchmark values for computer analyses of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) and REM EEG in normal young adult and elderly subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2001; 112(8): 1540–52.
[21] CirulliET, GoldsteinDB. Uncovering the roles of rare variants in common disease through whole-genome sequencing. Nat Rev Genet 2010; 11(6): 415–25.
[22] ManolioTA. Genomewide association studies and assessment of the risk of disease. N Engl J Med 2010; 363(2): 166–76.
[23] ManolioTA, CollinsFS, CoxNJ, et al. Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature 2009; 461(7265): 747–53.
[24] ArcherSN, RobilliardDL, SkeneDJ, et al. A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. Sleep 2003; 26: 413–15.
[25] KatzenbergD, YoungT, FinnL, et al. A CLOCK polymorphism associated with human diurnal preference. Sleep 1998; 21: 569–76.
[26] EbisawaT, UshiyamaM, KajimuraN, et al. Association of structural polymorphisms in the human period3 gene with delayed sleep phase syndrome. EMBO Rep 2001; 21: 342–6.
[27] PereiraDS, TufikS, LouzadaFM, et al. Association of the length polymorphism in the human Per3 gene with the delayed sleep-phase syndrome: does latitude have an influence upon it? Sleep 2005; 28(1): 29–32.
[28] ViolaAU, ArcherSN, JamesLM, et al. PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr Biol 2007; 17: 613–18.
[29] GoelN, BanksS, MignotE, DingesDF. PER3 polymorphism predicts cumulative sleep homeostatic but not neurobehavioral changes to chronic partial sleep deprivation. PLoS One 2009; 4: e5874.
[30] VandewalleG, ArcherSN, WuillaumeC, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 7948–56.
[31] RuppTL, WesenstenNJ, BalkinTJ. PER3 and ADORA2A polymorphisms impact neurobehavioral performance during chronic sleep restriction. Sleep and Biological Rhythms (Suppl) 2011; 9: 414.
[32] BasheerR, StreckerRE, ThakkarMM, McCarleyRW. Adenosine and sleep-wake regulation. Prog Neurobiol 2004; 73(6): 379–96.
[33] HirschhornR, YangDR, IsraniA. An Asp8Asn substitution results in the adenosine deaminase (ADA) genetic polymorphism (ADA 2 allozyme): occurrence on different chromosomal backgrounds and apparent intragenic crossover. Ann Hum Genet 1994; 58: 1–9.
[34] FredholmBB, ChenJF, CunhaRA, SvenningssonP, VaugeoisJM. Adenosine and brain function. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2005; 63: 191–270.
[35] RiksenNP, FrankeB, van den BroekP, et al. The 22G>A polymorphism in the adenosine deaminase gene impairs catalytic function but does not affect reactive hyperaemia in humans in vivo. Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2008; 18(10): 843–6.
[36] PersicoAM, MiliterniR, BravaccioC, et al. Adenosine deaminase alleles and autistic disorder: case-control and family-based association studies. Am J Med Genet 2000; 96: 784–90.
[37] RéteyJV, AdamM, HoneggerE, et al. A functional genetic variation of adenosine deaminase affects the duration and intensity of deep sleep in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005; 102(43): 15 676–81.
[38] BachmannV, KlausF, BodenmannS, et al. Functional ADA polymorphism increases sleep depth and reduces vigilant attention in humans. Cereb Cortex 2011; Jul 6. [Epub ahead of print].
[39] FredholmBB, BattigK, HolmenJ, NehligA, ZvartauEE. Actions of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors that contribute to its widespread use. Pharmacol Rev 1999; 51: 83–133.
[40] PerlisML, SmithMT, AndrewsPJ, OrffH, GilesDE. Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 2001; 24: 110–17.
[41] BodenmannS, HohoffC, FreitagC, et al. Polymorphisms of ADORA2A modulate psychomotor vigilance and the effects of caffeine on neurobehavioral performance and sleep EEG after sleep deprivation. Br J Pharmacol; 2011 Sep 26. doi: 10.1111/j.1476–5381.2011.01689.x [Epub ahead of print].
