Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T13:03:39.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

27 - The developmental environment: influences on subsequent cognitive function and behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Jason Landon
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Michael Davison
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Bernhard H. Breier
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Peter Gluckman
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Mark Hanson
Affiliation:
University of Southampton
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The concept of the developmental origins of adult disease was initially based on epidemiological observations relating evidence of a constrained fetal environment to a greater risk of metabolic and cardiovascular disorders in adult life. It was later recognised that those at greatest risk also have an increased propensity to developing obesity in childhood and during adult life (Barker and Osmond 1988, Hales and Barker 1992, Gluckman and Hanson 2004). The interest in the later effects of early environmental events has since been generalised to include possible prenatal effects on offspring behaviour and cognition. In this chapter, we review the existing human and animal literature on the early-life influences on offspring cognitive function and behaviour.

Human studies

Recently, interest has grown in the possibility of a relation between birthweight and subsequent cognitive function. Early research focused on comparisons between individuals of low birthweight (LBW) or small for gestational age (SGA) and those with normal birthweights (e.g., Ounsted et al. 1983, Pharoah et al. 1994, Strauss 2000). These studies showed that LBW and SGA individuals had an increased incidence of neurological deficits and/or poorer cognitive skills throughout childhood. Subsequent research has examined whether this relationship persists across the normal range of birthweights (e.g. Sorenson et al. 1997, Matte et al. 2001, Richards et al. 2001, 2002, Shenkin et al. 2001, Jefferis et al. 2002). The consensus from this research is that the relationship does hold across the range of normal birthweights, but that the effect is small.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J. and Galler, J. R. (1996a). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects avoidance but not escape behavior in the elevated T-maze test. Physiol. Behav., 60, 191–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J. and Galler, J. R. (1996b). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects exploratory behaviour of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol. Behav., 60, 675–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J. and Galler, J. R. (1996c). Prenatal protein malnutrition affects the social interactions of juvenile rats. Physiol. Behav., 60, 197–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagalkote, H., Pang, D. and Jones, P. B. (2001). Maternal influenza and schizophrenia. Int. J. Ment. Health, 29, 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, D. J. P. and Osmond, C. (1988). Low birthweight and hypertension. BMJ, 297, 134–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N. (1995). Psychiatric sequelae of low birthweight. Epidemiol. Rev., 17, 96–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslau, N., Klein, N. and Allen, L. (1988). Very low birthweight: behavioural sequelae at nine years of age. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 27, 605–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Begg, M. S., Gravenstein, S.et al. (2004). Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 61, 774–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coe, C. L., Kramer, M., Czéh, B.et al. (2003). Prenatal stress diminishes neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of juvenile rhesus monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry, 54, 1025–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, M. and Baum, W. M. (2000). Choice in a variable environment: Every reinforcer counts. J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 74, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davison, M. and McCarthy, D. (1988). The Matching Law: a Research Review. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dwyer, C. M., Lawrence, A. B., Bishop, S. C. and Lewis, M. (2003). Ewe–lamb bonding behaviours are affected by maternal undernutrition in pregnancy. J. Nutr., 89, 123–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D. and Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 1155–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gale, C. R. and Martyn, C. N. (2004). Birthweight and later risk of depression in a national birth cohort. Br. J. Psychiatry, 184, 28–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gale, C. R., O'Callaghan, F. J., Godfrey, K. M., Law, C. M. and Martyn, C. N. (2004). Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in children. Brain, 127, 321–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P. D. and Hanson, M. A. (2004). Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease. Science, 305, 1733–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. and McNaughton, N. (1983). Comparison between the behavioural effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 7, 119–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hales, C. N. and Barker, D. J. P. (1992). Type 2 (non insulin dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia, 35, 595–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayashi, A., Nagaoka, M., Yamada, K., Ichitani, Y., Miake, Y. and Okado, N. (1998). Maternal stress induces synaptic loss and developmental disabilities of offspring. Int. J. Dev. Neurosc., 16, 209–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoek, H. W., Brown, A. S. and Susser, E. (1998). The Dutch famine and schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol., 33, 373–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hursh, S. R. (1984). Behavioral economics. J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 42, 435–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jefferis, B. J. M. H., Power, C. and Hertzman, C. (2002). Birthweight, childhood socioeconomic environment, and cognitive development in the 1958 British birth cohort study. BMJ, 325, 305–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, R. S., Debassion, W. A., Kemper, T. L.et al. (2004). Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition and acute postnatal stress on granule cell genesis in the fascia dentate of neonatal and juvenile rats. Dev. Brain Res., 150, 9–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landon, J. and Davison, M. (2001). Reinforcer-ratio variation and its effect on rate of adaptation. J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 75, 207–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martyn, C. N., Gale, C. R., Sayer, A. A. and Fall, C. (1996). Growth in utero and cognitive function in adult life: follow up study of people born between 1920 and 1943. BMJ, 312, 1393–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matte, T. D., Bresnahan, M., Begge, M. D. and Susser, E. (2001). Influence of variation in birthweight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. BMJ, 323, 310–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 24, 1161–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A., Machon, R. A., Huttenen, M. O. and Bonett, D. (1988). Adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to an influenza epidemic. