Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-05T18:04:25.527Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Multiple-Text Comprehension

from Part III - Reading and Writing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2019

John Dunlosky
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Katherine A. Rawson
Affiliation:
Kent State University, Ohio
Get access

Summary

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albrecht, J. E. & O’brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1061.Google Scholar
Anmarkrud, Ø., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2014). Multiple-documents literacy: Strategic processing, source awareness, and argumentation when reading multiple conflicting documents. Learning and Individual Differences, 30, 6476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.007Google Scholar
Banas, S. & Sanchez, C. A. (2012). Working memory capacity and learning underlying conceptual relationships across multiple documents. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(4), 594600.Google Scholar
Barzilai, S. & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2015). The role of epistemic perspectives in comprehension of multiple author viewpoints. Learning and Instruction, 36, 86103.Google Scholar
Barzilai, S., Tzadok, E., & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2015). Sourcing while reading divergent expert accounts: Pathways from views of knowing to written argumentation. Instructional Science, 43(6), 737.Google Scholar
Bazerman, C. (1985). Physicists reading physics: Schema-laden purposes and purpose-laden schema. Written Communication, 2, 323.Google Scholar
Braasch, J. L. G., Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., Anmarkrud, Ø., & Ferguson, L. E. (2013). Promoting secondary school students’ evaluation of source features of multiple documents. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 180195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.03.003Google Scholar
Braasch, J. L., McCabe, R. M., & Daniel, F. (2016). Content integration across multiple documents reduces memory for sources. Reading and Writing, 29(8), 15711598.Google Scholar
Braasch, J. L. G., Rouet, J.-F., Vibert, N., & Britt, M. A. (2012). Readers’ use of source information in text comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 40, 450465. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011–0160-6.Google Scholar
Bråten, I., Anmarkrud, Ø., Brandmo, C., & Strømsø, H. I. (2014). Developing and testing a model of direct and indirect relationships between individual differences, processing, and multiple-text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 30, 924.Google Scholar
Bråten, I., Ferguson, L., Anmarkrud, Ø., & Strømsø, H. (2013). Prediction of learning and comprehension when adolescents read multiple texts: The roles of word-level processing, strategic approach, and reading motivation. Reading and Writing, 26, 321348.Google Scholar
Bråten, I. & Strømsø, H. I. (2009). Effects of task instruction and personal epistemology on the understanding of multiple texts about climate change. Discourse Processes, 47(1), 131.Google Scholar
Bråten, I. & Strømsø, H. I. (2010). When law students read multiple documents about global warming: Examining the role of topic-specific beliefs about the nature of knowledge and knowing. Instructional Science, 38, 635657.Google Scholar
Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Britt, M. A. (2009). Trust matters: Examining the role of source evaluation in students’ construction of meaning within and across multiple texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 628.Google Scholar
Bråten, I., Strømsø, H., Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J.-F. (2011). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: Towards an integrated model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 4870.Google Scholar
Bråten, I., Strømsø, H., & Salmerón, L. (2010). Trust and mistrust when students read multiple information sources about climate change. Learning and Instruction, 21, 180192.Google Scholar
Brem, S. K., Russell, J., & Weems, L. (2001). Science on the Web: Students’ evaluation of scientific arguments. Discourse Processes, 32, 191213.Google Scholar
Britt, M. A. & Aglinskas, C. (2002). Improving students’ ability to identify and use source information. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 485522.Google Scholar
Britt, M. A., Perfetti, C. A., Sandak, R., & Rouet, J. F. (1999). Content integration and source separation in learning from multiple texts. In Goldman, S. R., Graesser, A. C., & van den Broek, P. (eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 209233). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J. -F. (2012). Learning with multiple documents: Component skills and their acquisition. In Lawson, M.J. and Kirby, J.R. (Eds.), Enhancing the Quality of Learning: Dispositions, Instruction, and Learning Processes, (pp. 276314). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Britt, M. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Durik, A. M. (2018). Literacy beyond text comprehension: A theory of purposeful reading. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Burton, C. & Daneman, M. (2007). Compensating for a limited working memory capacity during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Reading Psychology, 28(2), 163186.Google Scholar
De Pereyra, G., Britt, M. A., Braasch, J. L. G, & Rouet, J. F. (2014). Reader’s memory for information sources in simple news stories: Effects of text and task features. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 187204.Google Scholar
Gerjets, P., Kammerer, Y., & Werner, B. (2011). Measuring spontaneous and instructed evaluation processes during Web search: Integrating concurrent thinking-aloud protocols and eye-tracking data. Learning and Instruction, 21(2), 220231.Google Scholar
Gil, L., Bråten, I., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010). Summary versus argument tasks when working with multiple documents: Which is better for whom? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 157173.Google Scholar
