Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T19:30:47.512Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Motor Adaptation and Proprioceptive Recalibration

from I - VISUAL AND VISUOMOTOR PLASTICITY

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2013

Danielle Salomonczyk
Affiliation:
York University
Erin K. Cressman
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Denise Y. P. Henriques
Affiliation:
York University
Jennifer K. E. Steeves
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Laurence R. Harris
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The central nervous system (CNS) integrates information from multiple sensory modalities, including visual and proprioceptive information, when planning a reaching movement (Jeannerod, 1988). Although visual and proprioceptive information regarding hand (or end point effector) position are not always consistent, performance is typically better under reaching conditions in which both sources of information are available. Under certain task conditions, visual signals tend to dominate such that one relies more on visual information than proprioception to guide movement. For example, individuals reaching to a target with misaligned visual feedback of the hand, as experienced when reaching in a virtual reality environment or while wearing prism displacement goggles, adjust their movements in order for the visual representation of the hand to achieve the desired end point even when their actual hand is elsewhere in the workspace (Krakauer et al., 1999, 2000; Redding and Wallace, 1996; Simani et al., 2007). This motor adaptation typically occurs rapidly, reaching baseline levels within twenty trials per target, and without participants' awareness (Krakauer et al., 2000). Furthermore, participants reach with these adapted movement patterns following removal of the distortion, and hence show aftereffects (Baraduc and Wolpert, 2002; Buch et al., 2003; Krakauer et al., 1999, 2000; Martin et al., 1996). These aftereffects provide a measure of motor learning referred to as visuomotor adaptation and result from the CNS learning a new visuomotor mapping to guide movement.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abeele, S. and Bock, O. (2001). Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Exp. Brain Res., 140: 407–410.Google Scholar
Adamo, D. E., Alexander, N. B. and Brown, S. H. (2009). The influence of age and physical activity on upper limb proprioceptive ability. J. Aging Phys. Act., 17: 272–293.Google Scholar
Adamo, D. E., Martin, B. J. and Brown, S. H. (2007). Age-related differences in upper limb proprioceptive acuity. Percept. Mot. Skills, 104: 1297–1309.Google Scholar
al-Falahe, N. A., Nagaoka, M. and Vallbo, A. B. (1990). Response profiles of human muscle afferents during active finger movements. Brain, 113: 325–346.Google Scholar
Baraduc, P. and Wolpert, D. M. (2002). Adaptation to a visuomotor shift depends on the starting posture. J. Neurophysiol., 88: 973–981.Google Scholar
Bernier, P. M., Chua, R. and Franks, I. M. (2005). Is proprioception calibrated during visually guided movements?Exp. Brain Res., 167: 292–296.Google Scholar
Berniker, M. and Kording, K. (2008). Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization. Nat. Neurosci., 11: 1454–1461.Google Scholar
Bock, O. and Girgenrath, M. (2006). Relationship between sensorimotor adaptation and cognitive functions in younger and older subjects. Exp. Brain Res., 169: 400–406.Google Scholar
Buch, E. R., Young, S. and Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2003). Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learn. Mem., 10: 55–63.Google Scholar
Clower, D. M. and Boussaoud, D. (2000). Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. J. Neurophysiol., 84: 2703–2708.Google Scholar
Coslett, H., Buxbaum, L. and Schwoebel, J. (2008). Accurate reaching after active but not passive movements of the hand: evidence for forward modeling. Behav. Neurol., 19: 117–125.Google Scholar
Craske, B. and Gregg, S. J. (1966). Prism after-effects: identical results for visual targets and unexposed limb. Nature, 212: 104–105.Google Scholar
Cressman, E. K. and Henriques, D. Y. (2009). Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol., 102: 3505–3518.Google Scholar
Cressman, E. K. and Henriques, D. Y. (2010). Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. J. Neurophysiol., 103: 1888–1895.Google Scholar
Cressman, E. K., Salomonczyk, D. and Henriques, D. Y. (2010). Visuomotor adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration in older adults. Exp. Brain Res., 205: 533–544.Google Scholar
Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Bastian, A. J. and Shadmehr, R. (2010). Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. J. Neurophysiol., 103: 2275–2284.Google Scholar
Fuentes, C. T. and Bastian, A. J. (2010). Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks. J. Neurophysiol., 103: 164–171.Google Scholar
Goble, D. J. and Brown, S. H. (2008). Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. J. Neurophysiol., 99: 3063–3074.Google Scholar
Goble, D. J., Coxon, J. P., Wenderoth, N., Van Impe, A. and Swinnen, S. P. (2009). Proprioceptive sensibility in the elderly: degeneration, functional consequences and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 33: 271–278.Google Scholar
Harris, C. S. (1963). Adaptation to displaced vision: visual, motor, or proprioceptive change?Science, 140: 812–813.Google Scholar
Harris, C. S. (1965). Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. Psychol. Rev., 72: 419–444.Google Scholar
Hatada, Y., Rossetti, Y. and Miall, R. C. (2006). Long-lasting aftereffect of a single prism adaptation: shifts in vision and proprioception are independent. Exp. Brain Res., 173: 415–424.Google Scholar
Hay, J. C. and Pick, H. L. Jr., (1966). Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. J. Exp. Psychol., 71: 150–158.Google Scholar
