Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Print publication year: 2008
  • Online publication date: June 2012

Part I - Backdrop

  • Edited by Philip Robbins, Washington University, St Louis, Murat Aydede, University of Florida
  • Publisher: Cambridge University Press
  • pp 1-52

References

Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2008). The bounds of cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Anderson, M. L. (2003). Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence, 149, 91–130.
Ashby, W. R. (1956). Introduction to cybernetics. New York: Wiley.
Beer, R. D. (1995). A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment interaction. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 173–215.
Block, N. (2005). Review of Alva Noë's Action in Perception. Journal of Philosophy, 102, 259–272.
Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–159.
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Bernston, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determination of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.
Chen, S., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavior dispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.
Clancey, W. J. (1997). Situated cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, A. (1989). Microcognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being there. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 10–23.
Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 558–565.
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–346.
Hurley, S. L. (1998). Consciousness in action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 7, 31–68.
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–549.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.
Markman, A. B., & Dietrich, E. (2000). Extending the classical view of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 470–475.
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 82, 435–450.
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 461–488.
Robbins, P. (2008). Consciousness and the social mind. Cognitive Systems Research, 9, 15–23.
Rupert, R. (2004). Challenging the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 101, 389–428.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
van Gelder, T. (1995). What might cognition be, if not computation? Journal of Philosophy, 91, 345–381.
Vera, A. H., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Situated action: A symbolic interpretation. Cognitive Science, 17, 7–48.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics; or, Control and communication in the animal and the machine. New York: Wiley.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636.
Wilson, R. A. (2004). Boundaries of the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Young, J. Z. (1964). A model of the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

References

Agrawala, A. K., & Noh, S. H. (1992). Computer systems. In S. C. Shapiro (Ed.), Encyclopedia of artificial intelligence (pp. 241–248). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Agre, P. E. (1997). Computation and human experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Altman, I., & Rogoff, B. (1987). World views in psychology: Trait, interactional, organismic, and transactional perspectives. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 7–40). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ashby, W. R. (1956). An introduction to cybernetics. London: Chapman & Hall.
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bamberger, J. (1991). The mind behind the musical ear. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bamberger, J., & Schön, D. A. (1983). Learning as reflective conversation with materials: Notes from work in progress. Art Education, 36, 68–73.
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Bartlett, F. C. (1977). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1932)
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine Books.
Bateson, G. (1988). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. New York: Bantam Books.
Bateson, G. (1991). Sacred unity. New York: Cornelia & Michael Bessie.
Berger, P., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. New York: Anchor Books.
Berkeley, G. (1963). A treatise concerning the principles of human knowledge. La Salle, IL: Open Court. (Originally published 1710)
Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism: Perspective and method. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bransford, J. D., McCarrell, N. S., Franks, J. J., & Nitsch, K. E. (1977). Toward unexplaining memory. In R. E. Shaw & J. D. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 431–466). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bredo, E. (1994). Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyan pragmatism. Educational Psychologist, 29(1), 23–35.
Brooks, R. A. (1991). How to build complete creatures rather than isolated cognitive simulators. In K. VanLehn (Ed.), Architectures for intelligence: The 22nd Carnegie Mellon Symposium on Cognition (pp. 225–240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1), 32–42.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Capra, F. (1996). The web of life. New York: Anchor Books.
Carroll, S. B. (2005). Endless forms most beautiful: The new science of Evo Devo and the making of the animal kingdom. New York: W. W. Norton.
Center for the Study of Complex Systems. (n.d.). Retrieved June 7, 2006, from http://www.cscs.umich.edu/
Clancey, W. J. (1992). Representations of knowing: In defense of cognitive apprenticeship. Journal of AI Education, 3(2), 139–168.
Clancey, W. J. (1993). Situated action: A neuropsychological interpretation (Response to Vera and Simon). Cognitive Science, 17(1), 87–116.