Gil, L., Bråten, I., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010b). Understanding and integrating multiple science texts: Summary tasks are sometimes better than argument tasks. Reading Psychology, 31, 3068.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 6983.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. R., Braasch, J. L. G., Wiley, J., Graesser, A. C., & Brodowinska, K. (2012). Comprehending and learning from Internet sources: Processing patterns of better and poorer learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 356381. https://doi.org/10.1002/RRQ.027Google Scholar
Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., Britt, M. A., & Salas, C. R. (2012). The role of CLEAR thinking in learning science from multiple-document inquiry tasks. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 5, 6378.Google Scholar
Hagen, Å. M., Braasch, J. L., & Bråten, I. (2014). Relationships between spontaneous note‐taking, self‐reported strategies and comprehension when reading multiple texts in different task conditions. Journal of Research in Reading, 37, 141157.Google Scholar
Hofer, B. K. & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67(1), 88140.Google Scholar
Jurica, P. J. & Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Monitoring item and source information: Evidence for a negative generation effect in source memory. Memory and Cognition, 27(4), 648656.Google Scholar
Kammerer, Y., Amann, D., & Gerjets, P. (2015). When adults without university education search the Internet for health information: The roles of Internet-specific epistemic beliefs and a source evaluation intervention. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 297309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological review, 95(2), 163.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Young, S. R. (1984). Selective recall of decision-relevant information from texts. Memory and Cognition, 12(2), 112117.Google Scholar
Kopp, K. (2013). Selecting and using information from multiple documents for argumentation. Unpublished dissertation, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL. http://search.proquest.com/docview/1459216967.Google Scholar
Le Bigot, L. & Rouet, J.-F. (2007). The impact of presentation format, task assignment, and prior knowledge on students’ comprehension of multiple online documents. Journal of Literacy Research, 39, 445470.Google Scholar
Lelivrescolaire (2017). Habiter une métroploe d’un pays emergent: Mumbai (2/2) [to live in the capital of an emerging country: Mumbai (2/2)]. https://fr.calameo.com/read/000596729dc5e7968d39cGoogle Scholar
Linderholm, T., Therriault, D. J., & Kwon, H. (2014). Multiple science text processing: Building comprehension skills for college student readers. Reading Psychology, 35(4), 332356.Google Scholar
Macedo-Rouet, M., Braasch, J.G.L., Britt, M.A., & Rouet, J.-F. (2013). Teaching fourth and fifth graders to evaluate information sources during text comprehension. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 204226.Google Scholar
Maggioni, L. & Fox, E. (2009). Adolescents’ reading of multiple history texts: An interdisciplinary investigation of historical thinking, intertextual reading, and domain-specific epistemic beliefs. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Maier, J. & Richter, T. (2013). Text belief consistency effects in the comprehension of multiple texts with conflicting information. Cognition and Instruction, 31(2), 151175.Google Scholar
Mason, L., Ariasi, N., & Boldrin, A. (2011). Epistemic beliefs in action: Spontaneous reflections about knowledge and knowing during online information searching and their influence on learning. Learning and Instruction, 21, 137151. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.01.001Google Scholar
Mason, L., Junyent, A. A., & Tornatora, M. C. (2014). Epistemic evaluation and comprehension of web-source information on controversial science-related topics: Effects of a short-term instructional intervention. Computers and Education, 76, 143157.Google Scholar
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 143.Google Scholar
McNamara, D.S., O’Reilly, T., Best, R., & Ozuru, Y. (2006). Improving adolescent students’ reading comprehension with iSTART. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 34, 147171.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D., van den Broek, P., & Ruiz, A. (1999). The influence of reading purpose on inference generation and comprehension in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 3, 488496.Google Scholar
Nokes, J. D., Dole, J. A., & Hacker, D. J. (2007). Teaching high school students to use heuristics while reading historical texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 492.Google Scholar
Paris, S. G., Cross, D. R. & Lipson, M. Y. (1984). Informed strategies for learning: A program to improve children’s reading awareness and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 12391252.Google Scholar
Pieschl, S., Stahl, E., & Bromme, R. (2008). Epistemological beliefs and self-regulated learning with hypertext. Metacognition and Learning, 3, 1737.Google Scholar
Pérez, A., Potocki, A., Stadtler, M., Macedo-Rouet, M., Paul, J., Salmerón, L., & Rouet, J-F. (2018). Fostering Teenagers’ Assessment of Information Reliability: Effects of a Classroom Intervention focused on Critical Source Dimensions. Learning and Instruction, 58, 5364.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Towards a theory of documents representation. In van Oostendorp, H. & Goldman, S. (eds.) The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 99122). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F. & Britt, M. A. (2011). Relevance processes in multiple document comprehension. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Text relevance and learning from text (pp. 1952). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F. & Britt, M. A. (2014). Learning from multiple documents. In Mayer, R.E. (ed.) Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning, 2nd edn (pp. 813841). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139547369.039Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Britt, M. A., & Durik, A. (2017). RESOLV: Readers’ representation of reading: Contexts and tasks. Educational Psychologist, 52(3), 200215. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1329015Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Britt, M. A., Mason, R. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using multiple sources of evidence to reason about history. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 478493.Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Favart, M., Britt, M. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Studying and using multiple documents in history: Effects of discipline expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 85106.Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Le Bigot, L., de Pereyra, G., & Britt, M. A. (2016). Whose story is this? Discrepancy triggers readers’ attention to source information in short narratives. Reading and Writing, 29, 15491570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016–9625-0Google Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Levonen, J., Dillon, A. P. and Spiro, R. J. (eds.) (1996). Hypertext and cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Salmerón, L., Macedo-Rouet, M., & Rouet, J.-F. (2016). Multiple viewpoints increase students’ attention to source features in social question and answer forum messages. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology. 67, 24042419. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.23585Google Scholar
Shanahan, C., Shanahan, T., & Misischia, C. (2011). Analysis of expert readers in three disciplines: History, mathematics, and chemistry. Journal of Literacy Research, 43(4), 393429.Google Scholar
Spörer, N., Brunstein, J. C., & Kieschke, U. (2009). Improving students’ reading comprehension skills: Effects of strategy instruction and reciprocal teaching. Learning and Instruction, 19, 272286.Google Scholar
Stadtler, M. & Bromme, R. (2007). Dealing with multiple documents on the WWW: The role of metacognition in the formation of documents models. International Journal of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning, 2, 191210.Google Scholar
Stadtler, M., Paul, J., Globoschütz, S., & Bromme, R. (2015). Watch out! – An instruction raising students’ epistemic vigilance augments their sourcing activities. In: Noelle, D. C., Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., Yoshimi, J., Matlock, T., Jennings, C. D., & Maglio, P. P. (eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 22782283). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B., & Bromme, R. (2013). Dealing with uncertainty: Readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as function of presentation format and source expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 130150.Google Scholar
Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Macedo-Rouet, M., Rouet, J. F., & Bromme, R. (2016). Improving vocational students’ consideration of source information when deciding about science controversies. Reading and Writing, 29(4), 705.Google Scholar
Stahl, S. A., Hynd, C. R., Britton, B. K., McNish, M. M. & Bosquet, D. (1996). What happens when students read multiple source documents in history? Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 4, 430456.Google Scholar
Strømsø, H. I. & Bråten, I. (2009). Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and multiple-text comprehension among upper secondary students. Educational Psychology, 29, 425445.Google Scholar
Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Britt, M.A. (2010). Reading multiple texts about climate change: The relationship between memory for sources and text comprehension. Learning and Instruction, 20,192204.Google Scholar
Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., & Ferguson, L. E. (2013). Spontaneous sourcing among students reading multiple documents. Cognition and Instruction, 31, 176203. doi:10.1080/07370008.2013.769994Google Scholar
Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2008). Dimensions of topic-specific epistemological beliefs as predictors of multiple-text understanding. Learning and Instruction, 18, 513527. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.11.001.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T. & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612630.Google Scholar
Turner, K. & Greene, E. (1977). The construction and use of a propositional text base. Technical report No. 63, University of Colorado.Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
VanSledright, B. A. & Kelly, C. (1998). Reading American history: The influence of multiple sources on six fifth graders. The Elementary School Journal, 98(3), 239265.Google Scholar
Voss, J.-F. & Wiley, J. (1997). Developing understanding while writing essays in history. International Journal of Educational Research, 27, 255265.Google Scholar
Walczyk, J. J., Kelly, K. E., Meche, S. D., & Braud, H. (1999). Time limitations enhance reading comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 156165.Google Scholar
Walraven, A., Brand-Gruwel, S., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2013). Fostering students’ evaluation behavior while searching the Internet. Instructional Science, 41, 125146.Google Scholar
Wiley, J., Goldman, S., Graesser, A., Sanchez., C., Ash, I. & Hemmerich, J. (2009) Source evaluation, comprehension, and learning in internet science inquiry tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 46, 10601106. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0002831209333183Google Scholar
Wiley, J. & Voss, J. F. (1996). The effects of “playing historian” on learning in history. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(7), 6372.Google Scholar
Wiley, J. & Voss, J. F. (1999). Constructing arguments from multiple sources: Tasks that promote understanding not just memory for text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 301311.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. J. & Sherrell, D. L. (1993). Source effects in communication and persuasion research: A meta-analysis of effect size. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 21, 101112. 10.1007/BF02894421Google Scholar
Wineburg, S. S. (1991). Historical problem solving: A study of the cognitive processes used in the evaluation of documentary and pictorial evidence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 7387.Google Scholar
Wineburg, S. S. (1994). The cognitive representation of historical texts. In Leinhardt, G., Beck, I. & Stainton, C. (eds.) Teaching and learning in history (pp. 85135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wolfe, M. B. & Griffin, T. D. (2017). Beliefs and discourse processing. In Schober, M. F., Rapp, D. N., & Britt, M. A. (eds.), Handbook of discourse processes, 2nd edn (pp. 295314). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×