Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-r5zm4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T15:03:56.820Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VI - Domains for the Evaluation of Morphological Theories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2017

Andrew Hippisley
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Gregory Stump
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Alegre, Maria, and Gordon, Peter. 1999. Frequency effects and the representational status of regular inflections. Journal of Memory and Language 40, 4161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, Eduardo G.; Pierrehumbert, Janet B., and Motter, Adilson E.. 2009. Beyond word frequency: Bursts, lulls, and scaling in the temporal distributions of words. PLoS One 4.ll. e7678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anshen, Frank, and Aronoff, Mark. 1997. Morphology in real time. Yearbook of Morphology 1996, 9–12.Google Scholar
Aronoff, Mark. 1980. The relevance of productivity in a synchronic description of word formation. In Fisiak, Jacek (ed.), Historical Morphology: Papers prepared for the International Conference on Historical Morphology Held at Boszkowo, Poland, 15–18 March 1978, 7182. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, Mark, and Anshen, Frank. 2001. Morphology and the lexicon: Lexicalization and productivity. In Spencer, Andrew and Zwicky, Arnold M. (eds.), The Handbook of Morphology, 237–47. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Aslin, Richard N.; Saffran, Jenny R., and Newport, Elissa L.. 1998. Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science 9.4, 321–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auer, Edward T.; Bernstein, Lynne E., and Tucker, Paula E.. 2000. Is subjective word familiarity a meter of ambient language? A natural experiment on effects of perceptual experience. Memory and Cognition 28.5, 789–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 1992a. On frequency, transparency and productivity. Yearbook of Morphology 1991, 181–208.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 1992b. Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity. Yearbook of Morphology 1991, 109–49.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 1994. Derivational productivity and text typology. Journal of Quantitatie Linguistics 1.1, 1634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2000. Word Frequency Distributions. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2009. Corpus linguistics in morphology: Morphological productivity. In Luedeling, A. and Kyto, M. (eds.), Corpus Linguistics: An International Handbook, 909–19. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2010. Demythologizing the word frequency effect: A discriminative learning perspective. The Mental Lexicon 5, 436–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2011. Corpus linguistics and naive discriminative learning. Revista Brasileira de Linguistica Aplicada 11.2, 295328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, and Hendrix, Peter. 2011. Sidestepping the combinatorial explosion: Towards a processing model based on discriminative learning. In Empirically examining parsimony and redundancy in usage-based models, Isa workshop.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, and Renouf, Antoinette. 1996. Chronicling the Times: Productive lexical innovations in an English newspaper. Language 72, 6996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald; Piepenbrock, Richard, and van Rijn, Hedderik. 1993. The CELEX Lexical Database. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald; Dijkstra, Ton, and Schreuder, Robert. 1997. Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language 37.1, 94117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald; Milin, P., Filipovic Durdevic, D., Hendrix, P., and Marelli, M.. 2011. An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review 118, 438–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, Paul. 2010. Diachronic variation. In Sociolinguistics and Corpus Linguistics, 5780. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Balota, David A.; Pilotti, Maura, and Cortese, Michael J. 2001. Subjective frequency estimates for 2,938 monosyllabic words. Memory and Cognition 29.4, 639–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Laurie. 1994. Watching English Change. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Bauer, Laurie. 2001. Morphological Productivity, Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 95. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, Alan; Brenier, Jason, Gregory, Michelle, Giraud, Cynthia, and Jurafsky, Dan. 2009. Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language 60, 92111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, Thomas. 2011. A diachronic frequency account of the allomorphy of some grammatical markers. Journal of Linguistics 47.1, 3164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertram, R.; Schreuder, Robert, and Harald Baayen, R.. 2000. The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: The role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26, 123.Google ScholarPubMed
Blevins, James P. 2003. Stems and paradigms. Language 79.4, 737–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burani, C., and Caramazza, A.. 1987. Representation and processing of derived words. Language and Cognitive Processes 2, 217–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1985. Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan 1995. Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10.5, 425–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan, and Thompson, Sandra. 1997. Three frequency effects in syntax. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on Pragmatics and Grammatical Structure, 378–88. Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Chapman, Don, and Skousen, Royal. 2005. Analogical Modeling and morphological change: The case of the adjectival negative prefix in English. English Language and Linguistics 9.2, 333–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciszek, Ewa. 2012. The Middle English suffix -ish: Reasons for decline in productivity. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 47.2–3, 2739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connine, Cynthia M.; Mullennix, John, Shernoff, Eve, and Yelen, Jennifer. 1990. Word familiarity and frequency in visual and auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16.6, 1084–96.Google ScholarPubMed
Dabrowska, Ewa. 2008. The effects of frequency and neighbourhood density on adult speakers: Productivity with Polish case inflections. An empirical test of usage based approaches to morphology. Journal of Memory and Language 58.4, 931–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daelemans, W.; Zavrel, J., van der Sloot, K., and van den Bosch, A.. 2007. TiMBL: Tilburg Memory Based Learner, version 6.1, Reference Guide. Tech. rep. ILK Research Group Technical Report Series no. 07-07.Google Scholar
Dalton-Puffer, Christiane. 1996. The French Influence on Middle English Morphology: A Corpus-based Study of Derivation, Topics in English Linguistics 20. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalton-Puffer, Christiane, and Cowie, Claire. 2002. Diachronic word-formation and studying changes in productivity over time: Theoretical and methodological considerations. In Dfaz Vera, J. E. (ed.), A Changing World of Words: Studies in English Historical Lexicography, Lexicology and Semantics, 410–37. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Davies, Mark 2008. The corpus of contemporary American English: 425 million words, 1990-present. Available online at http://corpus.byu.edu/coca (accessed April 18, 2016).Google Scholar
de Jong, Ninja; Schreuder, Robert, and R. Harald Baayen, . 2000. The morphological family size effect and morphology. Journal of Memory and Language 42, 390405.Google Scholar
Enger, Hans-Olav. 2004. On the relation between gender and declension: A diachronic perspective from Norwegian. Studies in Language 28.1, 5182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, Stefan A.; Pierrehumbert, Janet B., and Broe, Michael B.. 2004. Similarity avoidance and the OCP. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 22, 179228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, Morton A. 1984. Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113.2, 256–81.Google ScholarPubMed
Gonnerman, L.; Seidenberg, M. S., and Andersen, E.. 2007. Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136, 323–45.Google ScholarPubMed
Gries, Stefan T. 2008. Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 13.4, 403–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan T. 2009. Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora: Further explorations. Language and Computers 71.1, 197212.Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan T. 2011. Frequency tables: Tests, effect sizes, and explorations. In Glynn, Dylan and Robinson, Justyna A. (eds.), Polysemy and Synonymy: Corpus Methods and Applications in Cognitive Linguistics, 365–89. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan T. 2014a. Corpus and quantitative methods. In The Bloomsbury Companion to Cognitive Linguistics, 279300. London and, New York: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan T. 2014b. Quantitative corpus approaches to linguistic analysis: Seven or eight levels of resolution and the lessons they teach us. In Taavitsainen, Irma, Kyto, Merja, Claridge, Claudia, and Smith, Jeremy (eds.), Developments in English: Expanding Electronic Evidence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan T., and Hilpert, Martin. 2008. The identification of stages in diachronic data: Variability-based neighbour clustering. Corpora 3.1, 5981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan T., and Hilpert, Martin. 2010. Modeling diachronic change in the third person singular: A multifactorial, verb- and author-specific exploratory approach. English Language and Linguistics 14.3, 293320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan T., and Hilpert, Martin. 2012. Variability-based neighbor clustering: A bottom-up approach to periodization in historical linguistics. In Nevalainen, Terttu and Traugott, Elizabeth Closs (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of the History of English. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and Newman, John 2013. Research methods in Linguistics chap. Creating and Using Corpora. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer B. 2001. Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative? Linguistics 39.6, 1041–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer B. 2003. Causes and Consequences of Word Structure. New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer B., and Baayen, R. Harald. 2002. Parsing and productivity. Yearbook of Morphology 2001, 203–35.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer B., and Baayen, R. Harald. 2005. Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9.7, 342–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hay, Jennifer B., Jannedy, Stefanie, and Bod, Rens (eds.) 2003. Probabilistic Linguistics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hilpert, Martin. 2011. Diachronic collostructional analysis: How to use it and how to deal with confounding factors. In Allan, Kathryn and Robinson, Justyna A. (eds.), Current Methods in Historical Semantics, Topics in English Linguistics 73, 133–60. Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hilpert, Martin, and Gries, Stefan T.. 2009. Assessing frequency changes in multistage diachronic corpora: Applications for historical corpus linguistics and the study of language acquisition. Literary and Linguistic Computing 24.4, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinneburg, Alexander; Mannila, Heikki, Kaislaniemi, Samuli, Nevalainen, Terttu, and Raumolin-Brunberg, Helena. 2007. How to handle small samples: Bootstrap and Bayesian methods in the analysis of linguistic change. Literary and Linguistic Computing 22.2, 137–50.Google Scholar
