Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
  • Online publication date: July 2015

Part VII - Individual and Population Differences

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Cambridge Handbook of Applied Perception Research
  • Online ISBN: 9780511973017
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511973017
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×

References

Anderson, D. R., Choi, H. P. and Lorch, E. P. (1987). Attentional inertia reduces distractibility during young children’s TV viewing. Child Development, 58, 798–806.
Anderson, D. R. and Levin, S. (1976). Young children’s attention to Sesame Street. Child Development, 47, 806–811.
Anderson, D. R., Lorch, E. P., Field, D. E. and Sanders, J. (1981). The effect of television program comprehensibility on preschool children’s visual attention to television. Child Development, 52, 151–157.
Aylward, G. P., Brager, P. and Harper, D. C. (2002). Relations between visual and auditory continuous performance tests in a clinical population: A descriptive study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 285–303.
Ballard, J. C. (1996). Computerized assessment of sustained attention: A review of factors affecting vigilance performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18(6), 843–863.
Baron, I. S. (2004). Neurological evaluation of the child. New York: Oxford University Press.
Beatty, J. (1982). Phasic not tonic papillary responses vary with auditory vigilance performance. Psychophysiology, 19, 167–172.
Beatty, J. and Kahneman, D. (1966). Pupillary changes in two memory tasks. Psychonomic Science, 5, 371–372.
Berg, K. M., Berg, W. K. and Graham F. K. (1971). Infant heart rate response as a function of stimulus and state. Psychophysiology, 8, 30–44.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw Hill.
Betts, J., Mckay, J., Maruff, P. and Anderson, V. (2006). The development of sustained attention in children: The effect of age and task load. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3), 205–221.
Booth, J. E., Carlson, C. L. and Tucker, D. T. (2007). Performance on a neurocognitive measure of alerting differentiates the ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: A preliminary report. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 423–432.
Borgaro, S., Pogge, D. L., DeLuca, V. A., Bilginer, L., Stokes, J. and Harvey, P. D. (2003). Convergence of different versions of the continuous performance test: Clinical and scientific implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 283–292.
Braet, W., Johnson, K. A., Tobin, C. T., Acheson, R., Bellgrove, M. A., Robertson, I. H. and Garavan, H. (2009). Functional developmental changes underlying response inhibition and error-detection processes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3143–3151.
Brez, C. C. and Colombo, J. (2012). Your eyes say “No,” but your heart says “Yes”: Behavioral and psychophysiological indices in infant quantitative processing. Infancy, 17(4), 445–454.
Brodeur, D. A. (1990). Covert orienting in young children. In J. T. Enns (Ed.), The development of attention (pp. 211–226). North Holland: Elsevier.
Buckner, D. N. and McGrath, J. J. (1963). Vigilance: A symposium. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chee, P., Logan, G., Schachar, R., Lindsay, P. and Wachsmuth, R. (1989). Effects of event rate and display time on sustained attention in hyperactive, normal, and control children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17(4), 371–391.
Choi, H. P. and Anderson, D. R. (1991). A temporal analysis of toy play and distractibility in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52, 41–69.
Clifton, R. (1972). Heart rate conditioning in newborn infant. Psychophysiology, 9, 272–312.
Colombo, J. (1993). Infant cognition: Predicting later intellectual functioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337–367.
Colombo, J. (2002). Infant attention grows up: The emergence of a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 196–200.
Colombo, J., Frick, J. E. and Gorman, S. A. (1997). Sensitization during visual habituation sequences: Procedural effects and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 223–235.
Colombo, J., Shaddy, D. J., Richman, W. A., Maikranz, J. M. and Blaga, O. (2004). Developmental course of visual habituation preschool cognitive language outcome. Infancy, 5, 1–38.
Conners, C. K. (2000). Conners’ continuous performance test II: Technical guide. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Conners, C. K., Epstein, J. N., Angold, A. and Klaric, J. (2002). Continuous performance test performance in a normative epidemiological sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 555–562.
Cooley, E. L. and Morris, R. D. (1990). Attention in children: A neuropsychologically based model for assessment. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 239–274.
Corkum, P. V., Byrne, J. M. and Ellsworth, C. (1995). Clinical assessment of sustained attention in preschoolers. Neuropsychology, 1(1), 3–18.
Corkum, P. V. and Siegel, L. S. (1993). Is the continuous performance task a valuable research tool for use with children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity-disorder? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 34, 1217–1239.
Courage, M. L., Reynolds, G. D. and Richards, J. E. (2006). Infants’ attention to patterned stimuli: Developmental change from 3 to 12 months of age. Child Development, 77, 680–695.
Craig, A. (1984). Human engineering: The control of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 247–291). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Curtindale, L. M., Laurie-Rose, C., Bennett-Murphy, L. and Hull, S. (2007). Sensory modality, temperament, and the development of sustained attention: A vigilance study in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 43, 576–589.
Danis, A., Pecheux, M. G., Lefevre, C., Bourdais, C. and Serres-Ruel, J. (2008). A continuous performance task in preschool children: Relations between attention and performance. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 401–418.
Davies, D. R. and Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press.
Davies, D. R., Jones, D. M. and Taylor, A. (1984). Selective- and sustained-attention tasks: Individual and group differences. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 395–447). New York: Academic Press.
Dember, W. N. and Warm, J. S. (1979). The psychology of perception (2nd ed.), New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Enns, J. T., Brodeur, D. A. and Trick, L. M. (1998). Selective attention over the lifespan: Behavioral measures. In J. E. Richards (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention: A developmental perspective (pp. 393–418). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Enns, J. T. and Burack, J. A. (1997). Attention, development, and psychopathology: Bridging disciplines. In J. A. Burack and J. T. Enns (Eds.), Attention, development, and psychopathology (pp. 3–28). New York: Guilford Press.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A. and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.
Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146, 668–670.
Freeseman, L. J., Colombo, J. and Coldren, J. T. (1993). Individual differences in infant visual attention: Four-month-olds’ discrimination and generalization of global and local stimulus properties. Child Development, 64, 1191–1203.
Frick, J. E. and Richards, J. E. (2001). Individual differences in infants’ recognition of briefly presented visual stimuli. Infancy, 2, 331–352.
Friedman, S. (1972). Habituation and recovery of visual response in the alert human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 13, 339–349.
Gordon M. (1991). Instruction manual for the Gordon Diagnostic System, Model III-R. Dewitt, NY: Gordon Systems.
Greenberg, L. M. and Waldman, I. D. (1993). Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (T.O.V.A.). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1019–1030.
Groves, P. M. and Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psychological Review, 77, 419–450.
Halperin, J. M. (1996) Conceptualizing, describing, and measuring components of attention. In G. R. Lyon and N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory and executive function. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Hancock, P. A. (1984). Environmental stressors. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 103–142). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Hancock, P. A. and Szalma, J. L. (2014). Sex differences in sustained attention. Unpublished Manuscript.
Hockey, G. R. J. (1984). Varieties of attentional states. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 449–483). New York: Academic Press.
Horowitz, F. D., Paden, L., Bhana, K. and Self, P. (1972). An infant-control procedure for studying infant visual fixations. Developmental Psychology, 7, 90.
Hrabok, M., Kerns, K. and Müller, U. (2007). The vigilance, orienting, and executive attention networks in 4-year-old children. Child Neuropsychology, 13(5), 408–421.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.
