Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T14:35:44.741Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Toward Building a Better Brain: Neurobehavioral Outcomes, Mechanisms, and Processes of Environmental Enrichment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

W. John Curtis
Affiliation:
Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY
Charles A. Nelson
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN
Suniya S. Luthar
Affiliation:
Columbia University, New York
Get access

Summary

Research over the second half of the 20th century has painted a very consistent picture of the impact of differential environments on the brain and behavior of various nonhuman animal species. Beginning with the early pioneering work of Hebb in the 1940s (1947, 1949), scores of studies have demonstrated that rearing animals in enriched environments results in discernible differences in both brain morphology and observable behavior when compared to animals reared in standard laboratory conditions. Many different animal species have been utilized in many variations on this basic experimental design and, invariably, similar results emerge. Indeed, it has become nearly axiomatic that exposing experimental animals to enriched environments leads to positive outcomes in terms of brain development and their ability to learn.

During the early and mid-1960s, when many of the initial animal enrichment experiments were first underway, a social and political experiment, also based on the idea of environmental enrichment, was being formulated. The central idea was that enrichment of the environment of disadvantaged children would result in enhanced cognitive development and social competence beyond that which would be expected in these children given the disadvantage of poverty. The juxtaposition of these two endeavors, one occurring in the realm of biological science and the other motivated by governmental social policy, was probably not coincidental. More likely, both were to some degree the result of a prevailing optimism in the culture during the 1960s about the potential for improving the human condition via modification of the environment.