Johnston, Robert, and Barry, Christopher. 2006. Age of acquisition and lexical processing. Visual Cognition 13, 789845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaunisto, Mark. 2009. The rivalry between English adjectives ending in -ive and -ory. In McConchie, R. W., Honkapohja, Alpo, and Tyrkko, Jukka (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2008 Symposium on New Approaches in English Historical Lexis (HEL-LEX 2), 7487. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Kennedy, Alan; Hill, Robin, and Pynte, Joel. 2003. The Dundee Corpus. In Proceedings of the 12th European Conference on Eye Movement. University of Dundee.Google Scholar
Kilgarriff, Adam, and Grefenstette, Gregory. 2003. Introduction to the special issue on the web as corpus. Computational Linguistics 29.3, 333–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, Klaus-Michael. 1988. Schemas in German plural formation. Lingua 74, 303–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kostić, Aleksandar. 1995. Information load constraints on processing inflected morphology. In Feldman, Laurie Beth (ed.), Morphological Aspects of Language Processing, 317–44. Hillsdale, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kostić, Aleksandar; Markovic, Tanja, and Baucal, Aleksandar. 2003. Inflectional morphology and word meaning: Orthogonal or co-implicative cognitive domains? In Harald Baayen, R. and Schreuder, Robert (eds.), Morphological Structure in Language Processing, Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs (TiLSM) 151, 144. Amsterdam: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kuc̆era, Henry, and Francis, Winthrop Nelson. 1967. Computational Analysis of Present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University.Google Scholar
Kuc̆era, Karol. 2007. Mapping the time continuum: A major raison d’être for diachronic corpora. In Proceedings of Corpus Linguistics Birmingham 2007, University of Birmingham.Google Scholar
Laitinen, Mikko. 2008. Sociolinguistic patterns in grammaticalization: He, they, and those in human indefinite reference. Language Variation and Change 20, 155–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites, vol. 1. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lüdeling, Anke, and Evert, Stefan. 2005. The emergence of non-medical -itis: Corpus evidence and qualitative analysis. In Kepser, S. and Reis, M. (eds.), Linguistic Evidence. Empirical, Theoretical, and Computational Perspectives, 315–33. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Manning, Christopher D., and Schütze, Hinrich. 1999. Foundations of Statistical Natural Language Processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Medina Urrea, Alfonso. 2009. Toward a comparison of unsupervised diachronic morphological profiles. Language and Computers 71, 2945.Google Scholar
Milin, Petar; Kuperman, Victor, Kostic, Aleksandar, and Harald Baayen, R.. 2009. Paradigms bit by bit: An information theoretic approach to the processing of paradigmatic structure in inflection and derivation. In Blevins, James P. and Blevins, Juliette (eds.), Analogy in Grammar: Form and Acquisition, 214–52. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moscoso del Prado Martín, Fermin; Bertram, Raymond, Haikio, Tuomo, Schreuder, Robert, and Harald Baayen, R.. 2004a. Morphological family size in a morphologically rich language: The case of Finnish compared to Dutch and Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30, 1271–8.Google Scholar
Moscoso del Prado Martín, Fermin; Bertram, Raymond, Kostic, Aleksandar, and Harald Baayen, R.. 2004b. Putting the bits together: An information theoretical perspective on morphological processing. Cognition 94.1, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevalainen, Terttu. 2000. Gender differences in the evolution of Standard English: Evidence from the Corpus of Early English Correspondence. Journal of English Linguistics 28.1, 3859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevalainen, Terttu, and Raumolin-Brunberg, Helena. 2003. Historical Sociolinguistics: Language Change in Tudor and Stuart England. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Pappas, Panayiotis A. 2001. The microcosm of a morphological change: Variation in thelo + infinitive futures and ethela + infinitive counterfactuals in Early Modern Greek. Diachronica 18.1, 5992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2012. Burstiness of verbs and derived nouns. In Santos, Diana, Lindén, Krister, and Ng’ang’a, Wanjiku (eds.), Shall We Play the Festschrift Game? Essays on the Occasion of Lauri Carlson’s 6Oth Birthday, 99115. Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plag, Ingo. 1999. Morphological Productivity: Structural Constraints in English Derivation, Topics in English Linguistics 28. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Plag, Ingo, and Harald Baayen, R.. 2009. Suffix ordering and morphological processing. Language 85, 106–49.Google Scholar
Plag, Ingo; Dalton-Puffer, Christiane, Harald Baayen, R., et al. 1999. Morphological productivity across speech and writing. English Language and Linguistics 3.2, 209–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prasada, S., and Pinker, S., 1993. Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes 8.1, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raumolin-Brunberg, Helena, 2005. The diffusion of subject you: A case in historical sociolinguistics. Language Variation and Change 17.1, 5573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, Jenny R.; Newport, Elissa L., and Aslin, Richard N.. 1996. Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language 35.4, 606–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Säily, Tanja. 2008. Productivity of the Suffixes -ness and -ity in 17th Century English Letters: A Sociolinguistic Approach. Master’s thesis, Helsinki University of Helsinki.Google Scholar
Säily, Tanja 2011. Variation in morphological productivity in the BNC: Sociolinguistic and methodological considerations. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 7.1, 119–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Säily, Tanja, and Suomela, Jukka. 2009. Comparing type counts: The case of women, men and -ty in early English letters. In Renouf, Antoinette and Kehoe, A. (eds.), Corpus Linguistics: Refinements and Reassessments, 87109. Amsterdam: Rodopi.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreuder, Robert, and Harald Baayen, R.. 1995. Modeling morphological processing. In Feldman, Laurie (ed.), Morphological Aspects of Language Processing, 131–54. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schreuder, Robert, and R. Harald Baayen, . 1997. How complex simplex words can be. Journal of Memory and Language 37.1, 118–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skousen, Royal. 2002. Analogical Modeling: An Exemplar-based Approach to Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stichauer, Pavel. 2009. Morphological productivity in diachrony: The case of deverbal nouns in -mento, -zione and -gione in Old Italian from the 13th to the 16th century. In Montermini, Fabio, Boy, Gilles, and Tseng, Jesse (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Décembrettes: Morphology in Bordeaux, 138–47. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Taft, Marcus. 1979. Recognition of affixed words and the word frequency effect. Memory and Cognition 7.4, 263–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tily, Harry; Gabi, Susanne, Arnon, Jubal, Snider, Neal, Kothari, Anubha, and Bresnan, Joan. 2009. Syntactic probabilities affect pronunciation variation in spontaneous speech. Language and Cognition 1.2, 147–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trips, Carola. 2009. Lexical Semantics and Diachronic Morphology: The Development of -hood, -dom and -ship in the History of English, Linguistische Arbeiten 527. Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, Abby, and Hay, Jennifer B.. 2011. Congruence between “word age” and “voice age” facilitates lexical access. Laboratory Phonology 2.1, 219–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witten, I. H.; Frank, E., Trigg, L., Hall, M., Holmes, G., and Cunningham, S. J.. 1999. Weka: Practical machine learning tools and techniques with Java implementations. ICONIP/ANZIIS/ANNES, 99, 192–6.Google Scholar
Zipf, George Kingsley. 1935. The Psycho-biology of Language: An Introduction to Dynamic Philology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zséder, Attila; Recski, Gabor, Varga, Daniel, and Kornai, Andras. 2012. Rapid creation of large-scale corpora and frequency dictionaries. In Calzolari, Nicoletta (ed.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’12), 1462–5. Istanbul: European Language Resources Association (ELBA).Google Scholar

References

Aissen, Judith. 1997. On the syntax of obviation. Language 73,705–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aissen, Judith. 1999. Markedness and subject choice in Optimality Theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 17, 673711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21, 435–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansaldo, Umberto; Don, Jan, and Pfau, Roland (eds.) 2010. Parts of Speech: Empirical and Theoretical Advances. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arkad’ev, P. M.; Lander, J. A., Letuchij, A. B., Sumbatova, N. R., and Testelec, J. G.. 2009. Vvedenie: Osnovnye svedenija ob adygejskom jazyke. In Arkad’ev, P. M., Lander, J. A., Letuchij, A. B., Sumbatova, N. R., and Testelec, J. G. (eds.), Aspekty polisintetizma: Ocherki po grammatike adygejskogo jazyka, 17120. Moscow: Rossijskij gosudarstvennyj gumanitarnyj universitet.Google Scholar
Atkinson, Quentin D. 2011. Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science 332, 346–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, Mark C. 2003. Lexical Categories: Verbs, Nouns, and Adjectives. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, David. 2013. Unidirectional flexibility and the noun-verb distinction in Lushootseed. In Rijkhoff, Jan, and van Lier, Eva (eds.), Flexible Word Classes: Typological Studies of Underspecified Parts of Speech, 185220. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berge, Anna. 2015. Reexamining the Linguistic Prehistory of Aleut. Paper presented at N-TAG XV, Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar. 2007. Typology in the 21st century: Major current developments. Linguistic Typology 11, 239–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar. 2011. Grammatical relations typology. In Song, Jae Jung (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology, 399444. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar. 2013. Distributional biases in language families. In Bickel, et al. (eds.), Linguistic Typology and Historical Contingency, 415–44. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar, and Nichols, Johanna. 2007. Inflectional morphology. In Shopen, Tim (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, 3, 169240. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar, and Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena. 2008. Referential scales and case alignment: Reviewing the typological evidence. In Malchukov, Andrej L. and Richards, Marc (eds.), Scales, 137. Leipzig: Institut für Linguistik.Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar; Hildebrandt, Kristine A., and Schiering, René. 2009. The distribution of phonological word domains: A probabilistic typology. In Grijzenhout, Janet and Kabak, Bariş (eds.), Phonological Domains: Universals and Deviations, 4775. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar; Zakharko, Taras, Bierkandt, Lennart, and Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena. 2014. Semantic role clustering: An empirical assessment of semantic role types in non-default case assignment. Studies in Language 38.3, 485511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, Frans. 1911. Introduction: Handbook of American Indian Languages, vol. 1, 183. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bond, Oliver. 2013. A base for canonical negation. In Brown, Dunstan, Chumakina, Marina, and Corbett, Greville (eds.), Canonical Morphology and Syntax, 2047. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Ina; Malchukov, Andrej L., and Richards, Marc D. (eds.) 2015. Scales and Hierarchies: A Cross-disciplinary Perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Dunstan; Chumakina, Marina, and Corbett, Greville (eds.) 2013. Canonical Morphology and Syntax. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger, and Gilman, Albert. 1972. The pronouns of solidarity and power. In Giglioli, Pier Paolo (ed.), Language and Social Context, 252–82. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1985. Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1981. Language Universals and Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Corbett, Greville. 2001. Number. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Corbett, Greville. 2004. The Russian adjective: A pervasive yet elusive category. In Dixon, R. M. W. and Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (eds.), Adjective Classes: A Cross-linguistic Typology, 199222. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, Greville. 2007. Canonical typology, suppletion, and possible words. Language 83.1, 842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, Greville. 2010. Canonical derivational morphology. Word Structure, 3.2. 141–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, Greville G. 2013a. Canonical morphosyntactic features. In Brown, Dunstan, Chumakina, Marina, and Corbett, Greville (eds.), Canonical Morphology and Syntax, 4865. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Corbett, Greville G. 2013b. The unique challenge of the Archi paradigm. In Cathcart, Chundra, Kang, Shinae, and Sandy, Clare S. (eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting, Berkeley Linguistics Society: Special Session on Languages of the Caucasus, 5267. Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Corbett, Greville G. 2015. Morphosyntactic complexity: A typology of lexical splits. Language 91.1, 145–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creissels, Denis. 2008. Remarks on So-called “Conjunct/Disjunct” Systems. Paper presented in Syntax Of The World’s Languages III. Berlin.Google Scholar
Curnow, Timothy J. 1997. A Grammar of Awa Pit (Cuaiquer): An Indigenous Language of Southwestern Colombia. Ph.D. dissertation, Australian National University.Google Scholar
Curnow, Timothy J. 2002. Conjunct/disjunct marking in Awa Pit. Linguistics 40, 611–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cysouw, Michael. 2003. The Paradigmatic Structure of Person Marking. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, Östen. 2004. The Growth and Maintenance of Linguistic Complexity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny, J. Peter. 1989. The nature of polysynthesis in Algonquian and Eskimo. In Gerdts, Donna B. and Michelson, Karin (eds.), Theoretical Perspectives on Native American Languages, 230–58. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Dickinson, Connie. 2000. Mirativity in Tsafiki. Studies in Language 24, 379421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. 1977. A Grammar of Yidiny. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. and Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (eds.) 2002. Word: A Cross-linguistic Typology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Donohue, Mark. 2008. Complex predicate and bipartite stems in Skou. Studies in Language 32, 279335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donohue, Mark, and Wichmann, Søren (eds.) 2008. The Typology of Semantic Alignment Systems. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dryer, Matthew S., and Haspelmath, Martin (eds.) 2013. The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library, http://wals.info (accessed May 1, 2016).Google Scholar
Evans, Nicholas, and Osada, Toshiki. 2005. Mundari: The myth of a language without word classes. Linguistic Typology 9, 351–90.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1997. Lectures on Deixis. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Floyd, Simeon; Norcliffe, Elisabeth, and Roque, Lila San (eds.) 2016. Egophoricity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Foley, William A. 1998. Symmetrical voice systems and precategoriality in Philippine languages. Presentation at LFG 3 Conference, Brisbane.Google Scholar
Foley, William A. Forthcoming. The Epidemiology of Language: The Evolution of Word Class Categorialization in the Austronesian Languages.Google Scholar
Foley, William A., and Nichols, Johanna. In preparation. Rigid, flexible, precategorical: A wordlist-based part-of-speech typology.Google Scholar
Fortescue, Michael. 2013. Polysynthesis in the Subarctic: How recent is it? In Bickel, Balthasar, Grenoble, Lenore, Peterson, David A., and Timberlake, Alan (eds.), Language Typology and Historical Contingency, 241–64. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fortescue, Michael. In press. What are the limits of polysynthesis? In Nicholas Evans, Marianne Mithun, and Michael Fortescue (eds.), Polysynthesis, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garrett, Andrew. 1990. Hittite enclitic subjects and transitive verbs. Journal of Cuneiform Studies 42.2, 227–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gil, David. 2013. Riau Indonesian: A language without nouns and verbs. In Rijkhoff, Jan, and van Lier, Eva (eds.), Flexible Word Classes: Typological Studies of Underspecified Parts of Speech, 89130. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Good, Jeffrey C. (ed.) 2008. Linguistic Universals and Language Change. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Good, Jeff. 2016. The Linguistic Typology of Templates. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1963. Some universals of language with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Greenberg, Joseph H. (ed.), Universals of Language, 73113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hale, Austin. 1980. Person markers: Finite conjunct and disjunct verb forms in Newari. In Trail, Ron (ed.), Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics, no. 7, 95106. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Hanks, William. 1990. Referential Practice. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1996. Word-class-changing inflection and morphological theory. Yearbook of Morphology 1995, 43–66.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2011. The indeterminacy of word segmentation and the nature of morphology and syntax. Folia Linguistica 45.1, 3180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, Jeffrey. 1991. Pragmatic disguise in pronominal-affix paradigms. In Plank, Frans (ed.), Paradigms: The Economy of Inflection, 7589. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, Jeffrey. 1998. Pragmatic skewing in 1 < > 2 pronominal combinations in Native American languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 64.2, 83104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmbrecht, Johannes. 2003. Politeness distinctions in second person pronouns. In Lenz, Friedrich (ed.), Deictic Conceptualization of Space, Time, and Person, 185202. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmbrecht, Johannes. 2013. Politeness distinctions in pronouns. In Dryer, Matthew S. and Haspelmath, Martin (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/45 (accessed May 29, 2015).Google Scholar
Hengeveld, Kees; Rijkhoff, Jan, and Siewierska, Anna. 2004. Parts-of-speech systems and word order. Journal of Linguistics 40, 527–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hildebrandt, Kristine. 2007. Prosodic and grammatical domains in Limbu. Himalayan Linguistics 8, 134.Google Scholar
Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 1991. The Philippine Challenge to Universal Grammar. Universität zu Köln. Institut für Sprachwissenschaft.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry. 2008. Directional asymmetries in the morphology and phonology of words, with special reference to Bantu. Linguistics 46, 309–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, William H. Jr. 1979. Noun and verb in Nootkan. In Efrat, Barbara (ed.), The Victoria Conference on Northwestern Languages, 83153. Victoria: British Columbia Provincial Museum.Google Scholar
Janhunen, Juha. 2002. On the chronology of the Ainu ethnic complex. Bulletin of the Hokkaido Museum of Northern Peoples 11, 120.Google Scholar
Janunan, Juha. 2012. Mongolian. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelinek, Eloise. 1984. Empty categories, case, and configurationality. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 2, 3676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juola, Patrick. 2008. Assessing linguistic complexity. In Miestamo, Matti, Sinnemäki, Kaius, and Karlsson, Fred (eds.), Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change, 89108. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, Edward L., and Comrie, Bernard. 1977. Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8, 6399.Google Scholar
Kemmer, Suzanne. 1993. The Middle Voice. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kibrik, Andrej A. 2011. Reference in Discourse. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinkade, M. Dale. 1983. Salish evidence against the universality of “Noun” and “Verb.” Lingua 60, 2539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaiman, M. H. 1991. Grammatical Voice. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kusters, Wouter. 2003. Linguistic Complexity: The Influence of Social Change on Verbal Inflection. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Leiden.Google Scholar
Leipzig Valence Classes Project. n.d. Available online at www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/valency/index.php (accessed April 12, 2016)Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 1983. Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieber, Rochelle, and Štekauer, Pavol (eds.) 2014. The Oxford Handbook of Derivational Morphology. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miestamo, Matti. 2008. Grammatical complexity in cross-linguistic perspective. In Miestamo, Matti, Sinnemäki, Kaius, and Karlsson, Fred (eds.), Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change, 2341. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miestamo, Matti, and Wälchli, Bernhard (eds.) 2007. New Challenges in Typology. Trends in Linguistics: Studies and Monographs. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miestamo, Matti; Sinnemäki, Kaius, and Karlsson, Fred (eds.) 2008. Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Wick R. 1965. Acoma Grammar and Texts. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Mosel, Ulrike, and Hovdhaugen, Even. 1992. Samoan Reference Grammar. Oslo: Scandinavia University Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, Frederick J., and Preston, Laurel B. (eds.) 2014. Measuring Grammatical Complexity. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 2009. Linguistic complexity: A comprehensive definition and survey. In Sampson, Geoffrey, Gil, David, and Trudgill, Peter (eds.), Language Complexity as an Evolving Variable, 110–25. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 2011. Forerunners to globalization: The Eurasian steppe and its periphery. In Hasselblatt, Cornelius, Houtzagers, Peter, and van Pareren, Remco (eds.), Language Contact in Times of Globalization, 177–95. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 2013a. The origin and evolution of case-suppletive pronouns: Eurasian evidence. In Bakker, Dik and Haspelmath, Martin (eds.), Languages across Boundaries: Studies in Memory of Anna Siewierska, 313–45. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 2013b. The vertical archipelago: Adding the third dimension to linguistic geography. In Auer, Peter, Hilpert, Martin, Stukenbrock, Anja, and Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt (eds.), Space in Language and Linguistics, 3860. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 2016. Verb-based and noun-based languages. Presentation at Societas Linguistica Europaea annual meeting, Naples.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. In press. Polysynthesis and head marking. In Nicholas Evans, Marianne Mithun, and Michael Fortescue (eds.), Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna, and Nichols, Lynn. 2007. Lexical derivational properties resist diffusion. Workshop on Language Contact and Morphosyntactic Variation and Change, ALT 7, Paris.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna, and Peterson, David A.. 2013. M-T pronouns: N-M pronouns. In Dryer, Matthew S. and Haspelmath, Martin (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/136 (accessed May 29, 2015).Google Scholar