[42] ChildsE, HohoffC, DeckertJ, et al. Association between ADORA2A and DRD2 polymorphisms and caffeine-induced anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33(12): 2791–800.
[43] RogersPJ, HohoffC, HeatherleySV, et al. Association of the anxiogenic and alerting effects of caffeine with ADORA2A and ADORA1 polymorphisms and habitual level of caffeine consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35(9): 1973–83.
[44] CornelisMC, El-SohemyA, CamposH. Genetic polymorphism of the adenosine A2A receptor is associated with habitual caffeine consumption. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 86(1): 240–4.
[45] AkilM, KolachanaBS, RothmondDA, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype and dopamine regulation in the human brain. J Neurosci 2003; 23: 2008–13.
[46] ChenJ, LipskaBK, HalimN, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 2004; 75(5): 807–21.
[47] HoneaR, VerchinskiBA, PezawasL, et al. Impact of interacting functional variants in COMT on regional gray matter volume in human brain. Neuroimage 2009; 45(1): 44–51.
[48] EnochMA, SchuckitMA, JohnsonBA, GoldmanD. Genetics of alcoholism using intermediate phenotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2003; 27(2): 169–76.
[49] BodenmannS, RusterholzT, DürrR, et al. The functional Val158Met polymorphism of COMT predicts interindividual differences in brain alpha oscillations in young men. J Neurosci 2009; 29(35): 10 855–62.
[50] BodenmannS, XuS, LuhmannUF, et al. Pharmacogenetics of modafinil after sleep loss: catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype modulates waking functions but not recovery sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2009; 85(3): 296–304.
[51] BodenmannS, LandoltHP. Effects of modafinil on the sleep EEG depends on Val158Met genotype of COMT. Sleep 2010; 33(8): 1027–35.
[52] BoutrelB, KoobGF. What keeps us awake: the neuropharmacology of stimulants and wakefulness-promoting medications. Sleep 2004; 27(6): 1181–94.
[53] LuikartBW, ParadaLF. Receptor tyrosine kinase B-mediated excitatory synaptogenesis. Prog Brain Res 2006; 157: 15–24.
[54] HaririAR, GoldbergTE, MattayVS, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J Neurosci 2003; 23(17): 6690–4.
[55] EganMF, KojimaM, CallicottJH, et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 2003; 112(2): 257–69.
[56] GoldmanD, DeanM, LuB, WeinbergerDR. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 2003; 112(2): 257–69.
[57] BachmannV, KleinC, BodenmannS, et al. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism modulates sleep intensity: EEG frequency- and state-specificity. Sleep; In press.
[58] MignotE, YoungT, LinL, FinnL. Nocturnal sleep and daytime sleepiness in normal subjects with HLA-DQB1*0602. Sleep 1999; 22: 347–52.
[59] GoelN, BanksS, MignotE, Dinges DF. DQB1*0602 predicts interindividual differences in physiologic sleep, sleepiness, and fatigue. Neurology 2010; 75: 1509–19.
[60] GottliebDJ, O’ConnorGT, WilkJB. Genome-wide association of sleep and circadian phenotypes. BMC Med Genet 2007; 8 Suppl 1: S9.
[61] YangA, PalmerAA, de WitH. Genetics of caffeine consumption and responses to caffeine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010; 211(3): 245–57.
[62] CornelisMC, MondaKL, YuK, et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis identifies regions on 7p21 (AHR) and 15q24 (CYP1A2) as determinants of habitual caffeine consumption. PLoS Genet 2011; 7(4): e1002033.
[63] SulemP, GudbjartssonDF, GellerF, et al. Sequence variants at CYP1A1-CYP1A2 and AHR associate with coffee consumption. Hum Mol Genet 2011; 20(10): 2071–7.
[64] DijkDJ, ArcherSN. PERIOD3, Circadian phenotypes, and sleep homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 2010; 14(3): 151–60.