Clancey, W. J. (1997). Situated cognition: On human knowledge and computer representations. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Clancey, W. J. (1999). Conceptual coordination: How the mind orders experience in time. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Clancey, W. J. (2002). Simulating activities: Relating motives, deliberation, and attentive coordination. Cognitive Systems Research, 3(3), 471–499.
Clancey, W. J. (2005). Folk theory of the so-cial mind: Policies, principles, and foundational metaphors. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Retrieved May 14, 2008, from http://www.cogsci.rpi.edu/CSJarchive/Proceedings/2005/docs/p465.pdf
Clancey, W. J. (2006). Observation of work practices in natural settings. In A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, & R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook on expertise and expert performance (pp. 127–145). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Clancey, W. J. (in press). A transactional perspective on the practice-based science of teaching and learning. In T. Koschmann (Ed.), Theorizing learning practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Clancey, W. J., Sachs, P., Sierhuis, M., & van Hoof, R. (1998). Brahms: Simulating practice for work systems design. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 49, 831–865.
Cole, M. (1996). Culture in mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cole, M., & Wertsch, J. V. (1996). Beyond the individual-social antimony in discussions of Piaget and Vygotsky. Human Development, 39, 250–256.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 453–494). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cronk, G. (2005). Mead. The Internet encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://www.iep.utm.edu/m/mead.htm
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3(4), 357–370.
Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education: An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: Macmillan.
Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Minton, Balch.
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Henry Holt.
Dewey, J. (1981). The child and the curriculum. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1902)
Dewey, J. (1989). Experience, knowledge and value: A rejoinder. In P. A. Schilpp, & L. E. Hahn (Eds.), Philosophy of John Dewey (3rd ed.). La Salle, IL: Open Court. (Original work published 1939)
Dewey, J., & Bentley, A. F. (1949). Knowing and the known. Boston: Beacon Press.
Durkheim, E. (1947). The elementary forms of religious experience. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published 1912)
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.
Freeman, W. J. (1991). The physiology of perception. Scientific American, 264(2), 78–87.
Gallagher, R., & Appenzeller, T. (1999, April 2). Complex systems [Special issue]. Science, 79.
Gardner, H. (1985). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
Gell-Mann, M. (1995). The quark and the jaguar: Adventures in the simple and complex. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. New York: Viking.
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 1–55.
Godwin, J. (1988). Australia on $25 a day. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday/Anchor.
Gould, J. L. (1986). The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science, 232(4752), 861–863.
Gould, S. J. (1987). An urchin in the storm. New York: W. W. Norton.
Greenbaum, J., & Kyng, M. (Eds.). (1991). Design at work: Cooperative design of computer systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Greeno, J. G. (1997). On claims that answer the wrong questions. Educational Researcher, 26, 5–17.
Greeno, J. G. (2006). Learning in activity. In R. Keith Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 79–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Griffin, D. R. (1992). Animal minds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hall, E. T. (1973). Silent language. New York: Anchor Press. (Original work published 1959)
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–346.
Hayes-Roth, F., Lenat, D. B., & Waterman, D. A. (1983). Building expert systems. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Head, H. (1920). Studies in neurology. London: Oxford University Press.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
Hewitt, C. (1977). Viewing control structures as patterns of passing messages. Artificial Intelligence, 8(3), 323–364.
Hewitt, C. (1985). The challenge of open systems. Byte Magazine, 10(4), 223–244.
Hoffman, R., & Nead, J. (1983). General contextualism, ecological science and cognitive research. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 4(4), 507–560.
Holland, J.H. (1995). Hidden order: How adaptation builds complexity. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Jaworski, J., & Flowers, B. (1996). Synchronicity: The inner path of leadership. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
Jenkins, J. J. (1974). Remember that old theory of memory? Well, forget it! American Psychologist, 29(11), 785–795.
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In R. Church & B. Campbell (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 279–296). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kauffman, S. A. (1993). The origins of order: Self-organization and selection in evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
Konvitz, M. R., & Kennedy, G. (Eds.). (1960). The American pragmatists. New York: World.