Öztürk, Balkiz, and Pöchtrager, Markus A. (eds.) 2011. Pazar Laz. LW/Materials. Munich: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Pakendorf, Brigitte. 2014. Paradigm copying in Tungusic: The Lamunkhin dialect of Èven. In Robbeets, Martine and Bisang, Walter (eds.), Paradigm Change in the Transeurasian Languages and Beyond, 287310. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Peterson, John. 2011. A Grammar of Kharia: A South Munda Language. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, John. 2013. Parts of speech in Kharia: A formal account. In Rijkhoff, Jan and van Lier, Eva (eds.), Flexible Word Classes: Typological Studies of Underspecified Parts of Speech, 131–68. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Plank, Frans (ed.) 2011. The vanishing phonemes debate, apropos of Atkinson 2011. Linguistic Typology 15.2, 147332.Google Scholar
Rice, Keren. 1989. A Grammar of Slave. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, Keren. 2000. Morpheme Order and Semantic Scope: Word Formation in the Athapaskan Verb. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, Keren. 2011. Principles of affix ordering: An overview. Word Structure 4.2, 169200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijkhoff, Jan, and van Lier, Eva (eds.) 2013. Flexible Word Classes: Typological Studies of Underspecified Parts of Speech. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Françoise 2009. A hierarchical indexation system: The example of Emerillon (Teko). In Epps, Patience and Arkhipov, Alexandre (eds.), New Challenges in Typology: Transcending the Borders and Refining the Distinctions, 6383. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, Geoffrey; Gil, David, and Trudgill, Peter (eds.) 2009. Linguistic Complexity as an Evolving Variable. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, Edward, and Swadesh, Morris. 1939. Nootka Texts: Tales and Ethnological Narratives with Grammatical Notes and Lexical Materials. Philadelphia: LSA and University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Sapir, Edward, and Swadesh, Morris. 1946. American Indian grammatical categories. Word 2, 103–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiering, René. 2009. Stress-timed = word-based? Testing a hypothesis in prosodic typology. Presented at 8th Biannual Meeting of the Association for Linguistic Typology, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Schiering, Rene; Bickel, Balthasar, and Hildebrandt, Kristine. 2010. The prosodic word is not universal but emergent. Journal of Linguistics 46, 657709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifart, Frank. 2012. The principle of morphosyntactic subsystem integrity in language contact: Evidence from morphological borrowing in Resígaro (Arawakan). Diachronica 29.4, 471504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seifart, Frank. 2013. AfBo: A worldwide survey of affix borrowing. Available online at http://afbo.info (accessed April 6, 2015).Google Scholar
Shosted, Ryan K. 2006. Correlating complexity: A typological approach. Linguistic Typology 10, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siewierska, Anna. 2004. Person. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siewierska, Anna. 2011. Person marking. In Song, Jae Jung (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology, 322–45. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Siewierska, Anna, and Bakker, Dik. 2013. Suppletion in person forms: The role of iconicity and frequency. In Bakker, Dik and Haspelmath, Martin (eds.), Languages across Boundaries: Studies in Memory of Anna Siewierska, 347–95. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Silverstein, Michael. 1976. Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Dixon, R. M. W. (ed.), Grammatical Categories in Australian Languages, 112–71. Canberra and Atlantic Highlands: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies and Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Sinnemäki, Kaius. 2011. Language Universals and Linguistic Complexity: Three Case Studies in Core Argument Marking. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Helsinki.Google Scholar
Sinnemäki, Kaius. 2009. Complexity in core argument marking and population size. In Sampson, Geoffrey, Gil, David, and Trudgill, Peter (eds.), Linguistic Complexity as an Evolving Vvariable, 126–40. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sinnemäki, Kaius. 2014. Global optimization and complexity tradeoffs. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 50.2, 179–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, Andrew, and Luís, Ana. 2013. The canonical clitic. In Brown, Dunstan, Chumakina, Marina, and Corbett, Greville (eds.), Canonical Morphology and Syntax, 123–50. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Štekauer, Pavol, and Lieber, Rochelle (eds.) 2005. Handbook of Word Formation. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stump, Gregory, and Finkel, Raphael A.. 2013. Morphological Typology: From Word to Paradigm. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svantesson, Jan-Olof. 2003. Khalkha. In Janhunen, Juha (ed.), The Mongolic Languages, 154–73. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tchekhoff, Claude. 1984. Une langue sans opposition verbo-nominale: Le Tongien. Modèles linguistiques 6, 125–32.Google Scholar
Thompson, Laurence C., and Thompson, M. Terry. 1992. The Thompson Language. Missoula: University of Montana.Google Scholar
Thompson, Laurence C., and Thompson, M. Terry. 1996. Thompson River Salish Dictionary. Missoula: University of Montana.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 2011. Sociolinguistic Typology: Social Determinants of Linguistic Structure and Complexity. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vajda, Edward, and Nichols, Johanna (co-organizers); Anderson, Gregory D. S., Aronoff, Mark, Hyman, Larry M., Rhodes, Richard, and Rice, Keren. 2012. The diachronic stability of complex templatic morphology. Organized symposium, LSA Annual Meeting, Portland.Google Scholar
van Gijn, Rik, and Zúñiga, Fernando (eds.) 2014. Wordhood: Theory and typology from an Americanist perspective. Morphology 24.3, special issue.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena. 2010. Typological Variation in Grammatical Relations. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Leipzig.Google Scholar
Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena; Zakharko, Taras, Bierkandt, Lennart, Zúñiga, Fernando, and Bickel, Balthasar. 2016. Decomposing hierarchical alignment: Co-arguments as conditions on alignment and the limits of referential hierarchies as explanations in verb agreement. Linguistics. Published online. DOI: 10.1515/ling-2016-0011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfart, H. C., and Carroll, J. F.. 1981. Meet Cree: A Guide to the Cree Language, 2nd edn. Edmonton: University of Alberta Press.Google Scholar
Zúñiga, Fernando. 2007. From the typology of inversion to the typology of alignment. In Miestamo, Matti and Wälchli, Bernhard (eds.), New Challenges in Typology, 199221. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zúñiga, Fernando. 2006. Deixis and Alignment: Inverse Systems in Indigenous Languages of the Americas. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Anttila, Raimo. 1977. Analogy. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beard, Robert. 1995. Lexeme-morpheme Base Morphology: A General Theory of Inflection and Word Formation. Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Grannes, Alf. 1977. The use of the Turkish pluralizer -lx2r in South Slavic and Albanian. The New Zealand Slavonic Journal 3.2, 8393.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 2004. On directionality in language change with particular reference to grammaticalization. In Fischer, Olga, Norde, Muriel, and Perridson, Harry (eds.), Up and Down the Cline: The Nature of Grammaticalization, 744. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hock, Hans Henrich. 1991. Principles of Historical Linguistics [1986], 2nd edn. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogg, Richard. 1980. Analogy as a source of morphological complexity. Folia Linguistica Historica 1.2, 277–84.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul. 1994. Phonogenesis. In Pagliuca, William (ed.), Perspectives on Grammaticalization, 2945. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul, and Traugott, Elizabeth C.. 2003. Grammaticalization [1993], 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janda, Richard D. 1985. From agreement affix to subject “clitic”—and bound root: mos > -nos vs. (-)nos(-) and nos-otros in New Mexican and other regional Spanish dialects. Chicago Linguistic Society 31.1, 118–39.Google Scholar
Janda, Richard D., and Joseph, Brian D.. 2003a. Reconsidering the canons of sound change: Towards a Big Bang Theory. In Blake, Barry, and Burridge, Kate (eds.), Historical Linguistics 2001: Selected Papers from the 15th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Melbourne, 13–17 August 2001, 205–19. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Janda, Richard D., and Joseph, Brian D.. 2003b. On language, change, and language change—Or, of history, linguistics, and historical linguistics. In Joseph, Brian D. and Janda, Richard D. (eds.), Handbook of Historical Linguistics, 3180. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Janse, Mark. 2009. Watkins’ Law and the development of agglutinative inflections in Asia Minor Greek. Journal of Greek Linguistics 9, 93109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, Brian D. 2001. Defining “Word” in Modern Greek: A Response to Philippaki-Warburton and Spyropoulos 1999. Yearbook of Morphology 2001, 87–114.Google Scholar
Joseph, Brian D. 2004. Rescuing traditional (historical) linguistics from grammaticalization “theory.” In Fischer, Olga, Norde, Muriel, and Perridon, Harry (eds.), Up and Down the Cline: The Nature of Grammaticalization, 4471. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Joseph, Brian D. 2011. Grammaticalization: A general critique. In Narrog, Heiko and Heine, Bernd (eds.), Handbook of Grammaticalization, 193205. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Joseph, Brian D. 2014. What counts as (an instance of) grammaticalization? Folia Linguistica, 48.2, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, Brian D., and Janda, Richard D.. 1988. The how and why of diachronic morphologization and demorphologization. In Hammond, Michael and Noonan, Michael (eds.), Theoretical Morphology: Approaches in Modern Linguistics, 193210. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuryłowicz, Jerzy. 1945–9. La nature des procès dits “analogiques.” Acta Linguistica 5, 1537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightner, Theodore. 1975. The role of derivational morphology in generative grammar. Language 51, 617–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightner, Theodore. 1983. Introduction to English Derivational Morphology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mańczak, Witold. 1957. Tendences générales des changements analogiques. Lingua 7, 298325, 387420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Méndez Dosuna, Julian. 1997. Fusion, fission, and relevance in language change: Deuniverbation in Greek verb morphology. Studies in Language 21, 577612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norde, Muriel. 2009. Degrammaticalization. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurneysen, Rudolph. 1946. A Grammar of Old Irish, translated from the German by Binchy, D. A. and Bergin, Osborn. The Dublin Institute for Advanced Studies.Google Scholar
Watkins, Calvert W. 1962. Indo-European Origins of the Celtic Verb I: The Sigmatic Aorist. The Dublin Institute for Advanced Studies.Google Scholar
Winters, Margaret E. 1995. Jerzy Kurylowicz: the so-called laws of analogy. Diachronica 12, 113–45.Google Scholar