Kannass, K. N. and Oakes, L. M. (2008). The development of attention and its relationship to language in infancy and toddlerhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9, 222–246. DOI: 10.1080/15248370802022696.
Karatekin, C, Marcus, J. D. and Couperus, W. J. (2007). Regulation of cognitive resources during sustained attention and working memory in 10-year-olds and adults. Psychophysiology, 44, 128–144.
Kirmizi-Alsan, E., Bayraktaroglu, Z., Gurvit, H., Keskin, Y. H., Emre, M. and Demiralp, T. (2006). Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: Decomposition of electrophysiological markers of response inhibition and sustained attention. Brain Research, 1104, 114–128.
Klein, C., Wendling, K., Huettner, P., Ruder, H. and Peper, M. (2006). Intra-subject variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1088–1097.
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M. and Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3-to-12-year old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 407–428.
Klimkeit, E. I., Mattingley, J. B., Sheppard, D. M., Farrow, M. and Bradshaw, J. L. (2004). Examining the development of attention and executive functions in children with a novel paradigm. Child Neuropsychology, 10(3), 201–211.
Laurie-Rose, C., Bennett-Murphy, L., Byard, L. and Nikzad, K. (2002). The role of the big five personality factors in vigilance performance and workload. European Journal of Personality, 16, 185–200.
Laurie-Rose, C., Bennett-Murphy, L., Curtindale, L. M., Granger, A. L. and Walker, H. B. (2005). Equating tasks and sustaining attention in children and adults: The methodological and theoretical utility of d’ matching. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 254–263.
Laurie-Rose, C., Bennett Murphy, L., Schickedantz, B. and Tucci, J. (2001). The effects of event rate and signal probability on children’s vigilance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 215–224.
Laurie-Rose, C, Frey, M. and Bowling, A. (2014). The modulating effects of event rate and temperament in sustained attention: A test of optimal stimulation theory. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Laurie-Rose, C., Frey, M., Ennis, A. and Zamary, A. (2014). Measuring perceived mental workload in children. American Journal of Psychology, 127, 107–125.
Lehman, E. B., Olson, V. A., Aquilino, S. A. and Hall, L. C. (2006). Auditory and visual continuous performance tests: Relationships with age, gender, cognitive functioning, and classroom behavior. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24(1), 36–51.
Levy, F. (1980). The development of sustained attention (vigilance) and inhibition in children: Some normative data. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 77–84.
Lewis, M. (1969). Infants’ responses to facial stimuli during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 1, 75–86.
Lin, C. C. H., Hsiao, C. K. and Chen, W. J. (1999). Development of sustained attention assessed using the continuous performance test among children 6–15 years of age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 403–412.
Lorch, E. P. and Castle, V. J. (1997). Preschool children’s attention to television: Visual attention and probe response times. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 111–127.
Losier, B. J., McGrath, P. J. and Klein, R. M. (1996). Error patterns on the continuous performance test in non-medicated and medicated samples of children with and without ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Sciences, 37, 971–987.
MacDonald, S. W. S., Hultsch, D. F. and Bunce, D. (2006). Intraindividual variability in vigilance performance: Does degrading visual stimuli mimic age-related “neural noise”? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 655–675.
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P. and Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1065–1081.
Marrocco, R. T. and Davidson, M. C. (1998). The neurochemistry of attention. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 35–50). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McKay, K., Halperin, J. M., Schwartz, S. T. and Sharma, V. (1994). Developmental analysis of three aspects of information processing: Sustained attention, selective attention, and response organization. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 121–132.
Noonan, T. K., Ash, D. W., Loeb, M. and Warm, J. S. (1985). Task complexity, subject sex, and noise meaningfulness as determinants of cognitive vigilance performance. In R. E. Eberts and C. G. Eberts (Eds.), Trends in ergonomics/human factors II. (pp. 181–187). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Oakes, L. M. and Tellinghuisen, D. J. (1994). Examining in infancy: Does it reflect active processing? Developmental Psychology, 30, 748–756.
Pancratz, C. N. and Cohen, L. B. (1970). Recovery of habituation in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9, 208–216.
Parasuraman, R. (1984). The psychobiology of sustained attention. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 61–101). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Parasuraman, R. and Davies, D. R. (1977). A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance, and physiological correlates (pp. 559–574). New York: Plenum.
Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1987). Vigilance: Taxonomy and utility. In L. S. Mark, J. S. Warm and R. L. Huston (Eds.), Ergonomics and human factors: Recent research (pp. 11–32). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Pascualvaca, D. M., Anthony, B. J., Arnold, L. E., Rebok, G. W., Ahearn, M. B., Kellam, S. G. and Mirsky, A. F. (1997). Attention performance in an epidemiological sample of urban children: The role of gender and verbal intelligence. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 13–27.
Pempek, T. A., Kirkorian, H. L., Richards, J. E., Anderson, D. R., Lund, A. F. and Stevens, M. (2010). Video comprehensibility and attention in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1283–1293.
Posner, M. I. and Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23.
Prather, P. A., Sarmento, N. and Alexander, A. (1995). Development of vigilance in preschoolers. Journal of International Neuropsychology, 1, 153.
Rebok, G. W., Smith, C. B., Pascualvaca, D. M., Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J. and Kellam, S. G. (1997). Developmental changes in attentional performance in urban children from 8 to 13 years. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 28–46.
Reynolds, G. D. and Richards, J. E. (2007). Infant heart rate: A developmental psychophysiological perspective. In L. A. Schmidt and S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Developmental psychophysiology: Theory, systems, and methods (pp. 173–212). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Richards, J. E. (2008). Attention in young infants: A developmental psychophysiological perspective. In C. A. Nelson and M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 479–497). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Richards, J. E. (2010). The development of attention to simple and complex visual stimuli in infants: Behavioral and psychophysiological measures. Developmental Review, 30, 203–219.
Richards, J. E. and Casey, B. J. (1991). Heart-rate variability during attention phases in young infants. Psychophysiology, 28, 43–53.
Richards, J. E. and Casey, B. J. (1992). Development of sustained visual attention in the human infant. In B. A. Campbell, H. Hayne and R. Richardson (Eds.), Attention and information processing in infants and adults: Perspectives from human and animal research (pp. 30–60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Richards, J. E. and Cronise, K. (2000). Extended visual fixation in the early preschool years: Look duration, heart rate changes, and attentional inertia. Child Development, 71, 602–620.
Richards, J. E., Reynolds, G. D. and Courage, M. I. (2010). The neural bases of infant attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 41–16.
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D. and Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343–350.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halperin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P. and Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attention during childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I. and Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 573–594.
Ruff, H. A. and Capozzoli, M. C. (2003). Development of attention and distractibility in the first 4 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 39, 877–890. DOI: 10.1037/0012–1649.39.5.877.
Ruff, H. A. and Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford University Press.
Saayman, G., Ames, E. W. and Moffett, A. (1964). Response to novelty as an indicator of visual discrimination in the human infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1, 189–198.
See, J. E., Howe, S. R., Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1995). Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230–249.
Segalowitz, S. J. and Davies, P. L. (2004). Charting the maturation of the frontal lobe: An electrophysiological strategy. Brain and Cognition, 55, 116–133.
Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L. and Jetha, M. K. (2010). Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: A review. Brain and Cognition, 72, 86–100.
Seidel, W. T. and Joschko, M. (1990). Evidence of difficulties in sustained attention in children with ADDH. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 217–229.
