Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2011
  • Online publication date: June 2012

4 - Cognitive Models of Task Performance for Scientific Reasoning and Discovery

Summary

Science and scientific progress are products of skilled methods of thinking about and acting on the world. The innovation, systematicness, and logic comprising scientific reasoning and discovery are therefore natural goals for governments interested in cultivating a literate, technologically savvy, and economically productive citizenry in the twenty-first century (Hanushek, 2005). As noted in Chapter 1, recent international assessments indicate variability in how well students of different nations have mastered scientific knowledge and skills. For example, U.S. examinees scored below average on two out of three content area sub-scales in the 2006 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), namely, explaining phenomena scientifically and using scientific evidence (see Baldi et al., 2007; see also Organization Economic Cooperation Development [OECD], 2006). This finding is not new. In 1982–1983, the National Commission on Excellence in Education, composed of a select group of university presidents, professors, and K–12 teachers, compiled a grim report on the state of science and mathematics' education in the United States: A Nation at Risk. Moreover, the National Research Council's (2007) Taking Science to School indicates that at the time A Nation at Risk (1983) was disseminated, U.S. President Ronald Reagan had already cautioned the National Academies of Sciences and Engineering of an impending decline in science and mathematics achievement, and of the erosion this decline could eventually cause the American people in their standards of living.