Zwicky, Arnold. 1994. What is a clitic? In Nevis, Joel A., Joseph, Brian D., Wanner, Dieter, and Zwicky, Arnold M. (eds.), Clitics: A Comprehensive Bibliography, 1892–1991, xiixx. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

References

Albright, A. 2005. The morphological basis of paradigm leveling. In Downing, L. J., Hall, T. A., and Raffelsiefen, R. (eds.), Paradigms in Phonological Theory, 1743. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Albright, A., and Hayes, B.. 2002. Modeling English past tense intuitions with minimal generalization. In Proceedings of the ACL-02 Workshop on Morphological and Phonological Learning, 5869. Association for Computational Linguistics.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albright, A., and Hayes, B. 2003. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition 90.2, 119–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, M., and Badecker, W.. 2002. Inflectional regularity: Probing the nature of lexical representation in a cross-modal priming task. Journal of Memory and Language 46.4, 705–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderwald, L. 2009. The Morphology of English Dialects: Verb-formation in Non-Standard English. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, S. L.; Gleitman, L. R., and Gleitman, H.. 1983. What some concepts might not be. Cognition 13.3, 263308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atserias, J.; Casas, B., Comelles, E., González, M., Padró, L., and Padró, M.. 2006. Freeling 1.3: Syntactic and semantic services in an open-source NLP library. In Proceedings of LREC 6, 4855.Google Scholar
Baerman, M.; Corbett, G. G., and Brown, D. (eds.) 2010. Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, M.; Ketrez, N., and Nevins, A.. 2011. The surfeit of the stimulus: Analytic biases filter lexical statistics in Turkish laryngeal alternations. Language 87.1, 84125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. 1958. The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 14, 150–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, B. 1947. English verb inflection. Language 23.4, 399418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1982. Reorganizational process in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L. R. (eds.), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art, 319–46. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brent, M.; Murthy, S., and Lundberg, A.. 1995. Discovering morphemic suffixes: A case study in minimum description length induction. In Proceedings of the Fifth International Workshop on Artificial Intelligence and Statistics, 264–71.Google Scholar
Brown, R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., and Hanlon, C.. 1970. Derivational complexity and the order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language, 1153. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. 1985. Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., and Moder, C. L.. 1983. Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59.2, 251–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L., and Slobin, D.. 1982. Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58.2, 265–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caprin, C., and Guasti, M. T.. 2009. The acquisition of morphosyntax in Italian: A cross-sectional study. Applied Psycholinguistics 30.1, 2352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, E. 2008. Structures and Distributions in Morphology Learning. Ph.D. thesis, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., and Halle, M.. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. 1999. Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22.6, 9911013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H.; Aveledo, F., and Roca, I.. 2002. The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language. Journal of Child Language 29.3, 591622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., and Penke, M.. 1992. The acquisition of agreement morphology and its syntactic consequences: New evidence on German child language from the simone corpus. In Meisel, J. (ed.), The Acquisition of Verb Placement 181234. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., and Rothweiler, M.. 1993. Inflectional rules in children’s grammars: Evidence from German participles. Yearbook of Morphology 1992, 1–34.Google Scholar
Creutz, M., and Lagus, K.. 2005. Unsupervised Morpheme Segmentation and Morphology Induction from Text Corpora Using Morfessor 1.0. Technical report, Helsinki University of Technology.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. 2001. Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language 28.3, 545–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deen, K. U. 2005. The Acquisition of Swahili, Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 40. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K. 2003. The acquisition of Bantu languages. In Nurse, D. and Philippson, G. (eds.), The Bantu Languages, 209–22. Richmond: Curzon Press.Google Scholar
Eddington, D. 1996. Diphthongization in Spanish derivational morphology: An empirical investigation. Hispanic Linguistics 8.1, 113.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. M., and Miller, W. R.. 1963. Language development. Child Psychology 62.1, 108–43.Google Scholar
Gerken, L.; Wilson, R., and Lewis, W.. 2005. Infants can use distributional cues to form syntactic categories. Journal of Child Language 32.2, 249–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, J. 2001. Unsupervised learning of the morphology of a natural language. Computational Linguistics 27.2, 153–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, J. 2006. An algorithm for the unsupervised learning of morphology. Natural Language Engineering 12.4, 353–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldwater, S.; Griffiths, T., and Johnson, M.. 2006. Interpolating between types and tokens by estimating power-law generators. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 18, 459–66.Google Scholar
Gómez, R. L., and Lakusta, L.. 2004. A first step in form-based category abstraction by 12-month-old infants. Developmental Science 7.5, 567–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guasti, M. T. 1993. Verb syntax in Italian child grammar: Finite and nonfinite verbs. Language Acquisition 3.1, 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gxilishe, S.; de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., Belikova, A., Meroni, L., and Umeda, M.. 2007. The acquisition of subject agreement in Xhosa. In Belikova, A., Meroni, L., and Umeda, M. (eds.), Proceedings of the Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition (GALANA), Vol. 2, 114–23. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Hafer, M. A., and Weiss, S. F.. 1974. Word segmentation by letter successor varieties. Information Storage and Retrieval 10.11, 371–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, U., and Nakisa, R. C.. 2000. German inflection: Single route or dual route? Cognitive Psychology 41.4, 313–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halle, M. 1973. Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Linguistic Inquiry 4.1, 316.Google Scholar
Halle, M., and Marantz, A.. 1993. Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J. (eds.), The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, 111–76. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Halle, M., and Mohanan, K. P.. 1985. Segmental phonology of Modern English. Linguistic Inquiry, 16.1, 57116.Google Scholar
Harris, J. W. 1969. Spanish Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harris, Z. S. 1955. From phoneme to morpheme. Language 31.2, 190222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., and Risley, T. R.. 2003. The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator 27.1, 49.Google Scholar
Hay, J., and Baayen, R. H.. 2005. Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9.7, 342–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, B.; Zuraw, K., Siptár, P., and Londe, Z.. 2009. Natural and unnatural constraints in Hungarian vowel harmony. Language 85.4, 822–63.Google Scholar
Herman, L., and Herman, M. S.. 2014. American Dialects: A Manual for Actors, Directors, and Writers. Abingdon: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarmulowicz, L. 2002. English derivational suffix frequency and children’s stress judgments. Brain and Language 81.1–3, 192204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.; Griffiths, T., and Goldwater, S.. 2007a. Bayesian inference for PCFGs via Markov Chain Monte Carlo. In Proceedings of NAACL HLT 2007, 139–46. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.; Griffiths, T. L., and Goldwater, S.. 2007b. Adaptor grammars: A framework for specifying compositional nonparametric bayesian models. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 19, 641–48.Google Scholar
Keshava, S., and Pitler, E.. 2006. A simpler, intuitive approach to morpheme induction. In Proceedings of The 2nd Pascal Challenges Workshop, 31–5.Google Scholar
Kuçera, H., and Francis, W. N.. 1967. Computational Analysis of Present-Day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1989. The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change 1.1, 8597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legate, J. A., and Yang, C.. 2007. Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition 14.3, 315–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lignos, C.; Chan, E., Marcus, M. P., and Yang, C.. 2009. A rule-based unsupervised morphology learning framework. In Working Notes of the 10th Workshop of the Cross-language Evaluation Forum (CLEF2009). Available online at www.clef-initiative.eu/edition/clef2009/working-notes (accessed May 1, 2016).Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. 2000. More overregularizations after all: New data and discussion on Marcus, Pinker, Ullman, Hollander, Rosen and Xu. Journal of Child Language 27.1, 183212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G.; Pinker, S., Ullman, M. T., Hollander, M., Rosen, J., and Xu, F.. 1992. Over-Regularization in Language Acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F. 1993. Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition 46.1, 5385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F.; Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., and Pinker, S.. 1995. German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology 29.3, 189256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, M. P.; Santorini, B., Marcinkiewicz, M. A., and Taylor, A.. 1999. Treebank-3. Linguistic Data Consortium: LDC99T42.Google Scholar
Maslen, R.; Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V., and Tomasello, M.. 2004. A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 47.6, 1319–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayol, L. 2007. Acquisition of irregular patterns in Spanish verbal morphology. In Nurmi, V. V. and Sustretov, D. (eds.), Proceedings of the Twelfth ESSLLI Student Session, 185–96.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., and Patterson, K.. 2002. Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6.11, 465–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monson, C. 2008. ParaMor: From Paradigm Structure to Natural Language Morphology Induction. Ph.D. thesis, Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
Mooney, R. J., and Califf, M. E.. 1995. Induction of first-order decision lists: Results on learning the past tense of English. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research 3.1, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J., and Stockall, L.. 2012. Early, equivalent ERP masked priming effects for regular and irregular morphology. Brain and language 123.2, 8193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, S. 1987. Vowel shortening in English. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5.4, 485518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naradowsky, J., and Goldwater, S.. 2009. Improving morphology induction by learning spelling rules. In Proceedings of the Twenty-first International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 1531–6. IJCAI Organization.Google Scholar