Shaddy, D. J. and Colombo, J. (2004). Developmental changes in infant attention to dynamic and static stimuli. Infancy, 5, 355–365.
Shaw, T. H., Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V. S., Silverman, L. and Costa, P. T. (2010). Individual differences in vigilance: Personality, ability and states of stress. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 297–308.
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: MacMillan.
Steele, A., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Cornish, K. and Scerif, G. (2012). The multiple subfunctions of attention: Differential development gateways to literacy and numeracy. Child Development, 83, 2028–2041.
Szalma, J. L., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Weiler, E. M., Meier, A. and Eggemeier, F. T. (2004). Effects of sensory modality and task duration on performance, workload, and stress in sustained attention. Human Factors, 46, 219–233.
Temple, J., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., Jones, K., LaGrange, C. M. and Matthews, G. (2000). The effects of signal salience and caffeine on performance, workload, and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Human Factors, 42, 183–194.
Thompson, R. F. and Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 73, 16–43.
Tolin, P. and Fisher, P. G. (1974). Sex differences and effects of irrelevant auditory stimulation on performance of a visual vigilance task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1255–1262.
van der Meere, J., Shalev, R., Borger, N. and Gross-Tsur, V. (1995). Sustained attention, activation, and MPH in ADHD: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36(4), 697–703.
van der Meere, M., Beyer, R., Horn, J., Foth, M., Bornemann, B., Ries, J., Wartenburger, I. (2010). Resource allocation and fluid intelligence: Insights from pupillometry. Psychophysiology, 47, 158–169.
Von Bargen, D. M. (1983). Infant heart-rate: A review of research and methodology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 115–149.
Warm, J. S. and Dember, W. N. (1998). Tests of vigilance taxonomy. In R. R. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick and J. S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 87–112). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N. and Hancock, P. A. (1996). Vigilance and workload in automated systems. In M. Mouloua and R. Parasuraman (Eds.), Automation and human performance: Theory and application (pp. 183–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Warm, J. S. and Jerison, H. J. (1984). The psychophysics of vigilance. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 15–20). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Westlye, L. T., Grydeland, H., Walhovd, K. B. and Fjell, A. M. (2011). Associations between regional cortical thickness and attentional networks as measured by the attention network test. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 345–356.
Zentall, S. S. and Zentall, T. R. (1983). Optimal stimulation: A model of disordered activity and performance in normal and deviant children. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 446–471.

References

Ak, E. and Koc¸ak, S. (2010). Coincidence-anticipation timing and reaction time in youth tennis and table tennis players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110(3), 879–887.
Alexander, J., Barham, P. and Black, I. (2002). Factors influencing the probability of an incident at junction: Results from an interactive driving simulator. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 34, 779–792.
Baker, S. P., Lamb, M. W., Grabowski, J. G., Rebok, G. and Li, G. (2001). Characteristics of general aviation crashes involving mature male and female pilots. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 72, 447–452.
Baldwin, R. O., Thor, D. H. and Wright, D. E. (1966). Sex differences in the sense of time: Failure to replicate a 1904 study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 22, 398.
Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A. and Zakay, D. (2000). Sex differences in duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Memory and Cognition, 28(8), 1333–1346.
Caird, J. K. and Hancock, P. A. (1994). The perception of arrival time for different oncoming vehicles at an intersection. Ecological Psychology, 6(2), 83–109.
Cameron, M. A., Baker, J., Peterson, M. and Braunsberger, K. (2003). The effects of music, wait-length evaluation, and mood on a low-cost wait experience. Journal of Business Research, 56, 421–430.
Chavez, B. R. (2003). Effects of stress and relaxation on time perception. Downloaded from http://handle.dtic.mil/100.2/ADA421211, 14 January 2011.
Chebat, J.-C., Salem, N. H., Poirier, J.-F. and Gélinas-Chebat, C. (2010). Reactions to waiting online by men and women. Psychological Reports, 106(3), 851–869.
Clarke, D. D., Forsyth, R. and Wright, R. (2005). A statistical profile of road accidents during cross-flow turns. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 721–730.
Delay, E. R. and Mathey, M. E. (1985). Effects of ambient noise on time estimation by humans. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61, 415–419.
Delay, E. R. and Richardson, M. A. (1981). Time estimation in humans: Effects of ambient illumination and sex. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 747–750.
DeLucia, P. R., Bleckley, M. K., Meyer, L. E. and Bush, J. M. (2003). Judgments about collision in younger and older drivers. Transportation Research Part F, 6, 63–80.
Diggles-Buckles, V. and Bassin, S. L. (1990). Knowledge of results, practice and sex: Anticipation of coincidence revisited. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 131–141.
Droit-Volet, S. and Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions color our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 504–513. DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008.
Ehrlich, R. (2005). The human brain’s algorithm for extrapolating motion, and its possible gender-dependence. Neuroscience Letters, 374, 38–42.
Eisler, A. D. and Eisler, H. (1994). Subjective time scaling: Influence of age, gender, and Type A and Type B behavior. Chronobiologia, 21(3–4), 185–200.
Espinosa-Fernández, L., Miró, E., Cano, M. and Buela-Casal, G. (2003). Age-related changes and gender differences in time estimation. Acta Psychologica, 112, 221–232.
Farjampour, P. and Hellström, Ǻ. (2000). Discrimination of tone durations across the menstrual cycle. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 1059–1070.
Foy, R. M. (2011). Ovarian hormones, aging and stress on hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95, 134–144. DOI:10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.003.
Fozard, J. L., Vercruyssen, M., Reynolds, S. L., Hancock, P. A. and Quilter, R. E. (1994). Age differences and changes in reaction time: The Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 49(4), 179–189.
Friedman, W. J. and Janssen, S. M. J. (2010). Aging and the speed of time. Acta Psychologica, 134, 130–141.
Gibbons, H. and Rammsayer, T. H. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of temporal generalization: Evidence for a two-process model of time perception. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 195–209.
Glicksohn, J. (2001). Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: A multiplicative function of apparent duration. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 1–25.
Glicksohn, J., Ohana, A. B., Dotan, T. B., Goldstein, A. and Donchin, O. (2009). Time production and EEG alpha revisited. NeuroQuantology, 7(1), 138–151.
Gutyrchik, E., Churan, J., Meindl, T., Bokde, A., L., W., von Bernewitz, H., Born, C., Reiser, M., Pöppel, E. and Wittmann, M. (2010). Functional neuroimaging of duration discrimination on two different time scales. Neuroscience Letters, 469, 411–415.
Hancock, P. A. (2001, November). Sex and age differences in time perception. Paper presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomics Society. Orlando, FL.
Hancock, P. A. (2011). Cognitive differences in the ways men and women perceive the dimension and duration of time. Contrasting Gaia and Chronos. Lewiston, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Hancock, P. A., Arthur, E. J., Chrysler, S. T. and Lee, J. (1994). The effects of sex, target duration, and illumination on the production of time intervals. Acta Psychologica, 86, 57–67.
Hancock, P. A., Kane, M. J., Scallen, S. and Albinson, C. B. (2002). Effects of gender and athletic participation on driving capability. International Journal of Occupational Safety and Ergonomics, 8(2), 281–292.
Hancock, P. A. and Manser, M. P. (1997). Time-to-contact: More than tau alone. Ecological Psychology, 9(4), 265–297.