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 45, 189–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menard, J. L., Champagne, D. L., Meaney, M. J. P. (2004). Variations of maternal care differentially influence ‘fear’ reactivity and regional patterns of cFOS immunoreactivity in response to the shock-probe burying test. Neuroscience, 129, 297–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirescu, C., Peters, J. D. and Gould, E. (2004). Early life experience alters response of adult neurogenesis to stress. Nat. Neurosci., 7, 841–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokler, D. J., Galler, J. R. and Morgane, P. J. (2003). Modulation of 5-HT release in the hippocampus of 30-day-old rats exposed in-utero to protein malnutrition. Dev. Brain Res., 142, 203–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgane, P. J., Mokler, D. J. and Galler, J. R. (2002). Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition on hippocampal formation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 26, 471–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neugebauer, R., Hoek, H. W. and Susser, E. (1999). Prenatal exposure to wartime famine and development of antisocial personality disorder in early adulthood. JAMA, 282, 455–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ounsted, M. K., Moar, V. A. and Scott, A. (1983). Small-for-dates babies at the age of four years: health, handicap, and developmental status. Early Hum. Dev., 8, 243–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E. and Gray, R. D., eds. (2001). Cycles of Contingency: Developmental systems and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pharoah, P. O., Stevenson, C. J., Cooke, R. W. and Stevenson, R. C. (1994). Clinical and sub-clinical deficits at 8 years in a geographically defined cohort of low birthweight infants. Arch. Dis. Child., 70, 264–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, M., Hardy, R., Kuh, D. and Wadsworth, M. E. J. (2001). Birthweight, and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort: longitudinal population based study. BMJ, 322, 199–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, M., Hardy, R., Kuh, D. and Wadsworth, M. E. J. (2002). Birthweight, postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol., 31, 342–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenkin, S.D, Starr, J. M., Rush, M. A., Whalley, L. J. and Deary, I. J. (2001). birthweight and cognitive function at age 11 years: the Scottish Mental Survey 1932. Arch. Dis. Child., 85, 189–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, L., Fatemi, S. H., Sidwell, R. W. and Patterson, P. H. (2003). Maternal influenza causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J. Neurosci., 23, 297–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorenson, H. T., Sabroe, S., Olsen, J., Rothman, K. J., Gillman, M. W. and Fischer, P. (1997). Birthweight and cognitive function in young adult life: historical cohort study. BMJ, 315, 401–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, R. S. (2000). Adult functional outcome of those born small for gestation age: twenty-six-year follow-up of the 1970 British birth cohort. JAMA, 283, 625–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strupp, B. J. and Levitsky, D. A. (1995). Enduring cognitive effects of early malnutrition: a theoretical reappraisal. J. Nutr., 125, 2221–32S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szuran, T., Zimmerman, E. and Welzl, H. (1994).Water maze performance and hippocampal weight of prenatally stressed rats. Behav. Brain Res., 65, 153–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, C., Syddall, H., Rosin, I., Osmond, C. and Barker, D. J. P. (2001). Birthweight and the risk of depressive disorder in later life. Br. J. Psychiatry, 179, 450–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonkiss, J. and Galler, J. R. (1990). Prenatal protein malnutrition and working memory performance in adult rats. Behav. Brain Res., 40, 95–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonkiss, J., Galler, J. R., Formica, R. N., Shukitt-Hale, B. and Timm, R. R. (1990). Fetal protein malnutrition impairs acquisition of a DRL task in adult rats. Physiol. Behav., 48, 73–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonkiss, J., Shultz, P. and Galler, J. R. (1994). An analysis of spatial navigation in prenatally protein malnourished rats. Physiol. Behav., 55, 217–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonkiss, J., Harrison, R. H. and Galler, J. R. (1996). Differential effects of prenatal protein malnutrition and prenatal cocaine on a test of homing behavior in rat pups. Physiol. Behav., 60, 1013–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonkiss, J., Shultz, P. L., Shumsky, J. S. and Galler, J. R. (1997). Development of spatial navigation following prenatal cocaine and malnutrition in rats: lack of additive effects. Neurotoxicol. Teratol., 19, 363–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vickers, M. H., Breier, B. H., Cutfield, W. S., Hofman, P. L. and Gluckman, P. D. (2000). Fetal origins of hyperphagia, obesity, and hypertension and postnatal amplification by hypercaloric nutrition. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab., 279, E83–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vickers, M. H., Reddy, S., Ikenasio, B. A. and Breier, B. H. (2001). Dysregulation of the adipoinsular axis: a mechanism for the pathogenesis of hyperleptinemia and adipogenic diabetes induced by fetal programming. J. Endocrinol., 170, 323–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vickers, M. H., Breier, B. H., McCarthy, D. and Gluckman, P. D. (2003). Sedentary behavior during postnatal life is determined by the prenatal environment and exacerbated by postnatal hypercaloric nutrition. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol., 285, R271–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wahlbeck, K., Forsen, T., Osmond, C., Barker, D. J. P. and Eriksson, J. G. (2001). Association of schizophrenia with low maternal body mass index, small size at birth, and thinness during childhood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 58, 48–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A.et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci., 7, 847–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weller, A., Glaubman, H., Yehuda, S., Caspy, T. and Ben-Uria, Y. (1988). Acute and repeated gestational stress affect offspring learning and activity in rats. Physiol. Behav., 43, 139–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodall, S. M., Breier, B. H., Johnston, B. M. and Gluckman, P. D. (1996a). A model of intrauterine growth retardation caused by chronic maternal undernutrition in the rat: effects on the somatotrophic axis and postnatal growth. J. Endocrinol., 150, 231–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodall, S. M., Johnston, B. M., Breier, B. H. and Gluckman, P. D. (1996b). Chronic maternal undernutrition in the rat leads to delayed postnatal growth and elevated blood pressure in offspring. Pediatr. Res., 40, 438–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×