Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T18:11:53.385Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2009

Paul Churchland
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (1996). “Neuropsychological Approaches to Reasoning and Decision Making.” In Damasio, A. R. et al., eds., The Neurobiology of Decision-Making, pp. 157–80. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, Object, and Conceptual Development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S. W. (1994). “Insensitivity to Future Consequences Following Damage to Human Prefrontal Cortex.” Cognition 50:7–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickle, J. (1998). Psychoneural Reduction: The New Wave. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, A., and Hilbert, D. R. (2003). “Color Realism and Color Science.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26:3–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. (1974). “Evolutionary Epistemology,” in Schilpp, P. A., ed., The Philosophy of Karl Popper, pp. 413–63. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The Conscious Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cherniak, C., Changizi, M., and Kang, D. (1999). “Large-scale Optimization of Neuron Arbors.” Physical Review E 59, no. 5: 6001–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, P. M. (1979). Scientific Realism and the Plasticity of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1981). “Eliminative Materialism and the Propositional Attitudes.” Journal of Philosophy 78, no. 2: 67–90.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1982). “Is Thinker a Natural Kind?Dialogue 21, no. 2: 223–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1985). “Reduction, Qualia, and the Direct Introspection of Brain States.” Journal of Philosophy 82, no. 1: 8–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1986). Matter and Consciousness. Revised edition, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1988). “Perceptual Plasticity and Theoretical Neutrality: A Reply to Jerry Fodor.” Philosophy of Science 55: 167–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989a). A Neurocomputational Perspective: The Nature of Mind and the Structure of Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989b). “On the Nature of Theories: A Neurocomputational Perspective.” In Savage, W., ed., Scientific Theories. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 14, pp. 59–101. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Chapter 9 of P. M. Churchland (1989a).Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989c). “On the Nature of Explanation: A PDP Approach.” Chapter 10 of P. M. Churchland (1989a). Reprinted in Misiek, J. ed., Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science, Vol. 175, pp. 81–113. Dordrecht, Holland: Kluwer, 1995.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1989d). “Learning and Conceptual Change.” Chapter 11 of P. M. Churchland (1989a).
Churchland, P. M. (1995b). The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1996a). “Fodor and Lepore: State-Space Semantics and Meaning Holism.” In McCauley (1996), pp. 272–7.
Churchland, P. M., (1996b). “Second Reply to Fodor and Lepore.” In McCauley (1996), pp. 278–83.
Churchland, P. M. (1996c). “The Rediscovery of Light.” Journal of Philosophy 93, no. 5: 211–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1998). “Conceptual Similarity across Sensory and Neural Diversity: The Fodor/Lepore Challenge Answered.” Journal of Philosophy 95, no. 1 (Jan.): 5–32.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1999a). “Densmore and Dennett on Virtual Machines and Consciousness.” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 59, no. 3 (Sept): 763–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1999b). “Review of Reason, Regulation, and Realism.” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 58, no. 4: 541–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (2005). “Chimerical Colors: Some Phenomenological Predictions from Cognitive Neuroscience.” Philosophical Psychology 18, no. 5: 527–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (in preparation). “Inner Spaces and Outer Spaces: The New Epistemology.”
Churchland, P. M., and Churchland, P. S. (1997). “Recent Work on Consciousness: Philosophical, Theoretical, and Empirical.” Seminars in Neurology 17, no. 2: 179–86. Reprinted in Churchland, P. M. and Churchland, P. S.. On the Contrary, pp. 159–76. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1998.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1992). The Computational Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2000). “Word and Action: Reconciling Rules and Know-How in Moral Cognition.” In R. Campbell and B. Hunter, eds., Moral Epistemology Naturalized. Canadian Journal of Philosophy. Suppl. vol. 26: 267–90.Google Scholar
Clark, Austen (1993). Sensory Qualities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (forthcoming). “Color Properties and Color Ascriptions: A Relationalist Manifesto.” Philosophical Review.
Cottrell, G. (1991). “Extracting Features from Faces Using Compression Networks: Face, Identity, Emotions and Gender Recognition Using Holons.” In Touretzky, D., Elman, J., Sejnowski, T. and Hinton, G. eds., Connectionist Models: Proceedings of the 1990 Summer School, pp. 328–37. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Cottrell, G., and Laakso, A. (2000). “Qualia and Cluster Analysis: Assessing Representational Similarity between Neural Systems.” Philosophical Psychology 13, no. 1: 77–95.Google Scholar
Cottrell, G., and Metcalfe, J. (1991). “EMPATH: Face, Emotion, and Gender Recognition Using Holons.” In Lippman, R. et al., eds., Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, vol. 3, pp. 1–7. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Cottrell, G. W., and Tsung, F. (1993). “Learning Simple Arithmetic Procedures.” Connection Science 5, no. 1: 37–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, R. (1997). “The Lot of the Causal Theory of Mental Content.” Journal of Philosophy 94, no. 10: 535–42.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' Error. New York: Putnam.Google ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (1991). “Somatic Markers and the Guidance of Behavior.” In Levin, H. et al., eds., Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, pp. 217–29. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., et al., eds. (1996). The Neurobiology of Decision-Making. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. (1970). “Mental Events.” In Foster, L. and Swanson, J. eds., Experience and Theory, pp. 79–101. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, M. S. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, M. S. (1982). The Extended Phenotype. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1984). “Cognitive Wheels: The Frame Problem in Artificial Intelligence.” In Hookway, C. ed., Minds, Machines, and Evolution, pp. 129–51. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2005). “Two Steps Closer on Consciousness.” In Keeley, B. L., ed., Paul Churchland, pp. 193–209. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C., and Densmore, S. (1999). “The Virtues of Virtual Machines.” In Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 59, no. 3: 747–67.Google Scholar
DYG (2000). “Evolution and Creationism in Public Education: An In-depth Reading of Public Opinion” (March). A national survey by DYG, Inc., 36A Padanaram Road, Danbury, CT 06811.