Hancock, P. A. and Rausch, R. (2010). The effects of sex, age and interval duration on the perception of time. Acta Psychologica, 133, 170–179.
Hancock, P. A., Vercruyssen, M. and Rodenburg, G. J. (1992). The effect of gender and time-of-day on time perception and mental workload. Current Psychology: Research and Reviews, 11, 203–225.
Hancock, P. A. and Weaver, J. L. (2005). On time distortion under stress. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 6, 193–211.
Herlitz, A., Airaksinen, E. and Nordström, E. (1999). Sex differences in episodic memory: The impact of verbal and visuospatial ability. Neuropsychology, 13, 590–597.
Hilbrecht, M., Zuzanek, J. and Mannell, R. C. (2008). Time use, time pressure and gendered behavior in early and late adolescence. Sex Roles, 58, 342–357.
International Civil Aviation Organization, ICAO (2006). Airborne collision avoidance system (ACAS) manual. Doc 9863 AN/461.
Joubert, C. E. (1983). Subjective acceleration of time: Death anxiety and sex differences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 49–50.
Kalb, R., Jansen, S., Reulbach, U. and Kalb, S. (2004). Sex differences in simple reaction tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98, 739–802.
Kellaris, J. J. and Mantel, S. P. (1994). The influence of mood and gender on consumers’ time perceptions. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 514–518.
Koglbauer, I., Braunstingl, R., Haberkorn, T. Prehofer, B. (2012). How do pilots interpret and react to traffic display indications in VFR flight? Proceedings of the 30th EAAP Conference, pp. 227–231.
Kumar, P. (2005). The competitive impact of service process improvement: Examining customers’ waiting experiences in retail markets. Journal of Retailing, 3, 171–180.
Levitt, L. and Leventhal, G. (1978). Effect of density and environmental noise on perception of time, the situation, oneself and others. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 999–1009.
Lewin, C., Wolgers, G. and Herlitz, A. (2001). Sex differences favouring women in verbal but not in visuospatial episodic memory. Neuropsychology, 15, 165–173.
Lobjois, R. and Cavallo, V. (2007). Age-related differences in street-crossing decisions: The effects of vehicle speed and time constraints on gap selection in an estimation task. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 39, 934–943.
Lobjois, R. and Cavallo, V. (2009). The effects of aging on street-crossing behavior: From estimation to actual crossing. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41, 259–267.
MacDougall, R. (1904). Sex differences in the sense of time. Science, 19, 707–708.
Manrai, L. A. and Manrai, A. K. (1995). Effects of cultural-context, gender, and acculturation on perceptions of work versus social/leisure time usage. Journal of Business Research, 32, 115–128.
Manser, M. P. and Hancock, P. A. (1996). Influence of approach angle on estimates of time-to-contact. Ecological Psychology, 8(1), 71–79.
Mastrokalou, N. and Hatziharistos, D. (2007). Rhythmic ability in children and the effects of age, sex, and tempo. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104, 901–912.
McLeod, R. W. and Ross, H. E. (1983). Optic flow and cognitive factors in time-to-collision estimates. Perception, 12, 417–423.
Montare, A. (1988). Further learning effects of knowledge of results upon time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 579–588.
Morofushi, M., Shinohara, K. and Kimura, F. (2001). Menstrual and circadian variations in time perception in healthy women and women with premenstrual syndrome. Neuroscience Research, 41, 339–344.
Neufang, S., Fink, G. R., Herpertz-Dahlmann, Willmes, K. and Konrad, K. (2008). Developmental changes in neural activation and psychophysiological interaction patterns of brain regions associated with interference control and time perception. NeuroImage, 43, 399–409.
Nonis, S. A., Teng, J. K. and Ford, C. W. (2005). A cross-cultural investigation of time management practices and job outcomes. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 409–428.
Nowakowski, C., Shladover, S., Cody, D., Bu, F., O’Connell, J., Spring, J., Dickey, S. and Nelson, D. (2011). Cooperative adaptive cruise control: Testing drivers’ choices of following distances. Technical Report UCB-ITS-PRR-2011-01. Berkeley: California PATH, Institute of Transportation Studies, University of California.
Nugent, A. C., Bain, E. E., Thayer, J. F., Sollers, J. J. and Drevets, W. C. (2011). Sex differences in the neural correlates of autonomic arousal: A pilot PET study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80, 182–191.
Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Salerno, S., Torriero, S., Lo Gerfo, E. and Caltagirone, C. (2009). Representation of time intervals in the right posterior parietal cortex: Implications for a mental time line. NeuroImage, 46, 1173–1179.
Philbin, T. and Seidenstadt, R. M. (1983). Feedback and time perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 308.
Pouthas, V., George, N., Poline, J.-B., Pfeuty, M., VandeMoorteele, P.-F., Hugueville, L., Ferrandez, A.-M., Lehericy, S., LeBihan, D. and Renault, B. (2005). Neural network involved in time perception: A fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 433–441.
Rammsayer, T. and Lustnauer, S. (1989). Sex differences in time perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 195–198.
Rammsayer, T. and Troche, S. (2010). Sex differences in the processing of temporal information in the sub-second range. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 923–927.
Sanders, G. (2011). Sex differences in coincidence-anticipation timing (CAT): A review. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 112, 61–90.
Sanders, G. and Sinclair, K. (2010). Sex differences in accuracy and precision when judging time to arrival: Data from two internet studies. Archives of Sexual Behavior. DOI: 10.1007/s10508–010–9704–2.
Schiff, W. and Oldak, R. (1990). Accuracy of judging time to arrival: Effects of modularity, trajectory and gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 303–316.
Schiff, W., Oldak, R. and Shah, V. (1992). Aging persons’ estimates of vehicular motion. Psychology and Aging, 7, 518–525.
Schmidt, R. A. (1982). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sebel, A. J. and Wilsoncroft, W. E. (1983). Auditory and visual differences in time perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 295–300.
Seow, S. C. (2008). Designing and engineering time: The psychology of time perception in software. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Sevinc¸, E. (2007). The effects of extensive musical training on time perception regarding hemispheric lateralization, different time ranges and generalization to different modalities. Downloaded from www.bilissel.net/kutuphane/Emre_Sevinc_tez.pdf, 14 January 2011.
Smythe, E. J. and Goldstone, S. (1957). The time sense: A normative, genetic study of the development of time perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 7, 49–59.
Späti, J., Münch, M., Blatter, K., Knoblauch, V., Jones, L. A. and Cajochen, C. (2009). Impact of age, sleep pressure and circadian phase on time-of-day estimates. Behavioural Brain Research, 201, 48–52.
Szelag, E., Kowalska, J., Rymarczyk, K. and Pöppel, E. (2002). Duration processing in children as determined by time reproduction: Implications for a few seconds temporal window. Acta Psychologica, 110, 1–19.
Szymaszek, A., Szelag, E. and Sliwowska, M. (2006). Auditory perception of temporal order in humans: The effect of age, gender, listener practice and stimulus presentation mode. Neuroscience Letters, 403, 190–194.
Thilers, P. P., MacDonald, S. W. and Herlitz, A. (2006). The association between endogenous free testosterone and cognitive performance: A population based study in 35 to 90 year-old men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 565–576.
Thilers, P. P., MacDonald, S. W. and Herlitz, A. (2007). Sex differences in cognition: The role of handedness. Physiology and Behavior, 92, 105–109.