O’Donnell, T. 2011. Productivity and Reuse in Language. Ph.D. thesis, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Parkes, C.; Malek, A., and Marcus, M.. 1998. Towards unsupervised extraction of verb paradigms from large corpora. In Charniak, Eugene (ed.), Proceedings of the Sixth Workshop on Very Large Corpora, 110–17. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. 1995. Syntax at age two: Cross-linguistic differences. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 26, 325–82.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1989. Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1995. Why the child holded the baby rabbit: A case study in language acquisition. In Gleitman, L. R. and Liberman, M. (eds.), An Invitation to Cognitive Science, vol. 1: Language, 107–33. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., and Prince, A.. 1988. On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28.1, 73193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S., and Ullman, M. T.. 2002. The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Science 6.11, 456–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzuto, E., and Caselli, M. C.. 1994. The acquisition of Italian verb morphology in a cross-linguistic perspective. In Levy, Y. (ed.), Other Children, Other Languages: Issues in the Theory of Language Acquisition, 137–87. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., and Juola, P.. 1999. A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science 23.4, 463–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prince, A., and Smolensky, P.. 2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Real Academia Española 1992. Diccionario de la lengua española, 21st edn. Madrid: Real Academia Española.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L.. 1986. On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., and the PDP Research Group (eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations into the Microstructure of Cognition, vol. 2: Psychological and Biological Models, 216–71. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schütze, C. T. 2005. Thinking about what we are asking speakers to do. In Kepser, S. and Reis, M. (eds.), Linguistic Evidence: Empirical, Theoretical, and Computational Perspectives, 457–85. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Sims, A. D. 2006. Minding the Gap: Inflectional Defectiveness in a Paradigmatic Theory. Ph.D. thesis, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Spiegler, S.; Golénia, B., and Flach, P.. 2009. Promodes: A probabilistic generative model for word decomposition. In Working Notes of the 10th Workshop of the Cross-language Evaluation Forum (CLEF2009). Available online at www.clef-initiative.eu/edition/clef2009/working-notes (accessed May 2, 2016).Google Scholar
Stockall, L., and Marantz, A.. 2006. A single route, full decomposition model of morphological complexity: MEG evidence. The Mental Lexicon 1.1, 85123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J. R. 2003. Linguistic Categorization. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2000. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74.3, 209–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Westfal, S. 1956. A Study in Polish Morphology: The Genitive Singular Masculine. The Hague: Mouton and Co.Google Scholar
Weyerts, H., and Clahsen, H.. 1994. Netzwerke und symbolische Regeln im Spracher werb: Experimentelle Ergebnisse zur Entwicklung der Flexionsmorphologie. Linguistische Berichte 154, 430–60.Google Scholar
Wicentowski, R. 2002. Modeling and Learning Multilingual Inflectional Morphology in a Minimally Supervised Framework. Ph.D. dissertation, Johns Hopkins University.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. 1996. The Phonology of German. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Wiese, R. 1999. On default rules and other rules. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22.6, 1043–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, D. 1999. German noun plural reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 1044–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, F., and Pinker, S.. 1995. Weird past tense forms. Journal of Child Language 22.3, 531–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. 2002. Knowledge and Learning in Natural Language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, C. 2004. Universal grammar, statistics or both? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8.10, 451–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. 2005. On productivity. Linguistic Variation Yearbook 5, 333–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. 2013. Ontogeny and phylogeny of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110.16, 6324–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. 2016. The Price of Linguistic Productivity: How Children Learn to Break the Rules of Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, K., and Sussman, G. J.. 1997. Sparse representations for fast, one-shot learning. In Proceedings of the National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 521–7. AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Zhang, J., and Lai, Y.. 2010. Testing the role of phonetic knowledge in Mandarin tone sandhi. Phonology 27.1, 153201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zipf, G. K. 1949. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort: An Introduction to Human Ecology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar

References

Albright, A. 2002. Islands of reliability for regular morphology: Evidence from Italian. Language 78, 684709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albright, A., and Hayes, B.. 2003. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition 90, 119–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegre, M., and Gordon, P.. 1999. Frequency effects and the representational status of regular inflections. Journal of Memory and Language 40, 4161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Álvareza, C.; Urrutiaa, M., Domínguez, A., and Sánchez-Casas, R.. 2011. Processing inflectional and derivational morphology: Electrophysiological evidence from Spanish. Neuroscience Letters 490, 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, S. 1992. A-morphous Morphology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, M. 1994. Morphology by Itself: Stems and Inflectional Classes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M. 2012. Morphological stems: What William of Ockham really said. Word Structure 5, 2851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H., and Neijt, N.. 1997. Productivity in context: A case study of Dutch suffix. Linguistics 35, 565–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H.; Milin, P., Ðurdević, D. Filipović´, Hendrix, P., and Marelli, M.. 2011. An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review 118, 438–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, H.; Feldman, L., and Schreuder, R.. 2006. Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language 55, 290313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H.; Wurm, L., and Aycock, J.. 2007. Lexical dynamics for low-frequency complex words: A regression study across tasks and modalities. The Mental Lexicon 2, 419–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H.; McQueen, J., Dijstra, T., and Schreuder, R.. 2003. Frequency effects in regular inflectional morphology: Revisiting Dutch plurals. In Baayen, H. and Schreuder, R. (eds.), Morphological Structure in Language Processing, 355–90. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beedham, C. 1994. The role of consonants in marking strong verb conjugation in German and English. Folia Linguistica 28, 279–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berent, I.; Vaknin, V., and Marcus, G.. 2007. Roots, stems, and the universality of lexical representations: Evidence from Hebrew. Cognition 104, 254–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berko, J. G. 1958. The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 14, 150–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bien, H.; Baayen, H., and Levelt, W.. 2011. Frequency effects in the production of Dutch deverbal adjectives and inflected verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes 26, 683715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bittner, A., and Köpcke, K.-M.. 2007. Überlegungen zur Repräsentation grammatischen Wissens am Beispiel der Verbmorphologie des Deutschen. In Di Meola, C. (ed.), Perspektiven Zwei. Akten der 2. Tagung Deutsche Sprachwissenschaft in Italien, 315. Rome: Istituto Italiano di Studi Germanici.Google Scholar
Blevins, J. 2006. Word-based morphology. Journal of Linguistics 42, 531–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonami, O.; Boyé, G., Giraudo, H., and Voga, M.. 2008. Quels verbes sont réguliers en français? In Durand, J., Habert, B., and Laks, B. (eds.), Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française: CMLF’08, 1511–23. Paris: Institut de Linguistique Française.Google Scholar
Božić, M.; Tyler, L., Su, L., Wingfield, C., and Marslen-Wilson, W.. 2013. Neurobiological systems for lexical representation and analysis in English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, 1678–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Božić, M., and Marslen‐Wilson, W.. 2010. Neurocognitive contexts for morphological complexity: Dissociating inflection and derivation. Language and Linguistics Compass 4, 1063–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., and Moder, C.. 1983. Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59, 251–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., and Pardo, E.. 1981. On lexical and morphological conditioning of alternations: A nonce-probe experiment with Spanish verbs. Linguistics 19, 937–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., and Hopper, P.. 2001. Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carstairs, A. 1987. Allomorphy in Inflexion. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. 1997. The representation of participles in the German mental lexicon: Evidence for the dual-mechanism model. Yearbook of Morphology 1996, 73–96.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. 2006. Dual-mechanism morphology. In Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, vol. 4, 15. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H.; Eisenbeiss, S., Hadler, M., and Sonnenstuhl, I.. 2001a. The mental representation of inflected words: An experimental study of adjectives and verbs in German. Language 77, 510–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H.; Sonnenstuhl, I., Hadler, M., and Eisenbeiss, S.. 2001b. Morphological paradigms in language processing and language disorders. Transactions of the Philological Society 99, 247–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H.; Prüfert, P., Eisenbeiss, S., and Cholin, J.. 2002. Strong stems in the German mental lexicon: Evidence from child language acquisition and adult processing. In Kaufmann, I. and Stiebels, B. (eds.), More than Words: A Festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich, 91112. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H.; Sonnenstuhl, I., and Blevins, J.. 2003. Derivational morphology in the German mental lexicon: A dual-mechanism account. In Baayen, H. and Schreuder, R. (eds.), Morphological Structure in Language Processing, 125–55. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., and Neubauer, K.. 2010. Morphology, frequency, and the processing of derived words in native and non-native speakers. Lingua 120, 2627–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., and Ikemoto, Y.. 2012. The mental representation of derived words: An experimental study of -sa and -mi nominals in Japanese. The Mental Lexicon 7, 147–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., and Fleischhauer, E.. 2014. Morphological priming in child German. Journal of Child Language, 41, 1305–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., and Veríssimo, J.. 2015. Morphology in the mental lexicon: Experimental evidence from the acquisition and processing of different languages. In Guijarro-Fuentes, P. et al. (eds.), The Acquisition of Romance Languages. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Colombo, L., and Burani, C.. 2002. The influence of age of acquisition, root frequency, and context availability in processing nouns and verbs. Brain and Language 81, 398411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colombo, L.; Stoianov, I., Pasini, M., and Zorzi, M.. 2006. The role of phonology in the inflection of Italian verbs: A connectionist investigation. The Mental Lexicon 1, 147–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G., and Fraser, N.. 1993. Network Morphology: A DATR account of Russian nominal inflection. Journal of Linguistics 29, 113–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M.; van Casteren, M., and Marslen-Wilson, W.. 2003. Frequency effects in processing inflected Dutch nouns: A distributed connectionist account. In Baayen, H. and Schreuder, R. (eds.), Morphological Structure in Language Processing, 427–62. Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Eddington, D. 2002. Dissociation in Italian conjugations: A single-route account. Brain and Language 81, 291302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Embick, D., and Halle, M. 2005. On the status of stems in morphological theory. In Geerts, T., van Ginneken, I., and Jacobs, H. (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2003: Selected Papers from “Going Romance” Nijmegen, 3762. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, G., and Frisch, S.. 2006. Effects of processing difficulty on judgments of acceptability. In Fanselow, G., Féry, C., Schlesewsky, M., and Vogel, R. (eds.), Gradience in Grammar: Generative Perspectives, 291316. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L., and Weber, K.. 2012. Morphological processing: A comparison of graded and categorical accounts. In Adelman, J. (ed.), Visual Word Recognition: Meaning and Context, Individuals and Development, vol. 2, 223. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fiorentino, R.; Naito-Billen, Y., and Minai, U.. 2015. Morphological decomposition in Japanese de-adjectival nominals: Masked and overt priming evidence. To appear in: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research; doi 10.1007/s10936-015–9349-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fruchter, J.; Stockall, L., and Marantz, A.. 2013. MEG masked priming evidence for form-based decomposition of irregular verbs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, J., and O’Brien, J.. 2006. Learning inflectional classes. Language Learning and Development 2, 219–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonnerman, L.; Seidenberg, M., and Andersen, E. S.. 2007. Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136, 323–45.Google ScholarPubMed
Halle, M., and Marantz, A.. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, K. and Keyser, J. (eds.), The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, 111–76. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hare, M.; Elman, J., and Daugherty, K.. 1995. Default generalisation in connectionist networks. Language and Cognitive Processes 10, 601–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Havas, V.; Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and Clahsen, H.. 2012. Brain potentials for derivational morphology: An ERP study of deadjectival nominalizations in Spanish. Brain and Language 120, 332–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, G., Fleischhauer, E., and Clahsen, H.. 2013. Stem allomorphy and affixation in morphological processing: A cross-modal priming study with late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16, 924–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janssen, U.; Eisenbeiss, S., and Penke, M.. 2001. Agreement features in sentence processing: A sentence-matching study on subject-verb and NP-internal agreement in German. Poster presented at the 14th CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Järvikivi, J.; Bertram, R., and Niemi, J.. 2006. Affixal salience and the processing of derivational morphology: The role of suffix allomorphy. Language and Cognitive Processes 21, 394431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastovsky, D. 1986. The problem of productivity in word formation. Linguistics 24, 585600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kielar, A., and Joanisse, M.. 2011. The role of semantic and phonological factors in word recognition: An ERP cross-modal priming study of derivational morphology. Neuropsychologia 49, 161–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiparsky, P. 1998. Paradigm Effects and Opacity. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Kuperman, V.; Bertram, R., and Baayen, H.. 2010. Processing trade-offs in the reading of Dutch derived words. Journal of Memory and Language 62, 8397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K.. 2011. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential. Annual Review of Psychology 62, 621–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laudanna, A., and Burani, C.. 1995. Distributional properties of derivational affixes: Implications of processing. In Feldman, L. (ed.), Morphological Aspects of Language Processing, 345–64. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leminen, A., and Clahsen, H.. 2014. Brain potentials to inflected adjectives: Beyond storage and decomposition. Brain Research 1543, 223–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linares, R. E.; Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and Clahsen, H.. 2006. Stem allomorphy in the Spanish mental lexicon: Evidence from behavioral and ERP experiments. Brain and Language 97, 110–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. 2007. Morphological processes in language comprehension. In Gaskel, G. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 175–93. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.; Tyler, L., Waksler, R., and Older, L.. 1994. Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review 101, 333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, P. 1972. Morphology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Matushansky, O., and Marantz, A. (eds.) 2013. Distributed Morphology Today: Morphemes for Morris Halle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowell, K.; Kerick, S., Maria, D. Santa, and Hatfield, B. 2003. Aging, physical activity, and cognitive processing: An examination of P300. Neurobiology of Aging 24, 597606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milin, P.; Kuperman, V., Kostić, A., and Baayen, H.. 2009. Words and paradigms bit by bit: An information-theoretic to the processing of inflection and derivation. In Blevins, J. P. and Blevins, J. (eds.), Analogy in Grammar: Form and Acquisition 214–53. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moscoso del Prado Martín, F.; Kostić, A., and Baayen, H.. 2004. Putting the bits together: An information theoretical perspective on morphological processing. Cognition 94, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K., and Clahsen, H.. 2009. Decomposition of inflected words in a second language: An experimental study of German participles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31, 403–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K. 2010. The Processing of Inflection and Derivation in German as a Second Language. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Essex.Google Scholar
Opitz, A.; Regel, S., Müller, G., and Friederici, A.. 2013. Neurophysiological evidence for morphological underspecification in German strong adjective inflection. Language 89, 231–64.Google Scholar
Penke, M.; Janssen, U., and Eisenbeiss, S.. 2004. Psycholinguistic evidence for the underspecification of morphosyntactic features. Brain and Language 90, 423–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Plag, I. 2003. Word-formation in English. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plaut, D. 2011. Connectionist perspectives on lexical representation. In Gaskell, G. and Zwitserlood, P. (eds.), Lexical Representation: A Multidisciplinary Approach, 149–70. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Prasada, S., and Pinker, S.. 1993. Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raveh, M., and Rueckl, J.. 2000. Equivalent effects of inflected and derived primes: Long-term morphological priming in fragment completion and lexical decision. Journal of Memory and Language 42, 103–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raveh, M. 2002. The contribution of frequency and semantic similarity to morphological processing. Brain and Language 81, 312–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampson, G. 2007. Grammar without grammaticality. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 3, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Say, T., and Clahsen, H.. 2002. Words, rules and stems in the Italian mental lexicon. In Nooteboom, S., Weerman, F., and Wijnen, F. (eds.), Storage and Computation in the Language Faculty, 93129. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I.; Fernández, E., and Clahsen, H. (eds.) 2008. Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-line Methods in Children’s Language Processing. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E.; Zwitserlood, P., and Rösler, F.. 2007. Stem access in regular and irregular inflection: Evidence from German participles. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 325–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenstuhl, I., and Huth, A.. 2002. Processing and representation of German -n plurals: A dual mechanism approach. Brain and Language 81, 276–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenstuhl, I.; Eisenbeiss, S., and Clahsen, H.. 1999. Morphological priming and the mental lexicon: Evidence from German. Cognition 72, 203–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A.; Lehtonen, M., and Laine, M.. 2007. Word frequency and morphological processing in Finnish revisited. The Mental Lexicon 3, 359–85.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. 2013. Lexical Relatedness. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanners, R.; Neiser, J., Hernon, W., and Hall, R.. 1979. Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18, 399412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockall, L., and Marantz, A.. 2006. A single route, full decomposition model of morphological complexity: MEG evidence. The Mental Lexicon 1, 85123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stump, G. 2001. Inflectional Morphology: A Theory of Paradigm Structure. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M. 2004. Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 745–65.Google Scholar
Traficante, D., and Burani, C.. 2003. Visual processing of Italian verbs and adjectives: the role of the inflectional family size. In Baayen, H. and Schreuder, R. (eds.), Morphological Structure in Language Processing, 4564. Berlin: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trompelt, H.; Bordag, D., and Pechmann, T.. 2013. (Ir)regularity of verbs revisited: Evidence for lexical entry complexity. The Mental Lexicon 8, 2652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L.; Stamatakis, E., Post, B., Randall, B., and Marslen-Wilson, W.. 2005. Temporal and frontal systems in speech comprehension: An fMRI study of past tense processing. Neuropsychologia 43, 1963–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H., and Ullman, M.. 2001. Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 177217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Herten, M.; Chwilla, D., and Kolk, H.. 2006. When heuristics clash with parsing routines: ERP evidence for conflict monitoring in sentence perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 1181–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veríssimo, J., and Clahsen, H.. 2014. Variables and similarity in linguistic generalization: Evidence from inflectional classes in Portuguese. Journal of Memory and Language 76, 6179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, I. 1993. Verbs in Italian morphology. Yearbook of Morphology 1993, 219–54.Google Scholar
Walenski, M.; Mostofsky, S., and Ullman, M.. 2014. Inflectional morphology in high-functioning autism: Evidence for speeded grammatical processing. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8, 1607–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, G., and Ruh, N.. 2012. A neuroconstructivist model of past tense development and processing. Psychological Review 119, 649–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wunderlich, D. 1996. Minimalist morphology: The role of paradigms. Yearbook of Morphology 1995, 93–114.Google Scholar