Related content

Powered by UNSILO
References
Agan, L. & Sneider, C. (2004). Learning about the earth's shape and gravity: A guide for teachers and curriculum developers. Astronomy Education Review, 2, 90–117. Available at http://aer.noao.edu/cgi-bin/new.pl [accessed Jan. 2010].
,American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1993). Benchmarks for science literacy. New York: Oxford University Press.
Baldi, S., Jin, Y., Skemer, M., Green, P.J., & Herget, D. (2007). Highlights from PISA 2006: Performance of U.S. 15-year-old students in science and mathematics literacy in an international context (NCES 2008–016). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Washington, DC.
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.
Boyd, S.E., Banilower, E.R., Pasley, J.D., & Weiss, I.R. (2003). Progress and pitfalls: A cross-site look at local systemic change through teacher enhancement. Chapel Hill, NC: Horizon Research.
Chi, M.T.H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 161–199.
Chinn, C.A. & Malhotra, B.A. (2002). Children's responses to anomalous scientific data: How is conceptual change impeded? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 327–343.
Choe, K., Keil, F.C., & Bloom, P. (2005). Children's understanding of the Ulysses conflict. Developmental Science, 8, 387–392.
Dawson, M.R.W. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Jong, T. & Joolingen, W.R. (1998). Scientific discovery learning with computer simulations of conceptual domains. Review of Educational Research, 68, 179–201.
Dean, D. & Kuhn, D. (2007). Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Science Education, 91, 384–397.
Dunbar, K. & Fugelsang, J. (2005). Scientific thinking and reasoning. In K. Holyoak & R.G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 706–725). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dunbar, K. & Klahr, D. (1989). Developmental differences in scientific discovery strategies. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 109–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ericsson, K.A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P.J., & Hoffman, R.R. (Eds.). (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Flores, F., Tovar, M., & Gallegos, L. (2003). Representation of the cell and its processes in high school students: An integrated view. International Journal of Science Education, 25, 269–286.
Garcia-Mila, M. & Andersen, C. (2007). Developmental change in note taking during scientific inquiry. International Journal of Science Education, 29, 1035–1058.
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.
Gonzales, P., Williams, T., Jocelyn, L., Roey, S., Kastberg, D., & Brenwald, S. (2008). Highlights from TIMSS 2007:Mathematics and science achievement of U.S. fourth- and eighth-grade students in an international context (NCES 2009–001 Revised). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Washington, DC.
Gorman, M.E., Tweney, R.D., Gooding, D.C., & Kincannon, A.P. (Eds.). (2005). Scientific and technological thinking. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grasshoff, G. & May, M. (1995). From historical case studies to systematic methods of discovery. Working notes: AAAI spring symposium on systematic methods of scientific discovery (pp. 46–57). Stanford, CA: AAAI.
Grigg, W., Lauko, M., & Brockway, D. (2006). The nation's report card: Science 2005 (NCES 2006–466). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC:U.S. Government Printing Office.
Hanushek, E.A. (2005). The economics of school quality. German Economic Review, 6, 269–286.
Kanari, Z. & Millar, R. (2004). Reasoning from data: How students collect and interpret data in science investigations. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41, 748–769.
Kim, J.J., Crasco, L.M., Smith, R.B., Johnson, G., Karantonis, A., & Leavitt, D.J. (2001). Academic excellence for all urban students: Their accomplishments in science and mathematics. Norwood, MA: Systemic Research.
Klahr, D. (2000). Exploring science: The cognition and development of discovery processes. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Klahr, D. & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual search space during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 12, 1–48.
Klahr, D., Fay, A.L., & Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 111–146.
Klahr, D. & Simon, H.A. (1999). Studies of scientific discovery: Complementary approaches and convergent findings. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 524–543.
Klahr, D. & Simon, H.A. (2001). What have psychologists (and others) discovered about the process of scientific discovery?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 75–79.
Klahr, D., Triona, L.M., & Williams, C. (2007). Hands on what? The relative effectiveness of physical vs. virtual materials in an engineering design project by middle school students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44, 183–203.
Koerber, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Nett, U. (2005). Scientific reasoning in young children: Preschoolers' ability to evaluate covariation evidence. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 64, 141–152.
Koslowski, B. & Masnick, A. (2002). The development of causal reasoning. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 257–281). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.
Kuhn, D. (2001). How do people know?Psychological Science, 12, 1–8.
Kuhn, D. (2002). What is scientific thinking and how does it develop? In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 371–393). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Kuhn, D. (2005). Education for thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O'Loughlin, M. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Kuhn, D., Black, J., Keselman, A., & Kaplan, D. (2000). The development of cognitive skills to support inquiry learning. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 495–523.
Kuhn, D. & Dean, D. (2004). Connecting scientific reasoning and causal inference. Journal of Cognition & Development, 5, 261–288.
Kuhn, D. & Dean, D. (2005). Is developing scientific thinking all about learning to control variables?Psychological Science, 16, 866–870.
Kuhn, D. & Franklin, S. (2006). The second decade: What develops (and how). In W. Damon, R.M. Lerner, (Series Eds.), D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception and language (6th ed.) (pp. 953–993). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60, 1–128.
Kuhn, D., Iordanou, K., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2008). Beyond control of variables: What needs to develop to achieve skilled scientific thinking?Cognitive Development, 23, 435–451.
Kuhn, D., Katz, J.B., DeanJr., D. (2004). Developing reason. Thinking & Reasoning, 10, 197–219.
Kuhn, D. & Phelps, E. (1982). The development of problem-solving strategies. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 17, 1– 44.
Kuhn, D., Schauble, L., & Garcia-Mila, M. (1992). Cross-domain development of scientific reasoning. Cognition & Instruction, 9, 285–327.
Kulkarni, D. & Simon, H.A. (1988). The process of scientific discovery: The strategy of experimentation. Cognitive Science, 12, 139–176.
Langley, P., Simon, H.A., Bradshaw, G.L., & Zytkow, J.M. (1987). Scientific discovery: Computational explorations of the creative processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lehrer, R. & Schauble, L. (1998). Reasoning about structure and function: Children's conceptions of gears. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31, 3–25.
Leighton, J. & Bisanz, G. L. (2003). Children's and adults' knowledge and reasoning about the ozone layer and its depletion. International Journal of Science Education, 25, 117–139.
Leighton, J. P., Gokiert, R.J., & Cui, Y. (2007). Using exploratory and confirmatory methods to identify the cognitive dimensions in a large-scale science assessment. International Journal of Testing, 7, 1–49.
Masnick, A.M. & Klahr, D. (2003). Error matters: An initial exploration of elementary school children's understanding of experimental error. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 67–98.
Masnick, A.M. & Morris, B.J. (2002). Reasoning from data: The effect of sample size and variability on children's and adults' conclusions. In Proceedings of the 24th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 643–648).
Minsky, M. (June, 1974). A framework for representing knowledge. MIT-AI Laboratory Memo 306. Accessed on January 2, 2010, at http://web.media.mit.edu/~minsky/papers/Frames/frames.html.
,National Commission on Excellence in Education. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative for educational reform. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
,National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. National Committee on Science Education Standards and Assessment. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
,National Research Council. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation.
Newcombe, N.S., Ambady, N., Eccles, J., Gomez, L., Klahr, D., Linn, M., Miller, K., & Mix, K. (2009). Psychology's role in mathematics and science education. American Psychologist, 64, 538–550.
Newell, A. & Simon, H.A. (1972). Human problem solving. Oxford, UK: Prentice-Hall.
No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub Law No. 107–110 (2002, January). Retrieved April 11, 2009 from http://www.ed.gov/policy/elsec/leg/esea02/107–110.pdf
,Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2006). PISA 2006: Science competencies for tomorrow's world: Volume 1: Analysis. Author.
,Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2009). Education today: The OECD perspective. Author.
Perkins, D. (1995). Outsmarting IQ: The emerging science of learnable intelligence. New York City: The Free Press.
Reid, D.J., Zhang, J., & Chen, Q. (2003). Supporting scientific discovery learning in a simulation environment. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 19, 9–20.
Resnick, L.B. (1987). Education and learning to think. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32, 102–119.
Siegler, R. (1998). Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.
Siegler, R. (2006). Microgenetic studies of learning. In D. Kuhn and R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Shtulman, A. (2006). Qualitative differences between naïve and scientific theories of evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 170–194.
Simon, H.A. (1977). Models of discovery. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reidel.
Simon, H.A. & Lea, G. (1974). Problem solving and rule induction: A unified view. In L.W. Gregg (Ed.), Knowledge and cognition (pp. 105–127). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Snow, R.E., Corno, L., & Jackson, D. (1996). Individual differences in affective and conative functions. In D.C. Berliner & R.C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology, (pp. 243–310). London, England: Prentice Hall.
Stanovich, K.E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Stathopoulou, C. & Vosniadou, S. (2007). Exploring the relationship between physics-related epistemological beliefs and physics understanding. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 255–281.
Tschirgi, J.E. (1980). Sensible reasoning: A hypothesis about hypotheses. Child Development, 51, 1–10.
Joolingen, W.R. & de Jong, T. (1997) An extended dual search space model of scientific discovery learning. Instructional Science, 25, 307–346.
Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.
Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning skills. Developmental Review, 20, 99–149.
Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Development Review, 27, 172–223.
Zimmerman, C. & Glaser, R. (2001). Testing positive versus negative claims: A preliminary investigation of the role of cover story in the assessment of experimental design skills (Tech. Rep. No. 554). Los Angeles, CA: UCLA National Center for Research on Evaluation, Standards, and Student Testing (CRESST).