Treisman, M. (1984). Temporal rhythms and cerebral rhythms. In: J. Gibbon and L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 423, 542–565.
Van Dam, S. B. J., Abeloos, An. L. M., Mulder, M. and van Paasen, M. M. (2004). Functional presentation of travel opportunities in flexible use airspace: An EID of an airborne conflict support tool. IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, 802–808.
Vitulli, W. F. and Crimmins, K. A. (1999). An experiment in time perception: Separating rate of presentation from type of estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 961–969.
Vitulli, W. F. and Rowe, J. L. (1999). Perception of time: Delay of estimation under auditory and visual tasks with type – A measures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 1209–1210.
Vitulli, W. F. and Shepard, H. A. (1996). Time estimation: Effects of cognitive task, presentation rate, and delay. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 1387–1394.
Warshawsky-Livne, L. and Shinar, D. (2002). Effects of uncertainty, transmission type, driver age and gender on brake reaction and movement time. Journal of Safety Research, 33, 117–128.
Watson, N. V. and Kimura, D. (1991). Nontrivial sex differences in throwing and intercepting: Relation to psychometrically-defined spatial functions. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 375–385.
Weber, D. S. (1965). A time perception task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 21, 863–866.
Wittmann, M. and Lehnhoff, S. (2005). Age effects in perception of time. Psychological Reports, 97, 921–935.
Yan, X., Radwan, E. and Guo, D. (2007). Effects of major-road vehicle speed and driver age and gender on left-turn gap acceptance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 39, 843–852.

References

Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J. and Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 52, 336–347.
Aine, C. J., Supek, S. and George, J. S. (1995). Temporal dynamics of visual-evoked neuromagnetic sources: Effects of stimulus parameters and selective attention. International Journal of Neuroscience, 80, 79–104.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J. and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.
Bedwell, J. S., Brown, J. M. and Miller, L. S. (2003). The magnocellular visual system and schizophrenia: What can the color red tell us? Schizophrenia Research, 63, 273–284.
Bedwell, J. S., Brown, J. M. and Orem, D. M. (2008). The effect of a red background on location backward masking by structure. Perception and Psychophysics, 70, 503–507.
Bedwell, J. S., Brown, J. M., Yanasak, N. E. and Miller, L. S. (2006). Schizophrenia and red light: fMRI evidence for a novel biobehavioral marker. International Journal of Neuroscience, 116, 881–894.
Bedwell, J. S., Chan, C. C., Trachik, B. J. and Rassovsky, Y. (2013). Changes in the visual-evoked P1 potential as a function of schizotypy and background color in healthy young adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 542–547.
Bedwell, J. S., Hernandez, D. C. and Ranieri, A. Y. (2011). The backward masking red light effect and schizotypy: The influence of sex. Psychiatry Research, 189, 228–232.
Bedwell, J. S. and Orem, D. M. (2008). The effect of red light on backward masking in individuals with psychometrically defined schizotypy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 491–504.
Bedwell, J. S., Orem, D. M., Rassovsky, Y., Allen, L. G. and Sutterby, S. R. (2009). A potential qualitative endophenotype for schizophrenia: Backward masking response to red light. Psychiatry Research, 166, 166–173.
Bedwell, J. S., Rassovsky, Y., Orem, D. M. and Kamath, V. (2011). The backward masking red light effect in schizophrenia: Relationship to clinical features and neurocognitive performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 490–496.
Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., Leonard, G. L., Kimchi, R., Luna, B., Humphreys, K. and Minshew, N. (2006). Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia, 44, 110–129.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P. and Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex” issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P. and Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 128, 2430–2441.
Breitmeyer, B. G. and Ganz, L. (1976). Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. Psychological Review, 83, 1–36.
Breitmeyer, B. G. and Williams, M. C. (1990). Effects of isoluminant-background color on metacontrast and stroboscopic motion: Interactions between sustained (P) and transient (M) channels. Vision Research, 30, 1069–1075.
Butler, P. D., Abeles, I. Y., Weiskopf, N. G., Tambini, A., Jalbrzikowski, M., Legatt, M. E. et al. (2009). Sensory contributions to impaired emotion processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 1095–1107.
Butler, P. D., Chen, Y., Ford, J. M., Geye, M. A., Silverstein, S. M., and Green, M. F. (2012). Perceptual measurement in schizophrenia: Promising electrophysiology and neuroimaging paradigms from CNTRICS. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 81–91.
Butler, P. D., Martinez, A., Foxe, J. J., Kim, D., Zemon, V., Silipo, G. et al. (2007). Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain, 130, 417–430.
Butler, P. D., Schechter, I., Zemon, V., Schwartz, S. G., Greenstein, V. C., Gordon, J. et al. (2001). Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1126–1133.
Butler, P. D., Zemon, V., Schechter, I., Saperstein, A. M., Hoptman, M. J., Lim, K. O. et al. (2005). Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 495–504.
Camchong, J., Dyckman, K. A., Austin, B. P., Clementz, B. A. and McDowell, J. E. (2008). Common neural circuitry supporting volitional saccades and its disruption in schizophrenia patients and relatives. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 1042–1050.
Carter, C. S., Robertson, L. C., Nordahl, T. E., Chaderjian, M. and Oshora-Celaya, L. (1996). Perceptual and attentional asymmetries in schizophrenia: Further evidence for a left hemisphere deficit. Psychiatry Research, 62, 111–119.
Chapman, C., Hoag, R. and Giaschi, D. (2004). The effect of disrupting the human magnocellular pathway on global motion perception. Vision Research, 44, 2551–2557.
Chen, Y., Bidwell, L. C. and Holzman, P. S. (2005). Visual motion integration in schizophrenia patients, their first-degree relatives, and patients with bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 74, 271–281.
Chen, Y., Levy, D. L., Sheremata, S., Nakayama, K., Matthysse, S. and Holzman, P. S. (2003). Effects of typical, atypical, and no antipsychotic drugs on visual contrast detection in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1795–1801.
Chen, Y., Nakayama, K., Levy, D., Matthysse, S. and Holzman, P. (2003). Processing of global, but not local, motion direction is deficient in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 61, 215–227.
Davis, R. A., Bockbrader, M. A., Murphy, R. R., Hetrick, W. P. and O’Donnell, B. F. (2006). Subjective perceptual distortions and visual dysfunction in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 199–210.
de Jonge, M. V., Kemner, C., de Haan, E. H., Coppens, J. E., van den Berg, T. J. T. P. and van Engeland, H. (2007). Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology, 21, 65–73.
Del Viva, M. M., Igliozzi, R., Tancredi, R. and Brizzolara, D. (2006). Spatial and motion integration in children with autism. Vision Research, 46, 1242–1252.
de Monasterio, F. M. (1978). Properties of concentrically organized X and Y ganglion cells of macaque retina. Journal of Neurophysiology, 41, 1394–1417.
Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B. and Fagot, J. (2006). Processing of compound visual stimuli by children with autism and Asperger syndrome. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 97–106.
DeSouza, J. F., Menon, R. S. and Everling, S. (2003). Preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in humans investigated with event-related FMRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 1016–1023.
Di Russo, F., Martinez, A., Sereno, M. I., Pitzalis, S. and Hillyard, S. A. (2002). Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential. Human Brain Mapping, 15, 95–111.
Donohoe, G., Morris, D. W., De Sanctis, P., Magno, E., Montesi, J. L., Garavan, H. P. et al. (2008). Early visual processing deficits in dysbindin-associated schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 484–489.
