Book contents
- Frontmatter
- Dedication
- Epigraph
- Contents
- List of figures, tables and boxes
- Acknowledgments
- Part I Human autonomy
- 1 Twins and autonomy
- 2 Justice and freedom
- 3 The great debate
- 4 Slow and quick decay of family effects
- 5 Reconciliation with twins and adoptions
- 6 The fairness factor
- Part II Intelligence
- For scholars who wish to use the Age-Table Method to measure family effects in nations other than the USA
- Appendix I Wechsler Vocabulary and description of method of analysis
- Appendix II Stanford-Binet Vocabulary
- Appendix III Raven's Progressive Matrices
- References
- Name index
- Subject index
2 - Justice and freedom
from Part I - Human autonomy
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 June 2016
- Frontmatter
- Dedication
- Epigraph
- Contents
- List of figures, tables and boxes
- Acknowledgments
- Part I Human autonomy
- 1 Twins and autonomy
- 2 Justice and freedom
- 3 The great debate
- 4 Slow and quick decay of family effects
- 5 Reconciliation with twins and adoptions
- 6 The fairness factor
- Part II Intelligence
- For scholars who wish to use the Age-Table Method to measure family effects in nations other than the USA
- Appendix I Wechsler Vocabulary and description of method of analysis
- Appendix II Stanford-Binet Vocabulary
- Appendix III Raven's Progressive Matrices
- References
- Name index
- Subject index
Summary
Questions
(1) When 17-year-olds take the SAT, do some homes enhance cognitive performance at this age more than others?
(2) After family effects are gone, can adults enhance their cognitive abilities?
At the age of 17, cognitive performance does much to determine the fate of American youth. That is the age at which they take the SAT and sort themselves out among various universities. I am going to ask you (for now) to accept two promissory notes: that we know how much family affects vocabulary at various levels of achievement; and that we have a rough estimate of the percentile gap between levels of achievement and the cognitive quality of the family typical at that level. For example, those at the 98th percentile of vocabulary come on average from homes just below the 70th percentile of cognitive quality. Assuming we have this knowledge, let us look at the consequences.
Vocabulary and family quality
When students sit the SAT, universities take scores on the SAT for reading (SAT-R) as the best measure of the viability of their students. Vocabulary is highly predictive of those scores. I will average Vocabulary results from all six of the data sets the leading IQ tests give us: Stanford-Binet tests from 1985 and 2001; Wechsler tests from 1950–55, 1975, 1992, and 2004–05 (these dates average the years when the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, the WISC and the WAIS, were actually normed). By combining their results, I hope to eliminate the vagaries of any particular set.
Table 1 shows that family has different effects at various levels of performance. For example: students whose vocabulary puts them at +2 SD above average, which is the 97.73 percentile (they are better than almost 98 percent of 17-year-olds), suffer from a typical disadvantage thanks to their families of about 1 IQ point; those at +1 SD (or at the 84th percentile – better than 84 percent of their peers) have a typical disadvantage of 3 IQ points; typical students at −1 SD (the 16th percentile) are advantaged by just under 3 points: while those way down at the −2 SD (the 2.27 percentile) are advantaged by over 7 IQ points.
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- Does your Family Make You Smarter?Nature, Nurture, and Human Autonomy, pp. 12 - 29Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 2016