Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T12:41:58.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Where do most spontaneous representational gestures actually occur with respect to speech?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 January 2010

Shuichi Nobe
Affiliation:
Department of English, Aoyama Gakuin University, Tokyo
David McNeill
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Speech-accompanying spontaneous gestures have attracted the attention of psycholinguists and cognitive psychologists in recent years. Theorists assert that spontaneous gestures, along with the speech they accompany, are a window into the nature of speakers' mental representations and processes (Goldin-Meadow et al. 1993; Kendon 1986; McNeill 1985, 1987, 1992). Researchers have shown that speech-accompanying gestures are inextricably intertwined temporally and semantically with speech (Kendon 1980; McNeill 1992). Various models of production of speech and representational gestures (i.e., iconics, metaphorics, and abstract deictics) have been presented (Beattie & Aboudan 1994; Butterworth & Hadar 1989; Kita 1993; Krauss et al. 1996; McNeill 1989, 1992; Nobe 1993, 1996).

There have recently been debates on the credibility of each model (see Hadar & Butterworth 1997; Hadar & Yadlin-Gedassy 1994; Hadar et al. 1998; and other chapters in this book). Unfortunately, the current chapter does not have any space (and thus it is not the writer's intention) to evaluate all models mentioned above. It is, however, a fact that many agree that the temporal relationship of representational gestures and speech is very significant in constructing gesture- and speech-production models. Moreover, the ‘tendency’ of gestures to occur with respect to their accompanied speech does matter. This chapter reports on how the timing between gestures and speech is measured, and how this gesture tendency is calculated and presented as evidence for the construction of speech and gesture models.

Comprehensive models of speech and gesture should account for as many gestures as possible. If the number of gestures which are accounted for by a model is rather small, we should not regard it as a general theory (Nobe 1996).

Type
Chapter
Information
Language and Gesture , pp. 186 - 198
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×