Dyckman, K. A., Lee, A. K., Agam, Y., Vangel, M., Goff, D. C., Barton, J. J. and Manoach, D. S. (2011). Abnormally persistent fMRI activation during antisaccades in schizophrenia: A neural correlate of perseveration? Schizophrenia Research, 132, 62–68.
Edgin, J. O. and Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 729–745.
Edwards, V. T., Hogben, J. H., Clark, C. D. and Pratt, C. (1996). Effects of a red background on magnocellular functioning in average and specifically disabled readers. Vision Research, 36, 1037–1045.
Everling, S. and Munoz, D. P. (2000). Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 387–400.
Ferman, T. J., Primeau, M., Delis, D. and Jampala, C. V. (1999). Global-local processing in schizophrenia: Hemispheric asymmetry and symptom-specific interference. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 5, 442–451.
Ford, K. A., Goltz, H. C., Brown, M. R. and Everling, S. (2005). Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 429–440.
Fox, K., Sato, H. and Daw, N. (1990). The effect of varying stimulus intensity on NMDA-receptor activity in cat visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64, 1413–1428.
Geschwind, D. H. (2009). Advances in autism. Annual Review of Medicine, 60, 367–380.
Granholm, E., Perry, W., Filoteo, J. V. and Braff, D. (1999). Hemispheric and attentional contributions to perceptual organization deficits on the global-local task in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 13, 271–281.
Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T. and Badcock, D. R. (2010). Vision in developmental disorders: Is there a dorsal stream deficit? Brain Research Bulletin, 82, 147–160.
Haenschel, C., Bittner, R. A., Haertling, F., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Maurer, K., Singer, W. and Linden, D. E. (2007). Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: A study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 1229–1240.
Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L. and Enns, J. T. (2006). Global–local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 117–129.
Insel, T. R. (2010). Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature, 458, 187–193.
Javitt, D. C. (2009). When doors of perception close: Bottom-up models of disrupted cognition in schizophrenia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 249–275.
Jemel, B., Mimeault, D., Saint-Amour, D., Hosein, A. and Mottron, L. (2010). visual-evoked potentials contrast sensitivity responses reveal reduced functional segregation of mid and high filters of visual channels in autism. Journal of Vision, 10, 1–13.
Johnson, S. C., Lowery, N., Kohler, C. and Turetsky, B. I. (2005). Global-local visual processing in schizophrenia: Evidence for an early visual processing deficit. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 937–946.
Kantrowitz, J. T., Butler, P. D., Schecter, I., Silipo, G. and Javitt, D. C. (2009). Seeing the world dimly: The impact of early visual deficits on visual experience in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 1085–1094.
Kaplan, E. and Shapley, R. M. (1986). The primate retina contains two types of ganglion cells, with high and low contrast sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 83, 2755–2757.
Kelemen, O., Erdelyi, R., Pataki, I., Benedek, G., Janka, Z. and Keri, S. (2005). Theory of mind and motion perception in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 19, 494–500.
Keshavan, M. S., Nasrallah, H. A. and Tandon, R. (2011). Schizophrenia, “Just the facts” 6. Moving ahead with the schizophrenia concept: From the elephant to the mouse. Schizophrenia Research, 127, 3–13.
Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38.
Kirkpatrick, B., Buchanan, R. W., Ross, D. E. and Carpenter, W. T., Jr. (2001). A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 165–171.
Kiss, I., Fabian, A., Benedek, G. and Keri, S. (2010). When doors of perception open: Visual contrast sensitivity in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 586–593.
Klistorner, A., Crewther, D. P. and Crewther, S. G. (1997). Separate magnocellular and parvocellular contributions from temporal analysis of the multifocal visual-evoked potentials. Vision Research, 37, 2161–2169.
Koh, H. C., Milne, E. and Dobkins, K. (2010). Spatial contrast sensitivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 978–987.
Koychev, I., El-Deredy, W., Haenschel, C. and Deakin, J. F. (2010). Visual information processing deficits as biomarkers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: An event-related potential study in schizotypy. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2205–2214.
Kwon, Y. H., Nelson, S. B., Toth, L. J. and Sur, M. (1992). Effect of stimulus contrast and size on NMDA receptor activity in cat lateral geniculate nucleus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 68, 182–196.
Lalor, E. C., Yeap, S., Reilly, R. B., Pearlmutter, B. A. and Foxe, J. J. (2008). Dissecting the cellular contributions to early visual sensory processing deficits in schizophrenia using the VESPA evoked response. Schizophrenia Research, 98, 256–264.
Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Purkhart, R., Ries, J., Olie, J. P. and van der Meer, E. (2011). Visuo-spatial cognition in schizophrenia: Confirmation of a preference for local information processing. Schizophrenia Research, 127, 163–170.
Lee, A. K., Hamalainen, M. S., Dyckman, K. A., Barton, J. J. and Manoach, D. S. (2011). Saccadic preparation in the frontal eye field is modulated by distinct trial history effects as revealed by magnetoencephalography. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 245–253.
Li, C.-S. R. (2002). Impaired detection of visual motion in schizophrenia patients. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 26, 929–934.
Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1984). Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 309–356.
Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 3416–3468.
Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Manoach, D. S., Thakkar, K. N., Cain, M. S., Polli, F. E., Edelman, J. A., Fischl, B. and Barton, J. J. (2007). Neural activity is modulated by trial history: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the effects of a previous antisaccade. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1791–1798.
Marco, E. J., Hinkley, L. B. N., Hill, S. S. and Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Sensory processing in autism: A review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatric Research, 69, 48R–54R.
McCleery, J. P., Allman, E., Carver, L. J. and Dobkins, K. R. (2007). Abnormal magnocellular pathway visual processing in infants at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 62(9), 1007–1014.
McDowell, J. E., Brown, G. G., Paulus, M., Martinez, A., Stewart, S. E., Dubowitz, D. J. and Braff, D. L. (2002). Neural correlates of refixation saccades and antisaccades in normal and schizophrenia subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 216–223.
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H. and Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 43, 255–263.
Milne, E., White, S., Campbell, R., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P. and Ramus, F. (2006). Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 225–237.
Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S. and Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 904–913.
Mottron, L., Burack, J., Stauder, J. and Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism. Journal of Child psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 203–211.
Nakashima, Y., Momose, T., Sano, I., Katayama, S., Nakajima, T., Niwa, S. and Matsushita, M. (1994). Cortical control of saccade in normal and schizophrenic subjects: A PET study using a task-evoked rCBF paradigm. Schizophrenia Research, 12, 259–264.
O’Donnell, B. F., Bismark, A., Hetrick, W. P., Bodkins, M., Vohs, J. L. and Shekhar, A. (2006). Early stage vision in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 86, 89–98.
O’Donoghue, T., Morris, D. W., Fahey, C., Costa, A. D., Foxe, J. J., Hoerold, D. et al. (in press). A NOS1 variant implicated in cognitive performance influences evoked neural responses during a high density EEG study of early visual perception. Human Brain Mapping.
Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K. and Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.
Pentland, A. (1980). Maximum likelihood estimation: The best PEST. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 377–379.
Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J. and Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742.
Raemaekers, M., Jansma, J. M., Cahn, W., Van der Geest, J. N., van der Linden, J. A., Kahn, R. S. and Ramsey, N. F. (2002). Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 313–320.