Type
Chapter
Information
Resilience and Vulnerability
Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities
, pp. 463 - 488
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alcantra, A. A., Saks, N. D., & Greenough, W. T. (1991). Fos is expressed in the rat during forelimb reaching task. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 17, 141Google Scholar
Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford: Blackwell
Barnett, W. S. (1996). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes. Future Children, 5, 25–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, E. L. (1976). Cerebral effects of differential experiences and training. In M. R. Rosenzweig & E. L. Bennett (Eds.), Neural mechanisms of learning and memory (pp. 279–289). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Bennett, E. L., Diamond, M. C., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain, Science, 46, 610–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertenthal, B., Campos, J., & Barrett, K. (1984). Self-produced locomotion: An organizer of emotional, cognitive, and social development in infancy. In R. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development (pp. 175–210). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Brown, R. T. (1968). Early experience and problem-solving ability. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 65, 433–440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, C. T. (1995). Cognitive and school outcomes for high-risk African-American students at middle adolescence: Positive effects of early intervention. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 743–772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapillon, P., Manneche, C., Belzung, C., & Caston, J. (1999). Rearing environmental enrichment in two inbred strains of mice: 1. Effects on emotional reactivity. Behavior Genetics, 29, 41–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denenberg, V. H., Woodcock, J. M., & Rosenberg, K. M. (1968). Long-term effects of preweaning and postweaning free-environment experience on rat problem-solving behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 533–535CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, M. C. (1988). Enriching heredity. News York: Free Press
Diamond, M. C., Law, F., Rhodes, H., Lindner, B., Rosenzweig, M. R., Krech, D., & Bennett, E. l. (1966). Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to enriched environments. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 128, 117–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, M. C., Linder, B., & Raymond, A. (1967). Extensive cortical depth measurements and neuron size increases in the cortex of environmentally enriched rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 131, 357–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1998). Intelligence. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Farran, D. C. (2000). Another decade of interventions for children who are low income or disabled: What do we know now? In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. 510–548). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Farran, D. C. (2001). Critical periods and early intervention. In D. B. Bailey, F. Symons, J. Bruer, & J. Lichtman (Eds.), Critical thinking about critical periods (pp. 233–266). Baltimore: Paul Brookes
Ferchmin, P. A., Bennett, E. L., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1975). Direct contact with enriched environment is required to alter cerebral weights in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 360–367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floeter, M. K. & Greenough, W. T. (1979). Cerebellar plasticity: Modification of Purkinje cell structure by differential rearing in monkeys. Science, 206, 227–229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., Hwang, H. M. F., & Gorman, C. (1985). Evidence for active synapse formation, or altered post-synaptic metabolism, in visual cortex of rats reared in complex environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 82, 4549–4552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., Volkmar, F. R., & Juraska, J. M. (1973). Effects of rearing complexity on dendritic branching in frontolateral and temporal cortex of the rat. Experimental Neurology, 41, 371–378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., Wallace, C. S., Alcantra, A. A., Anderson, B. J., Hawrylak, N., Sirevaag, A. M., Weiler, I. J., & Withers, G. S. (1993). Development of the brain: Experience affects the structure of neurons, glia, and blood vessels. In N. J. Anastasiow & S. Harel (Eds.). At-risk infants: Interventions, families, and research (pp. 173–185). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes
Griffin, D. R. (Ed.). (1982). Animal mind – human mind. New York: Springer Verlag
Grouse, L. D., Schrier, B. K., Bennett, E. L., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Nelson, P. G. (1978). Sequence diversity studies of rat brain RNA: Effects of environmental complexity on rat brain RNA diversity. Journal of Neurochemistry, 30, 191–203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D. O. (1947). The effects of early experience on problem-solving at maturity. American Psychologist, 2, 306–307Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley
Hebb, D. O., & Williams, K. (1946). A method of rating animal intelligence. Journal of General Psychology, 34, 56–65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, F. D., & Paden, L. Y. (1973). The effectiveness of environmental intervention programs. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 3, pp. 331–402). Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Hunt, J. M. (1964). The psychological basis for using pre-school enrichment as an antidote for cultural deprivation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 10, 209–248Google Scholar
Hymovitch, B. (1952). The effects of experimental variations on problem solving in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 313–321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement?Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, T. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1996). Ultrastructural evidence for increased contact between astrocytes and synapses in rats reared in a complex environment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65, 48–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, T. A., Klintsova, A. Y., Kilman, V. L., Sirevaag, A. M., & Greenough, W. T. (1997). Induction of multiple synapses by experience in the visual cortex of adult rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 68, 13–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juraska, J. M., Greenough, W. T., & Conlee, J. W. (1983). Differential rearing affects responsiveness of rats to depressant and convulsant drugs. Physiology and Behavior, 31, 711–715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, E. R., & Hawkins, R. D. (1992). The biological basis of learning and individuality. Scientific American, 267, 79–86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., & Gage, F. H. (1997). More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature, 386, 493–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiyono, S., Seo, M. L., & Shibagahi, M. (1981). Effects of rearing environments upon sleep-waking parameters in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 26, 391–395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiyono, S., Seo, M. L., Shibagaki, M., & Inouye, M. (1985). Facilitative effects of maternal environmental enrichment on maze learning in rat offspring. Physiology and Behavior, 34, 431–435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleim, J. A., Swain, R. A., Armstrong, K. A., Napper, R. M. A., Jones, T. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1998). Selective synaptic plasticity within the cerebellar cortex following complex motor skill learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 69, 274–289CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krech, D., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Bennett, E. L. (1960). Effects of environmental complexity and training on brain chemistry. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 53, 509–519CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krech, D., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Bennett, E. L. (1962). Relations between brain chemistry and problem-solving among rats raised in enriched and impoverished environments. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 801–807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuenzle, C. C., & Knusel, A. (1974). Mass training of rats in a superenriched environment. Physiology and Behavior, 13, 205–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazar, I., Darlington, R., Murray, H., Royce, J., & Snipper, A. (1982). Lasting effects of early education: A report from the Consortium for Longitudinal Studies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47 (Serial No. 195)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leah, J., Allardyce, H., & Cummins, R. (1985). Evoked cortical potential correlates of rearing environment in rats. Biological Psychology, 20, 21–29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lore, R. K. (1969). Pain avoidance behavior of rats reared in restricted and enriched environments. Developmental Psychology, 1, 482–484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luchins, A. S., & Forgus, R. H. (1955). The effects of differential post-weaning environment on the rigidity of an animal's behavior. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 86, 51–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malkasian, D., & Diamond, M. C. (1971). The effect of environmental manipulation on the morphology of the neonatal rat brain. International Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 161–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, S. L., Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1990). The prevention of intellectual impairment in children of impoverished families: Findings of a randomized trial of educational daycare. American Journal of Public Health, 80, 844–847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, M. J. (1973). Effects of postweaning environment of learning in the rat. Animal Behaviour, 21, 429–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ough, B. R., Beatty, W. W., & Khalili, J. (1972). Effects of isolated and enriched rearing on response inhibition. Psychonomic Science, 27, 293–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1992). Effective early intervention. Mental Retardation, 30, 337–345Google ScholarPubMed
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1998). Prevention of intellectual disabilities: Early interventions to improve cognitive development. Preventive Medicine, 27, 224–232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive matrices. London: Lewis
Renner, M. J., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1987). Enriched and impoverished environments: Effects on brain and behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag
Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L., & Diamond, M. C. (1972). Brain changes in response to experience. Scientific American, 226, 22–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L., Diamond, M. C., Wu, S. Y., Slagle, R., & Saffran, E. (1969). Influence of environmental complexity and visual stimulation on development of occipital cortex in the rat. Brain Research, 14, 427–445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, H. V. (1972). Effects of environmental enrichment on open-field activity and Hebb–Williams problem solving in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 80, 163–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyderman, M., & Rothman, S. (1988). The IQ controversy, the media, and public policy. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Susser, E. R., & Wallace, R. B. (1982). The effects of environmental complexity on the hippocampal formation of the adult rat. Acta Neurobiologica Experimentalis, 42, 203–207Google ScholarPubMed
Torasdotter, M., Metsis, M., Henriksson, B. G., Winblad, B., & Mohammed, A. H. (1998). Environmental enrichment results in higher levels of nerve growth factor mRNA in the rat visual cortex and hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research, 93, 83–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, A. M., & Greenough, W. T. (1985). Differential rearing effects on rat visual cortex synapses. I. Synaptic and neuronal density and synapses per neuron. Brain Research, 329, 195–203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wahlsten, D., & Gottlieb, G. (1997). The invalid separation of effects of nature and nurture: Lessons from animal experimentation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 163–192). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Weinberg, R. A., Scarr, S., & Waldman, I. D. (1992). The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A follow-up of IQ test performance at adolescence. Intelligence, 16, 117–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E., & Valentine, J. (Eds.). (1979). Project head start: A legacy of the war on poverty. New York: Free Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×