Korzybski, A. (1994). Science and sanity: An introduction to non-Aristotelian systems and general semantics (5th ed.). Englewood, NJ: Institute of General Semantics. (Original work published 1934)
Koschmann, T. (Ed.). (In press). Theorizing learning practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Langer, S. (1958). Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism of reason, rite, and art. New York: Mentor Books. (Original work published 1942)
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley.
Latour, B. (1999). Pandora's hope: Essays on the reality of science studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Leiter, K. (1980). A primer on ethnomethodology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lenat, D. B., & Guha, R. V. (1990). Building large knowledge-based systems. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Leont’ev, A. N. (1979). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 37–71). Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
Loftus, E. (1996). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1979)
Lorenz, K. (1973). Autobiography. Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://nobelprize.org/medicine/laureates/1973/lorenz-autobio.html
Luria, A. R. (1928). The problem of the cultural development of the child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35, 493–506.
Luria, A. R. (1979). The making of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Maturana, H. (1975). The organization of the living: A theory of the living organization. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 7, 313–332.
Maturana, H. (1978). Biology of language: The epistemology of reality. In G. A. Miller & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Psychology and biology of language and thought: Essays in honor of Eric Lenneberg (pp. 27–64). New York: Academic Press.
Maturana, H. R. (1983). What is it to see? Qué es ver? Archivos de Biología y Medicina Experimentales, 16, 255–269.
Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1980). Auto-poiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Maturana, H., & Varela, F. J. (1987). The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. Boston: New Science Library.
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart D. E., & PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Minsky, M. (1985). The society of mind. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Minsky, M. (1998). “The mind, artificial intelligence and emotions: Interview with Marvin Minsky,” conducted by Renato M. E. Sabbatini (Associate Editor, Brain & Mind Magazine). Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://www.cerebromente.org.br/n07/opiniao/minsky/minsky_i.htm
Minsky, M., & Papert, S. A. (1969). Perceptrons: An introduction to computational geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Morowitz, H. J. (2002). The emergence of everything: How the world became complex. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
New England Complex Systems Institute. (n.d.). Home page. Retrieved June 7, 2006, from http://www.necsi.org/
Papert, S., & Harel, I. (1991). Constructionism. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1970). Structuralism. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. (1971). Genetic epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton. (Original work published 1970)
Plucker, J. A. (Ed.). (2003). Human intelligence: Historical influences, controversies, teaching resources. Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://www.indiana.edu/~intell/titchener.shtml
Polyani, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. New York: Doubleday/Anchor Books.
Prigogine, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New York: Bantam Books.
Radford, G. P. (1994). Overcoming Dewey's “false psychology”: Reclaiming communication for communication studies. Presented at the 80th Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association, New Orleans, November 19–22. Retrieved August 5, 2005, from http://alpha.fdu.edu/∼gradford/dewey.html
Resnick, M. (1997). Turtles, termites, and traffic jams: Explorations in massively parallel microworlds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Riegler, A. (2001). Towards a radical constructivist understanding of science. Foundations of Science, 6(1–3), 1–30.
Rogoff, B., & Lave, J. (Eds.). (1984). Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.). (1984). Animal cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Roschelle, J., & Clancey, W. J. (1992). Learning as social and neural. Educational Psychologist, 27(4), 435–453.
Rosenfield, I. (1988). The invention of memory: A new view of the brain. New York: Basic Books.
Rosenfield, I. (2000). Freud's megalomania. New York: W. W. Norton.
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes & Noble.
Sacks, O. (1987). The man who mistook his wife for a hat. New York: Harper & Row.
Sacks, O. (1995). An anthropologist on Mars: Seven paradoxical tales. New York: Knopf.
Sandberg, J. A. C., & Wielinga, B. J. (1991). How situated is cognition? In Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Artificial Intelligence (pp. 341–346). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schank, R., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schön, D. A. (1979). Generative metaphor: A perspective on problem-setting in social polity. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 254–283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schön, D. A. (1987). Educating the reflective practitioner. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1973). Cognitive consequences of formal and informal education. Science, 182(4112), 553–559.