Revheim, N., Butler, P. D., Schechter, I., Jalbrzikowski, M., Silipo, G. and Javitt, D. C. (2006). Reading impairment and visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 87, 238–245.
Rondan, C. and Deruelle, C. (2007). Global and configural visual processing in adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, 197–206.
Schechter, I., Butler, P. D., Zemon, V. M., Revheim, N., Saperstein, A. M., Jalbrzikowski, M. et al. (2005). Impairments in generation of early-stage transient visual evoked potentials to magno- and parvocellular-selective stimuli in schizophrenia. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 2204–2215.
Scherf, K. S., Luna, B., Kimchi, R., Minshew, N. and Behrmann, M. (2008). Missing the big picture: Impaired development of global shape processing in autism. Autism Research, 1, 114–129.
Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 125–141.
Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P. and Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.
Slaghuis, W. L. (1998). Contrast sensitivity for stationary and drifting spatial frequency gratings in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 49–62.
Slaghuis, W. L. (2004). Spatio-temporal luminance contrast sensitivity and visual backward masking in schizophrenia. Experimental Brain Research, 156, 196–211.
Slaghuis, W. L., Holthouse, T., Hawkes, A. and Bruno, R. (2007). Eye movement and visual motion perception in schizophrenia, II: Global coherent motion as a function of target velocity and stimulus density. Experimental Brain Research, 182, 415–426.
Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J. and Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Neuroreport, 11, 2765–2767.
Stuve, T. A., Friedman, L., Jesberger, J. A., Gilmore, G. C., Strauss, M. E. and Meltzer, H. Y. (1997). The relationship between smooth pursuit performance, motion perception and sustained visual attention in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls. Psychological Medicine, 27, 143–152.
Sutherland, A. and Crewther, D. P. (2010). Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high autism spectrum quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain, 133, 2089–2097.
Sutter, E. (2000). The interpretation of multifocal binary kernels. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 100, 49–75.
Tu, P. C., Yang, T. H., Kuo, W. J., Hsieh, J. C. and Su, T. P. (2006). Neural correlates of antisaccade deficits in schizophrenia, an fMRI study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 606–612.
Vanni, S., Warnking, J., Dojat, M., Delon-Martin, C., Bullier, J. and Segebarth, C. (2004). Sequence of pattern onset responses in the human visual areas: An fMRI constrained visual-evoked potentials source analysis. Neuroimage, 21, 801–817.
Vialatte, F. B., Maurice, M., Dauwels, J. and Cichocki, A. (2010). Steady-state visually evoked potentials: Focus on essential paradigms and future perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology, 90, 418–438.
Volkmar, F. R., State, M. and Klin, A. (2009). Autism and autism spectrum disorders: Diagnostic issues for the coming decade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50, 108–115.
Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C. and Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.
West, G. L., Anderson, A. A. K., Bedwell, J. S. and Pratt, J. (2010). Red diffuse light suppresses the accelerated perception of fear. Psychological Science, 21, 992–999.
Yeap, S., Kelly, S. P., Sehatpour, P., Magno, E., Garavan, H., Thakore, J. H. and Foxe, J. J. (2008). Visual sensory processing deficits in Schizophrenia and their relationship to disease state. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258, 305–316.
Yeap, S., Kelly, S. P., Thakore, J. H. and Foxe, J. J. (2008). Visual sensory processing deficits in first-episode patients with Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 102, 340–343.
Zemon, V., Kaplan, E. and Ratliff, F. (1980). Bicuculline enhances a negative component and diminishes a positive component of the visual evoked cortical potential in the cat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 77, 7476–7478.
Zhang, X. (2003). Simultaneously recording local luminance responses, spatial and temporal interactions in the visual system with m-sequences. Vision Research, 43(15), 1689–1698.

References

Abel, S. M., Krever, E. M. and Alberti, P. W. (1990). Auditory detection, discrimination and speech processing in ageing, noise-sensitive and hearing-impaired listeners. Scandinavian Audiology, 19, 43–54.
Ball, K., Bear, B., Roenker, D., Miller, R. and Griggs, D. (1988). Age and visual search: Expanding the useful field of view. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics, Image Science and Vision, 5, 2210–2219.
Ball, K., Roenker, D. and Bruni, J. (1990). Developmental changes in attention and visual search throughout adulthood. In James T. Enns (Ed.), The development of attention: Research and theory. (pp. 489–508). North-Holland: Elsevier.
Beer, J. M. and Takayama, L. (2011). Mobile remote presence systems for older adults: Acceptance, benefits, and concerns. In Proceedings of the ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), pp. 19–26.
Biggs, J. and Srinivasan, M. A. (2002). Haptic interfaces. In K. M. Stanney (Ed.), Handbook of virtual environments (pp. 93–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Boot, W., Nichols, T. A., Rogers, W. A. and Fisk, A. D. (2012). Design for aging. In G. Salvendy (Ed.), Handbook of human factors and ergonomics, 4th ed. (pp. 1442–1471). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Bubb-Lewis, C. and Blanchard, H. E. (2000). User preferences in the footprint and weight of cellular phones. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 384–396). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Burda, A. N., Scherz, J. A., Hageman, C. F. and Edwards, H. T. (2003). Age and understanding speakers with Spanish or Taiwanese accents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 11–20.
Campbell V. A., Crews, J. E., Moriarity, D. G., Zack, M. M. and Blackman, D. K. (1999). Surveillance for sensory impairment, activity limitation, and health-related quality of life among older adults – United States, 1993–1997. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. CDC Surveillance Summaries / Centers for Disease Control, 48, 131–156.
Cerella, J. (1985). Age-related decline in extrafoveal letter perception. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 727–736.
Charness, N., Demiris, G. and Krupinski, E. (2011). Designing telehealth for an aging population: A human factors perspective. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Charness, N. and Dijkstra, K. (1999). Age, luminance, and print legibility in homes, offices, and public places. Human Factors, 41, 173–193.
Charness, N. and Holley, P. (2001). Human factors and environmental support in Alzheimer’s disease. Aging and Mental Health, 5, S65–S73.
Charness, N., Schumann, C. and Boritz, G. (1992). Training older adults in word processing: Effects of age, training technique, and computer anxiety. International Journal of Technology and Aging, 5, 79–105.
Chen, T. L., King, C.-H., Thomaz, A. L. and Kemp, C. C. (2011). Touched by a robot: An investigation of subjective responses to robot-initiated touch. In Proceedings of the ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).
Clark, C. L., Hardy, J. L., Volbrecht, V. J. and Werner, J. S. (2010). Scotopic spatiotemporal sensitivity differences between young and old adults. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 30, 339–350.
Costello, M., Madden, D., Mitroff, S. and Whiting, W. (2010) Age-related decline of visual processing components in change detection. Psychology and Aging, 25, 356–368.
Craig, J. C. and Sherrick, C. E. (1982). Dynamic tactile displays. In W. Schiff and E. Foulke (Eds.) Tactual perception: A sourcebook (pp. 209–233). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Craig, J., Rhodes, R. P., Busey, T. A., Kewley-Port, D. and Humes, L. E. (2010). Aging and tactile temporal order. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 226–235.
Cristarella, M. C. (1977). Visual functions of the elderly. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 31, 432–440.
Cruickshanks, K., Wiley, T. L., Tweed, T. S., Klein, B. E., Klein, R., Mares-Perlman, J. A. and Nondahl, D. M. (1998). Prevalence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin: The epidemiology of hearing loss study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 148, 879–886.