Edelman, S. (1998). “Representation Is Representation of similarities.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:449–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. L. (1992). “Grammatical Structure and Distributed Representations.” In Davis, S., ed., Connectionism: Theory and Practice. Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science, vol. 3, pp. 138–94. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elman, J., Bates, E., et al. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P. K. (1962). “Explanation, Reduction, and Empiricism.” In Feigl, H. and Maxwell, G., eds., Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 3, pp. 28–97. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P. K. (1963a). “How to Be a Good Empiricist – A Plea for Tolerance in Matters Epistemological.” In Baumrin, B., ed., Philosophy of Science: The Delaware Seminar, vol. 2, pp. 3–19. New York: Interscience Publications. Reprinted in B. Brody, ed., Readings in the Philosophy of Science, pp. 104–22. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1970.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P. K. (1963b). “Materialism and the Mind-Body Problem.” Review of Metaphysics 17: 49–66.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1991). Varieties of Moral Personality: Ethics and Psychological Realism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1996). “The Moral Network.” In McCauley (1996), pp. 192–215.
Fodor, J. A. (1974). “The Special Sciences,” Synthese 28: 77–115.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. New York: Crowell.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1984). “Observation Reconsidered,” Philosophy of Science 51: 23–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1988). “A Reply to Churchland's ‘Perceptual Plasticity and Theoretical Neutrality.’” Philosophy of Science 55: 188–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990). A Theory of Content and Other Essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2000). The Mind Doesn't Work That Way. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., and Lepore, E. (1992). “Paul Churchland and State-Space Semantics.” Chapter 7 of Holism: A Shopper's Guide. Oxford: Blackwell. Reprinted in McCauley (1996), pp. 187–207. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., and Lepore, E. (1996). “Reply to Churchland.” In McCauley (1996), pp. 159–62.
Fodor, J. A., and Lepore, E. (1999). “All at Sea in Semantic Space: Churchland on Meaning Similarity.” Journal of Philosophy 96, no. 8: 381–403.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Garrett, M., et al. (1985). “Against Definitions.” Cognition 8: 1–105. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. Reprinted in Margolis, E. and Laurence, S.Concepts: Core Readings, pp. 491–512. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999.Google Scholar
Fraser, B., et al. (2003). Color Management. Berkeley, CA: Peachpit Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1976). “Discrimination and Perceptual Knowledge.” Journal of Philosophy 73:771–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (1986). Epistemology and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. (1972). Problems and Projects: Seven Strictures on Similarity. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Gorman, R. P., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1988a). “Analysis of Hidden Units in a Layered Network Trained to Classify Sonar Targets.” Neural Networks 1:75–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorman, R. P., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1988b). “Learned Classification of Sonar Targets Using a Massively-Parallel Network.” IEEE Transactions: Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing 36:1135–40.Google Scholar
Griffin, L. D. (2001). “Similarity of Psychological and Physical Color Space Shown by Symmetry AnalysisColor: Research and Application 26, no. 2: 151–7.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardin, C. L. (1988). Color for Philosophers: Unweaving the Rainbow.
Hardin, C. L. (1993). Color for Philosophers: Unweaving the Rainbow. Expanded edition. Hackett.
Hooker, C. A. (1995). Reason, Regulation, and Realism: Toward a Regulatory Systems Theory of Reason and Evolutionary Epistemology. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Hurlbert, A. (2001). “Trading Faces.” Nature Neuroscience 4, no. 1: 3–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurvich, L. M. (1981). Color Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1866). Elementary Lessons in Physiology. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1982). “Epiphenomenal Qualia.” Philosophical Quarterly 32, no. 127: 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1993). Moral Imagination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1982). Abusing Science: The Case against Creationism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1972). “Naming and Necessity.” In Davidson, D. and Harman, G. eds., Semantics of Natural Language, pp. 253–355. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehni, R. (2003). Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1974). “Second Thoughts on Paradigms.” In Suppe, F. ed., The Structure of Scientific Theories, pp. 459–82. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I. (1970). “Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programs.” In Lakatos, I. and Musgrave, A., eds., Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge, pp. 91–196. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehky, S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1988). “Network Model of Shape-from-Shading: Neuronal Function Arises from Both Receptive and Projective Fields.” Nature 333:452–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehky, S., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1990). “Neural Network Model of Visual Cortex for Determining Surface Curvature from Images of Shaded Surfaces.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B240:251–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leopold, D. A., Toole, O' A. J., et al. (2001). “Prototype-Referenced Shape Encoding Revealed by High-Level Aftereffects,” Nature Neuroscience 4, no. 1: 89–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, J. (1983). “Materialism and Qualia: The Explanatory Gap.” Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 64:354–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. (1689). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, Book II, ch. viii.