Senge, P. M. (1990). The fifth discipline: The art and practice of the learning organization. New York: Doubleday/Currency.
Shapiro, S. C. (Ed.). (1992). Encyclopedia of artificial intelligence (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley-Interscience.
Shaw, R. E., & Todd, J. (1980). Abstract machine theory and direct perception [Commentary on “Against Direct Perception” by Shimon Ullman]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 400–401.
Shibutani, T. (1966). Improvised news: A sociological study of rumor. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
Simon, H. A. (1973). The structure of ill-structured problems. Artificial Intelligence, 4(3), 181–202.
Slezak, P. (1989). Scientific discovery by computer as refutation of the strong programme. Social Studies of Science, 19(4), 563–600.
Smoliar, S. (1971). A parallel processing model of musical structures (Tech. Rep. No. AI TR-242). Cambridge, MA: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, MIT.
Steels, L. (1990). Cooperation between distributed agents through self-organisation. In Y. Demazeau & J. P. Muller (Eds.), Decentralized AI (pp. 175–196). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tinbergen, N. (1953). The herring gull's world. London: Collins.
Turvey, M. T., & Shaw, R. E. (1995). Toward an ecological physics and a physical psychology. In R. L. Solso & D. W. Massaro (Eds.), The science of the mind (pp. 144–172). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tyler, S. (1978). The said and the unsaid: Mind, meaning and culture. New York: Academic Press.
van Gelder, T. (1991). Connectionism and dynamical explanation. In Proceedings of the 13th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 499–503). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Vera, A., & Simon, H. (1993). Situated action: Reply to William Clancey. Cognitive Science, 17(1), 117–135.
Vico, G. (1858). De antiquissima Italorum sapientia. Translated by F. S. Pomodoro, Naples: Stamperia de’ Classici Latini. (Originally published 1710)
von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General system theory: Foundations, development, applications. New York: George Braziller.
von Foerster, H. (1970). Thoughts and notes on cognition. In P. L. Garvin (Ed.), Cognition: A multiple view (pp. 25–48). New York: Spartan Books.
von Foerster, H. (2003). Understanding understanding. New York: Springer-Verlag.
von Glasersfeld, E. (1974). Piaget and the radical constructivist epistemology. In C. D. Smock & E. von Glasersfeld (Eds.), Epistemology and education (pp. 1–24). Athens, GA: Follow Through Publications.
von Glasersfeld, E. (1984). An introduction to radical constructivism. In P. Watzlawick (Ed.), The invented reality (pp. 17–40). New York: W. W. Norton.
von Glasersfeld, E. (1989). Facts and the self from a constructivist point of view. Poetics, 18(4–5), 435–448.
von Neumann, J., & Burks, A. W. (1966). Theory of self-reproducing automata. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waldrop, M. M. (1992). Complexity: The emerging science at the edge of order and chaos. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wertsch, J. V. (Ed.). (1979). The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Or control and communication in the animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wilden, A. (1987). The rules are no game: The strategy of communication. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding computers and cognition: A new foundation for design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Winston, P. H., with Shellard, S. A. (Eds.). (1990). Artificial intelligence at MIT: Expanding frontiers (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan. (Original work published 1953)
Wolfram, S. (2002). A new kind of science. Champaign, IL: Wolfram Media.

References

Anderson, M. L. (2003). Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence, 149, 91–130.
Baldwin, D. A., & Baird, J. A. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 171–178.
Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., Saylor, M. M., & Clark, M. A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 708–717.
Bergson, H. (1911). Matter and memory (N. M. Paul & W. S. Palmer, Trans.). London: George Allen & Unwin.
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: Reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bredo, E. (1994). Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyan pragmatism. Educational Psychologist, 29, 23–35.
