Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-w7rtg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-23T15:35:09.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Creativity and Constraint: Friends, Not Foes

from SECTION TWO - CREATIVITY AND REASON IN COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2016

Catrinel Haught-Tromp
Affiliation:
Rider University
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
John Baer
Affiliation:
Rider University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

The complex and sometimes elusive concept of creativity has inspired many creative approaches to its study. Despite competing accounts and progressively more methodical empirical scrutiny, creativity still seems surrounded by a fuzzy aura of ambiguity. Its very definition remains open to debate, and many other related issues risk to be buried under a similar state of inconclusiveness: Is creativity domain-specific or not (Ambrose, 2009; Baer, 1998, 2011, 2012; Silva, Kaufman, & Pertz, 2009)? Does it refer to a process, a set of aptitudes, or the resulting product? How is creativity assessed (e.g., Kaufman & Baer, 2012)? Is creativity restricted to only a few geniuses, or does creative potential exist in everyone? Does gender (e.g., Baer, 1997) or mental illness 2001 play a role (e.g., Kaufman, 2001)? Is creativity important (Kaufman, Davis, & Beghetto, 2012; Runco & Abdullah, 2014; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995)? If so, how does one best study it (Ambrose, 2006; Ambrose, Sriraman, & Pierce, 2014)? From a neuroimaging perspective (see Arden, Chavez, Grazioplene, & Jung, 2010 for an overview), how does creative thought develop in the brain, and where?

One of the less debated claims is that creativity can and does manifest itself in virtually all areas of human life. Creative instances range from a moving piano sonata to an original fashion show, from a major scientific breakthrough to witty dialogue in a novel, from an innovative business initiative to an inspired soccer game. Indeed, the adjective creative can precede a vast number of nouns: a creative experiment, creative architecture, a creative poem, creative landscaping.

Although creativity is often extolled in domains such as music, painting, and literature, few of us would readily associate it with more mundane tasks, such as making dinner or writing a note for someone's birthday. Why are we generally reluctant to apply the label creative to the production of an original meal or a sentence? The reason is a widespread intuition that “true” creativity must bring about, via its outcome, some major change or important contribution in an acknowledged field. For example, Sternberg, Kaufman, and Pretz (2002) put forth a Propulsion Theory of Creative Contributions to describe the processes by which a creative act can impact an entire field. Boden (1990) discussed this difference between two types of creativity: psychological (P-creativity) and historic (H-creativity).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambrose, D. (2006). Large-scale contextual influences on creativity: Evolving academic disciplines and global value systems. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 75–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, D. (2009). Expanding visions of creative intelligence: An interdisciplinary exploration. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.Google Scholar
Ambrose, D., Sriraman, B., & Pierce, K., Eds. (2014). A critique of creativity and complexity: Deconstructing clichés. Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., & Jung, R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: A psychometric view. Behavioral Brain Research, doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.015.CrossRef
Baer, J. (1997). Gender differences in the effects of anticipated evaluation on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 25–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. (1998). The case for domain specificity in creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 173–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. (2011). Why grand theories of creativity distort, distract, and disappoint. International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving, 21, 73–100.Google Scholar
Baer, J. (2012). Domain specificity and the limits of creativity theory. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 16–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudelaire, C. (1981). Baudelaire: Selected writings on art and artists. Translated by P.E. Chavret. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150–171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boden, M. A. (1990). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. London: George Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Braus, I. (2006). Classical cooks: A gastrohistory of Western music. London: Xlibris, Corp.Google Scholar
Bringsjord, S., & Ferrucci, D. A. (2000). Artificial intelligence and literary creativity: Inside the mind of BRUTUS, a storytelling machine. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chaplin, C. (1964). My autobiography. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Eisenman, R. (1999). Creative prisoners: Do they exist?Creativity Research Journal, 12, 205–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1993). Seven creators of the modern era. In Brockman, J. (Ed.), Creativity (pp. 28–47). New York: Simon and Schuster.Google ScholarPubMed
Glucksberg, S., & Haught, C. (2006). Can Florida become like the next Florida? When metaphoric comparisons fail. Psychological Science, 17, 935–938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haught, C. (2013). A tale of two tropes: How metaphor and simile differ. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 254–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haught, C. (2014). Spain is not Greece: How metaphors are understood. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 351–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haught, C. (2015). The role of constraints in creative sentence production. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 160–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987a). The mental representation of the meaning of words. Cognition, 25, 189–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987b). Reasoning, imagining, and creating. Bulleting of The British Psychological Society, 40, 121–129.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993). Human and machine thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2002). How jazz musicians improvise. Music Perception, 19, 415–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2001). Genius, lunatics, and poets: Mental illness in prize-winning authors. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 20, 305–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2012). Beyond new and appropriate: Who decides what is creative?Creativity Research Journal, 24, 83–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The Four C model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Davis, C. D., & Beghetto, R. A. (2012). Why creativity should matter, why it doesn't, and what we can do. In Ambrose, D. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Dogmatism and high ability: The erosion and warping of creative intelligence (pp. 145–156). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1987). Conceptual development and category structure. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 175–200). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. A. (2006, February 12). Creativity Loves Constraints. Bloomberg Business.
McDonald, J. L., Bock, K., & Kelly, M. H. (1993). Word and world order: Semantic, phonological, and metrical determinants of serial position. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 188–230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPhee, J. (2013). Draft No. 4: Replacing the words in boxes. New Yorker, April 29.
Orwell, G. (1968). Politics and the English language. In Orwell, S. & Angus, I. (Eds.), The collected essays, journalism and letters of George Orwell, Vol. 4 (pp. 127–140). New York: Harcourt Brace and World.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Abdullah, A. M. (2014). Why isn't creativity being supported? Distressing analyses of grants and awards for creativity research – or lack thereof. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 248–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Sakamoto, S. O. (1999). Experimental studies of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 62–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Silva, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2009). Is creativity domain-specific? Latent class models of creative accomplishments and creative self- descriptions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creative, and the Arts, 3, 139–148.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1990). Psychology, science, and history: An introduction to historiometry. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2013). Creative genius in science. In Feist, G. J. & Gorman, M. E. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of science (pp. 251–272). New Yor: Springer Publishing.Google Scholar
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.Google ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2002). The creativity conundrum. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Stokes, P. D. (2001). Variability, constraints, and creativity: Shedding light on Claude Monet. American Psychologist, 56, 355–359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stokes, P. D. (2005). Creativity from constraints: The psychology of breakthrough. New York: Springer Publishing.Google Scholar
Stokes, P. D. (2007). Using constraints to generate and sustain novelty. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 107–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stravinsky, I. (1956). The poetics of music. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W. (1986). Creativity: Genius and other myths. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×