Hay, J. C., Pick, H. L. Jr. and Ikeda, K. (1965). Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychon. Sci., 2: 215–216.Google Scholar
Ingram, H. A., van Donkelaar, P., Cole, J., Vercher, J. L., Gauthier, G. M. and Miall, R. C. (2000). The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task. Exp. Brain Res., 132: 114–126.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1988). The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal-Directed Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jones, S. A., Cressman, E. K. and Henriques, D. Y. (2010). Proprioceptive localization of the left and right hands. Exp. Brain Res., 204: 373–383.Google Scholar
Kagerer, F. A., Contreras-Vidal, J. L. and Stelmach, G. E. (1997). Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp. Brain Res., 115: 557–561.Google Scholar
Kaplan, F. S., Nixon, J. E., Reitz, M., Rindfleish, L. and Tucker, J. (1985). Age-related changes in proprioception and sensation of joint position. Acta Orthop. Scand., 56: 72–74.Google Scholar
Klassen, J., Tong, C. and Flanagan, J. R. (2005). Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations. Exp. Brain Res., 164: 250–259.Google Scholar
Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F. and Ghez, C. (1999). Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat. Neurosci., 2: 1026–1031.Google Scholar
Krakauer, J. W., Pine, Z. M., Ghilardi, M. F. and Ghez, C. (2000). Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J. Neurosci., 20: 8916–8924.Google Scholar
Laufer, Y., Hocherman, S. and Dickstein, R. (2001). Accuracy of reproducing hand position when using active compared with passive movement. Physiother. Res. Int., 6: 65–75.Google Scholar
Malfait, N., Henriques, D. Y. and Gribble, P. L. (2008). Shape distortion produced by isolated mismatch between vision and proprioception. J. Neurophysiol., 99: 231–243.Google Scholar
Martin, T. A., Keating, J. G., Goodkin, H. P., Bastian, A. J. and Thach, W. T. (1996). Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain, 119: 1183–1198.Google Scholar
McCloskey, D. (1980). Knowledge about muscular contractions. Trends Neurosci., 3: 311–314.Google Scholar
Miall, R. C. and Wolpert, D. M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw., 9: 1265–1279.Google Scholar
Michel, C., Pisella, L., Prablanc, C., Rode, G. and Rossetti, Y. (2007). Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 19: 341–350.Google Scholar
Ostry, D. J., Darainy, M., Mattar, A. A.,Wong, J. and Gribble, P. L. (2010). Somatosensory plasticity and motor learning. J. Neurosci., 30: 5384–5393.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (1978). Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation. Percept. Psychophys., 24: 58–62.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (1988). Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: components of prism adaptation. J. Mot. Behav., 20: 242–254.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (1996). Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptualmotor control. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 22: 379–394.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (1997). Prism adaptation during target pointing from visible and nonvisible starting locations. J. Mot. Behav., 29: 119–130.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (2000). Prism exposure aftereffects and direct effects for different movement and feedback times. J. Mot. Behav., 32: 83–99.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (2001). Calibration and alignment are separable: evidence from prism adaptation. J. Mot. Behav., 33: 401–412.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (2002). Strategic calibration and spatial alignment: a model from prism adaptation. J. Mot. Behav., 34: 126–138.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (2003). Dual prism adaptation: calibration or alignment?J. Mot. Behav., 35: 399–408.Google Scholar
Redding, G. M. and Wallace, B. (2006). Generalization of prism adaptation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 32: 1006–1022.Google Scholar
Salomonczyk, D., Cressman, E. K. and Henriques, D. Y. (2011). Proprioceptive recalibration following prolonged training and increasing distortions in visuomotor adaptation. Neuropsychologia, 49: 3053–3062.Google Scholar
Salomonczyk, D., Henriques, D. Y. and Cressman, E. K. (2012). Proprioceptive recalibration in the right and left hands following abrupt visuomotor adaptation. Exp. Brain Res., 217: 187–196.Google Scholar
Shadmehr, R., Smith, M.A. and Krakauer, J.W. (2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Ann. Rev. Neurosci., 33: 89–108.Google Scholar
Simani, M. C., McGuire, L. M. and Sabes, P. N. (2007). Visual-shift adaptation is composed of separable sensory and task-dependent effects. J. Neurophysiol., 98: 2827–2841.Google Scholar
Templeton, W. B., Howard, I. P. and Wilkinson, D. A. (1974). Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation. Percept. Psychophys., 15: 249–257.Google Scholar
Tseng, Y. W., Diedrichsen, J., Krakauer, J. W., Shadmehr, R. and Bastian, A. J. (2007). Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J. Neurophysiol., 98: 54–62.Google Scholar
van Beers, R. J., Wolpert, D. M. and Haggard, P. (2002). When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation. Curr. Biol., 12: 834–837.Google Scholar
Wei, K. and Kording, K. (2009). Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation?J. Neurophysiol., 101: 655–664.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. and Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269: 1880–1882.Google Scholar
Wong, A. and Shelhamer, M. (2011). Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. J. Neurophysiol., 105: 1130–1140.Google Scholar
Wong, T. and Henriques, D. Y. (2009). Visuomotor adaptation does not recalibrate kinesthetic sense of felt hand path. J. Neurophysiol., 101: 614–623.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×