Brooks, R. (1999). Cambrian intelligence: The early history of the new AI. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18, 32–42.
Clancey, W. J. (1997). Situated cognition: On human knowledge and computer representation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 527–533.
Dennett, D. (2003). Ludwig Wittgenstein. The Time 100. Retrieved May 9, 2008, from http://www.time.com/time/time100/scientist/profile/wittgenstein.html.
Dewey, J. (1884). The new psychology. Andover Review, 2, 278–289. Retrieved May 9, 2008, from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Dewey/newpsych.htm.
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370. Retrieved May 9, 2008, from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Dewey/reflex.htm.
Dewey, J. (1938a). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Dewey, J. (1938b). Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.
Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 83–107.
Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gallagher, S. (2007). Logical and phenomenological arguments against simulation theory. In D. Hutto & M. Ratcliffe (Eds.), Minding our practice: Folk psychology re-assessed (pp. 63–78). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.
Gallese, V. L., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.
Gauker, C. (2003). Words without meaning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gauker, C. (2005). On the evidence for prelinguistic concepts. Theoria, 54, 287–297.
Goldman, A. I., & Sripada, C. S. (2005). Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition, 94, 193–213.
Head, H. (1920). Studies in neurology (Vol. 2). London: Oxford University Press.
Head, H., & Holmes, G. (1911–1912). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102–245.
Heidegger, M. (1968). Being and time (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, Trans.). New York: Harper & Row.
Hurley, S. (1998). Consciousness in action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hurley, S. (2006). Active perception and perceiving action: The shared circuits hypothesis. In T. Gendler & J. Hawthorne (Eds.), Perceptual experience (pp. 205–259). New York: Oxford University Press.
Husserl, E. (1952). Ideen zu einer reinen phänomenologie und phänomenologischen philosophie. Zweites Buch. (M. Biemel, Ed.), Husserliana IV. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
Hutto, D. (2005). Knowing what? Radical versus conservative enactivism. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 389–405.
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109.
Jeannerod, M., & Pacherie, E. (2004). Agency, simulation, and self-identification. Mind & Language, 19(2), 113–146.
Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kirsh, D. (2004). Metacognition, distributed cognition and visual design. In P. Gärdenfors & P. Johansson (Eds.), Cognition, education, and communication technology (pp. 147–180). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Marx, K. (1974). Economic and philosophical manuscripts of 1844. Moscow: Progress.
Marx, K., & Engels, F. (1964). The German ideology (S. Ryazanskaya, Trans.). Moscow: Progress.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1968). The visible and the invisible (A. Lingis, Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Nagel, T. (1986). The view from nowhere. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Nietzsche, F. W. (1967). The will to power (R. Hollingdale & W. Kaufmann, Trans.). New York: Random House.
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Noë, A., & O’Regan, J. K. (2002). On the brain basis of perceptual consciousness. In A. Noë & E. Thompson (Eds.), Vision and mind: Selected readings in the philosophy of perception (pp. 567–598). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor approach to vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973.
Overgaard, S. (2004). The private language argument and externalism. Danish Yearbook of Philosophy, 39, 17–48.
Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language, and reality: Philosophical papers (Vol. 2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., & Fazio, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research, 111, 246–252.
Rowlands, M. (2007). Understanding the ‘active’ in ‘enactive.’ Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 427–443.
Santayana, G. (1939). Dewey's naturalistic metaphysics. In P. A. Schilpp (Ed.), The philosophy of John Dewey (pp. 245–261). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
Spiegelberg, H. (1976). The phenomenological tradition. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
Taylor, F. W. (1967). The principles of scientific management. New York: Harper & Brothers, W. W. Norton & Company. (Original work published 1911)
Thompson, E., & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 418–425.
Travis, C. (1989). The uses of sense: Wittgenstein's philosophy of language. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wheeler, M. (2005). Reconstructing the cognitive world. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations (G. E. M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Wittgenstein, L. (1967). Zettel. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Wittgenstein, L. (1969). On certainty. New York: Harper & Row.