Zwicky, A. 1985. How to describe inflection. Berkeley Linguistics Society 11, 372–86.Google Scholar

References

Alam, Yokiko Sasaki. 1983. A two-level morphological analysis of Japanese. Texas Linguistic Forum 22, 229–52.Google Scholar
Al-Najem, Salah R. 2007. Inheritance based approach to Arabic verbal root and pattern morphology. In Soudi, Abdelhadi, Neumann, Günter, and Van den Bosch, Antal (eds.), Arabic Computational Morphology: Knowledge-based and Empirical Methods, 6788. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barg, Petra. 1996. Automatic inference of DATR theories. In Bock, H.-H. and Polasek, W. (eds.), Data Analysis and Information Systems: Statistical and Conceptual Approaches, 506–15. Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Bird, Steven, and Klein, Ewen. 1990. Phonological Events. Journal of Linguistics 26.01, 3356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Dunstan. 1998. The General and the Exceptional in Russian Nominal Morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Surrey.Google Scholar
Brown, Dunstan, and Hippisley, Andrew. 1994. Conflict in Russian genitive plural assignment: A solution represented in DATR. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 2.1, 4876.Google Scholar
Brown, D., and Hippisley, Andrew. 2012. Network Morphology: A Defaults-based Theory of Word Structure. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckwalter, Tim. 2001. Arabic Transliteration. www.qamus.org/transliteration.htm (accessed June 21, 2015).Google Scholar
Buckwalter, Tim. 2004. Buckwalter Arabic Morphological Analyzer Version 2.0 LDC2004L02. Web Download. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Cahill, Lynne 2007. A syllable-based account of Arabic morphology. In Soudi, Abdelhadi, Neumann, Günter, and Van den Bosch, Antal (eds.), Arabic Computational Morphology: Knowledge-based and Empirical Methods, 4566. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, Lynne. 2010. A syllable-based approach to verbal morphology in Arabic. In LREC10 Workshop on Semitic Languages, 1926. Malta: European Language Resources Association (ELRA).Google Scholar
Cahill, Lynne, and Gazdar, Gerald. 1999. German noun inflection. Journal of Linguistics 35.1, 142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, Violetta, and Soudi, Abdelhadi. 2007. Arabic Computational Morphology: A trade-off between multiple operations and multiple stems. In Soudi, Abdelhadi, Neumann, Günter, and Van den Bosch, Antal (eds.), Arabic Computational Morphology: Knowledge-based and Empirical Methods, 89114. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic Structures, The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1970. Remarks on nominalization. In Jacobs, R. and Rosenbaum, P. (eds.), Readings in English Transformational Grammar, 184221. Waltham, MA: Ginn.Google Scholar
Clark, Alex. 2002. Memory-based learning of morphology with stochastic transducers. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 513–20. Morristown, NJ: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Cunningham, Hamish; Maynard, Diana, Bontcheva, Kalina, Tablan, Valentin, Aswani, Niraj, Roberts, Ian, Gorrell, Genevieve, Funk, Adam, Roberts, Angus, Damljanovic, Danica, Heitz, Thomas, Greenwood, Mark A., Saggion, Horacio, Petrak, Johann, Li, Yaoyong, and Peters, Wim. 2011. Text Processing with GATE (Version 6). University of Sheffield Department of Computer Science.Google Scholar
De Pauw, Gu; De Schryver, Gilles-Maurice, and van de Loo, Janneke. 2012. Resource-light Bantu part-of-speech tagging. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Language Technology for Normalisation of Less-resourced Languages (SaLTMiL 8—AfLaT 2012), 8592. European Language Resources Association (ELRA).Google Scholar
Dichy, Joseph, and Farghaly, Ali. 2007. Grammar-lexis relations in the computational morphology of Arabic. In Soudi, Abdelhadi, Neumann, Günter, and Van den Bosch, Antal (eds.), Arabic Computational Morphology: Knowledge-based and Empirical Methods, 115–40. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Springer.Google Scholar
Elwell, Robert. 2008. Finite State methods for Bantu morphology. In Gaylord, Nicholas, Hilderbrand, Stephen, Lyu, Heeyoung, Palmer, Alexis, and Ponvert, Elias (eds.), Texas Linguistics Society 10: Computational Linguistics for Less-studied Languages. Standford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Evans, Roger. 2013. The extended lexicon: Language processing as lexical description In Proceedings of the International Conference Recent Advances in Natural Language Processing (RANLP), Hissar, Bulgaria, September 7–13. INCOMA.Google Scholar
Evans, Roger, and Gazdar, Gerald. 1996. DATR: A language for lexical knowledge representation. Computational Linguistics 22.2, 167216.Google Scholar
Evans, Roger; Piwek, Paul, Cahill, Lynne, and Tipper, Neil. 2008. Natural language processing in CLIME, a multilingual legal advisory system. Natural Language Engineering 14, 101–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, Norman, and Corbett, Greville. 1997. Defaults in Arapesh. Lingua 103, 2557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaizauskas, Robert; Cahill, Lynne J., and Evans, Roger. 1992. POETIC: A system for gathering and disseminating traffic information. Proceedings of the International Conference on Artificial Intelligence Applications in Transportation Engineering, San Buenaventura, California, June. Irvine, CA: Institute of Transportation Studies.Google Scholar
Garside, R., and Smith, N. 1997. A hybrid grammatical tagger: CLAWS4. In Garside, R., Leech, G., and McEnery, A. (eds.), Corpus Annotation: Linguistic Information from Computer Text Corpora, 102–21. London: Longman.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, John. 2000. Linguistica: An automatic morphological analyser. In Okrent, Arika and Boyle, John (eds.), Proceedings from the Main Session of the Chicago Linguistic Society’s 36th Meeting, 125–39. Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John. 2001. Unsupervised learning of the morphology of a natural language. Computational Linguistics 27, 153–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, John. 2006. An algorithm for the unsupervised learning of morphology. Natural Language Engineering 12.4, 353–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grishman, Ralph, and Sundheim, Beth. 1996. Message Understanding Conference - 6: A Brief History. In Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Computational Linguistics (COLING), Copenhagen, Denmark, 466–71. International Committee on Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Habash, Nizar. 2010. An Introduction to Arabic Natural Language Processing. San Rafael: Morgan and Claypool.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hafer, Margaret A., and Weiss, Stephen F.. 1974. Word segmentation by letter successor varieties. Information Storage and Retrieval 10.11–12, 371–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Zellig. 1955. From phoneme to morpheme. Language 31.2, 190222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hausser, R. 1996. Linguistische Verifikation: Dokumentation zur Ersten Morpholympics. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Herring, Jon 2006. Orthography in Inheritance Lexicons. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Brighton.Google Scholar
Hurskainen, Arvi. 1992. A two-level computer formalism for the analysis of Bantu morphology: An application to Swahili. Nordic Journal of African Studies 1.1, 87119.Google Scholar
Kaplan, Ron, and Kay, Martin. 1981. Phonological rules and finite state transducers. Paper read at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America in New York City.Google Scholar
Karttunen, Lauri, and Wittenburg, Kent. 1983. A two-level analysis of English. Texas Linguistic Forum 22, 217–28.Google Scholar
Kartunnen, Lauri; Koskenniemi, Kimmo, and Kaplan, Ron. 1987. A compiler for two-level phonological rules. In Dalrymple, Mary, Kaplan, Ronald M., Karttunen, Lauri, Koskenniemi, Kimmo, Shaio, Sami, and Wescoat, Michael T. (eds.), CSLI Report No. 87-108, Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Kay, Martin. 1987. Non-concatenative finite-state morphology. In Proceedings of 3rd Conference of The European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (EACL), Copenhagen, Denmark, 210. International Committee on Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Khaliq, Bilal, and Carroll, John 2013. Unsupervised induction of Arabic root and pattern lexicons using machine learning. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Recent Advances in Natural Language Processing (RANLP), Hissar, Bulgaria, 350–6. INCOMA.Google Scholar
Koskenniemi, Kimmo. 1983. A Two-level Morphological Processor. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Helsinki.Google Scholar
Kurimo, Mikko; Virpioja, Sami, Turunen, Ville, and Lagus, Krista. 2010. Morpho Challenge competition 2005–2010: Evaluations and results. In Proceedings of the 11th Meeting of the ACL-SIGMORPHON, ACL 2010, Uppsala, Sweden, 15 July, 8795. International Committee on Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Luengen, Harald 1992. A DATR Description of Turkish Noun Inflection. Unpublished paper. University of BielefeldGoogle Scholar
Lun, S. 1983. A two-level morphological analysis of French. Texas Linguistic Forum 22, 271–8.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 1981. A prosodic theory of non-concatenative morphology. Linguistic Inquiry 12, 373418.Google Scholar
Oliver, Antoni; Castellón, Irene, and Màrquez, Luís. 2003. Automatic lexical acquisition from raw corpora: An application to Russian. In Proceedings of the 2003 EACL Workshop on Morphological Processing of Slavic Languages, Budapest, Hungary, 1724. International Committee on Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Petrie, Helen; Jones, Sarah Rees, Power, Christopher, Evans, Roger, Cahill, Lynne, Knobbe, Arno, Gervers, Michael, Sutherland-Harris, Robin, Kosto, Adam, and Crump, Jon. 2013. ChartEx: A project to extract information from the content of medieval charters and create a virtual workbench for historians to work with this information. Paper presented at Digital Humanities 2013, Lincoln, Nebraska, July 2013.Google Scholar
Sagot, Benoît. 2007. Automatic acquisition of a Slovak lexicon from a raw corpus. In Matoušek, V. and Mautner, P. (eds.), Proceedings of Text, Speech and Dialogue, 156–63. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Sagot, Benoît. 2009. Building a morphosyntactic lexicon and a pre-syntactic processing chain for Polish. In Zygmunt Vetulani and Hans Huszkoreit (eds.), Language and Technology Conference, Poznań, Poland (2007), Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Human Language Techology: Challenges of the Information Society. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santos, Diana; Costa, Luís, and Rocha, Paulo. 2003. Cooperatively evaluating Portuguese morphology. In Mamede, Nuno J., Baptista, Jorge, Trancoso, Isabel, and Nunes, Maria das Graças Volpe (eds.), Computational Processing of the Portuguese Language, 6th International Workshop, PROPOR 2003, Faro, 26–27 June, 259–66. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Soudi, Abdelhadi; Neumann, Günter, and Van den Bosch, Antal (eds.) 2007. Arabic Computational Morphology: Knowledge-based and Empirical Methods. Dordrecht: Kluwer/Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, Andrew. 2000. Inflection and the lexeme. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 47. 1–4, 335–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sproat, Richard, and Emerson, Thomas. 2003. The First International Chinese Word Segmentation Bakeoff. Proceedings of the Second SIGHAN Workshop on Chinese Language Processing, Sapporo, Japan, 133–43. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Wong, Kam-Fai; Li, Wenji, Xu, Ruifeng, and Zhang, Zheng-sheng. 2009. Introduction to Chinese Natural Language Processing. San Rafael: Morgan and Claypool.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×