Lockery, S. R., Fang, Y., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1991). “A Dynamical Neural Network Model of Sensorimotor Transformation in the Leech.” Neural Computation 2:274–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, A. (1981). After Virtue. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. (1999). Dependent Rational Animals: Why Human Beings Need the Virtues. La Salle, IL: The Open Court.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N. (1996). The Churchlands and Their Critics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What Is It Like to Be a Bat?Philosophical Review 83, no. 4: 435–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, G. (1999). “Connectionism, Analogicity and Mental Content.” Acta Analytica 22:111–31.Google Scholar
O'Brien, G., and Opie, J. (2006). “Notes Toward a Structuralist Theory of Mental Representation.” In Clapin, H. et al., eds., Representations in Mind: New Approaches to Mental Representation, in press. Westport, CT: Greenwood.Google Scholar
Pennock, R. T. (1999). Tower of Babel: The Evidence against the New Creationism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1934). Logik der Forschung. Wien. Published in English as The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Hutchison, 1980.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1972). “Conjectures and Refutations.” In Conjectures and Refutations, pp. 33–65. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1979). Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction a l'epistemologie genetique, 3 vols. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). Insights and Illusions of Philosophy. New York: Meridian Books.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic Epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press, 1970.Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. (1951). “Two Dogmas of Empiricism.” Philosophical Review 60:00–00.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. (1969). “Natural Kinds.” Ontological Relativity and Other Essays, pp. 69–90. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Rickless, Samuel C. (1997). “Locke on Primary and Secondary Qualities.” Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 78:297–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rojas, R. (1996). Neural Networks: A Systematic Introduction. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rorty, R. (1965). “Mind-Body Identity, Privacy, and Categories.” Review of Metaphysics 19:24–54.Google Scholar
Rosen, S., and Howell, P. (1987). “Auditory, Articulatory, and Learning Explanations of Categorical Perception in Speech.” In Harnad, S. ed., Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition, pp. 113–60. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, C. R., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1987). “Parallel Networks that Learn to Pronounce English Text.” Complex Systems 1:145–68.Google Scholar
Roweis, S. T., and Saul, L. K. (2000). “Nonlinear Dimensionality Reduction by Locally Linear Embedding.” Science 290, no. 5500 (Dec. 22): 2323–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saver, J. L., and Damasio, A. R. (1991). “Preserved Access and Processing of Social Knowledge in a Patient with Acquired Sociopathy Due to Ventromedial Frontal Damage.” Neuropsychologia 29:1241–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schrodinger, E. (1944). What is Life?Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sejnowski, T. J. (1988). “Computing Shape from Shading with a Neural Network Model.” In Schwartz, E., ed., Computational Neuroscience, pp. 452–4. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sellars, W. (1963). Science, Perception, and Reality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Seung, H. S., and Lee, D. D. (2000). “Cognition: The Manifold Ways of Perception.” Science 290, no. 5500 (Dec. 22): 2268–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1968). “Cognitive Psychology: A Review of the Book by Ulrich Neisser.” American Journal of Psychology 81:285–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1980). “Multidimensional Scaling, Tree-Fitting, and Clustering.” Science 210:390–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strevens, M. (2003). Bigger than Chaos. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tenenbaum, J. B., Silva, V., and Langford, J. C. (2000). “A Global Geometric Framework for Nonlinear Dimensionality Reduction.” Science 290, no. 5500 (Dec. 22): 2319–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, E., Palacios, A., and Varela, F. (1992). “Ways of Coloring: Comparative Color Vision as a Case Study for Cognitive Science.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiffany, E. (1999). “Comments and Criticism: Semantics San Diego Style.” Journal of Philosophy 96, no. 8: 416–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toulmin, S. (1972). Human Understanding. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Turing, A. (1950). “Computing Machinery and Intelligence.” Mind 59:433–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, J. (2000). The Computer and the Brain. New edition. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Zeki, S. (1980). “The Representation of Colours in the Cerebral Cortex.” Nature 284:412–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Paul Churchland, University of California, San Diego
  • Book: Neurophilosophy at Work
  • Online publication: 24 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511498435.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Paul Churchland, University of California, San Diego
  • Book: Neurophilosophy at Work
  • Online publication: 24 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511498435.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Paul Churchland, University of California, San Diego
  • Book: Neurophilosophy at Work
  • Online publication: 24 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511498435.014
Available formats
×