Czaja, S. J., Guerrier, J. H., Nair, Sankaran N. and Landauer, T. K. (1993). Computer communication as an aid to independence for older adults. Behaviour and Information Technology, 12, 197–207.
Czaja, S. J. and Lee, C. C. (2003). Designing computer systems for older adults. In J. A. Jacko and A. Sears (Eds.), The human-computer interaction handbook (pp. 413–427). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Donelan, K., Hill, C. A., Hoffman, C., Scoles, K., Feldman, P. H., Levine, C. and Gould, D. (2002). Challenged to care: Informal caregivers in a changing health system. Health Affairs, 21, 222–231.
Dubno, J. R., Ahlstrom, J. B. and Horwitz, A. R. (2000). Use of context by young and aged adults with normal hearing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 107, 538–546.
Fisk, A. D., Rogers, W. A., Charness, N., Czaja, S. J. and Sharit, J. (2009). Designing for older adults: Principles and creative human factors approaches (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Gelfand, S. A. (2009). Essentials of audiology (3rd ed.). New York: Thieme Medical.
Goodman, M. J., Tijerina, L., Bents, F. D. and Wierwille, W. W. (1999). Using cellular telephones in vehicles: Safe or unsafe? Transportation Human Factors, 1, 3–42.
Gordon-Salant, S. (2005). Hearing loss and aging: New research findings and clinical implications. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 42, 9–24.
Gordon-Salant, S., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Fitzgibbons, P. J. and Barrett, J. (2006). Age-related differences in identification and discrimination of temporal cues in speech segments. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 2455–2466.
Gottlob, L. and Madden, D. (1998). Time course of allocation of visual attention after equating for sensory differences: An age-related perspective. Psychology and Aging, 13, 138–149.
Gottlob, L. and Madden, D. (1999). Age differences in the strategic allocation of visual attention. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54B, 165–172.
Hagen, S. and Sandnes, F. E. (2009). Toward accessible self-service kiosks through intelligent user interfaces. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 14, 715–721.
Hale, K. S. and Stanney, K. M. (2004). Deriving haptic design guidelines from human physiological, psychophysical, and neurological foundations. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 43, 33–39.
He, N.-J., Dubno, J. R. and Mills, J. H. (1998). Frequency and intensity discrimination measured in a maximum-likelihood procedure from young and aged normal-hearing subjects. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 103, 553–565.
Herse, P. (1995). A new method for quantification of the dynamics of dark adaptation. Optometry and Vision Science, 72, 907–910.
Hoffman, R. R. and Elliott, L. R. (2010, Fall). Using Scenario-Based Envisioning to identify novel applications of wearable flexible displays. Ergonomics in Design, 1,8 4–8.
Hohberger, B., Laemmer, R., Adler, W., Juenemann, A. and Horn, F., (2007). Measuring contrast sensitivity in normal subjects with OPTEC® 6500: Influence of age and glare. Graefe’s Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 245, 1804–1814.
Howell, P. and Powell, D. (1987). Delayed auditory feedback with delayed sounds varying in duration. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 166–172.
Humes, L. E., Busey, T. A., Craig, J. C. and Kewley-Port, D. (2009). The effects of age on sensory thresholds and temporal gap detection in hearing, vision, and touch. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71, 860–871.
Illuminating Engineering Society of North America (IESNA) (2000). Lighting handbook: Reference and application (9th ed.). Mark Rea (Ed.). New York: IESNA.
Jacko, J. A., Emery, V. K., Edwards, P. G., Ashok, M., Barnard, L., Kongnakorn, T. et al. (2004). The effects of multimodal feedback on older adults’ task performance given varying levels of computer experience. Behaviour and Information Technology, 23, 247–264.
Jastrzembski, T. S. and Charness, N. (2007). The model human processor and the older adult: Parameter estimation and validation within a mobile phone task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 224–248.
Jiao, M., Wu, S. (2010). Sunlight readable fast-response transflective LCDs by horizontal electric fields. Journal of Display Technology, 6, 163–165
Jin, Z., Plocher, T. and Kiff, L. (2007). Touch screen user interfaces for older adults: Button size and spacing. Universal Access in Human Computer Interaction. Coping with Diversity, 4554, 933–941.
Kalasky, M. A., Czaja, S. J., Sharit, J. and Nair, S. N. (1999). Is speech recognition technology robust for older populations? In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 123–127). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Ketcham, C. J. and Stelmach, G. E. (2002). Motor control of older adults. In D. J. Ekerdt et al. (Eds.) Encyclopedia of aging. New York: Macmillan Reference USA.
Kirchner, C. and Peterson, R. (1979). The latest data on visual disability from NCHS. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 73, 151–157.
Kline, D. W. and Scialfa, C. T. (1997). Sensory and perceptual functioning: Basic research and human factors implications. In A. D. Fisk and W. A. Rogers (Eds.) Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 327–328). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Knoblauch, K., Saunders, F., Kusuda, M., Hynes, R., Podgor, M., Higgins, K. E. and de Monasterio, F. M. (1987). Age and illuminance effects in the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test. Applied Optics, 26, 1441–1448.
Kraft, J. M. and Werner, J. S. (1999). Aging and the saturation of colors, 1: Colorimetric purity discrimination. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics, Image Science and Vision, 16, 223–230.
Kraft, K. and Wölfel, M. (2007, December). Human robot noise suppression by particle filters for improved automatic speech recognition accuracy. In Proceedings of the IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), pp. 1737–1742.
Kroemer, K. H. E. (1997). Anthropometry and biomechanics. In A. D. Fisk and W. A. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 87–124). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kuo, I. H., Rabindran, J. M., Broadbent, E., Lee, Y. I., Kerse, N., Stafford, R. M. Q., and MacDonald, B. A. (2009). Age and gender factors in user acceptance of healthcare robots. Paper presented at the 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN 2009).
Lanzolla, V. R. and Mayhorn, C. B. (2004). The usability of personal digital assistants as prospective memory aids for medication adherence in young and older adults. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 258–261). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Legge, G. E. (2007). Psychophysics of reading in normal and low vision. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Leirer, V. O., Morrow, D. G., Tanke, E. D. and Pariante, G. M. (1991). Elder’s nonadherence: Its assessment and medication reminding by voice mail. The Gerontologist, 31, 514–520.
Lethbridge-Cejku, M., Schiller, J. S. and Bernadel, L. (2004). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: National health interview survey, 2002. Vital Health Statistics, 10, 1–151.
Library of Congress. (2006). Life expectancy in the United States. Downloaded from http://aging.senate.gov/crs/aging1.pdf, 28 June 2011.
Lighthouse Inc. (1995). The Lighthouse national survey on vision loss: Experience, attitudes, and knowledge of middle-aged and older Americans. New York: Lighthouse.
Lin, C. (2003). Effects of contrast ratio and text color on visual performance with TFT-LCD. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 31, 65–72.
Lockhart, T. and Shi, W. (2010). Effects of age on dynamic accommodation. Ergonomics, 53, 892–903.
Loricchio, D. F and Lewis, J. R. (1991). User assessment of standard and reduced-size numeric keypads. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 251–252). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Madden, D. J. (1988). Adult age differences in the effects of sentence context and stimulus degradation during visual word recognition. Psychology and Aging, 3, 167–172.
Madden, D. J. (2007). Aging and visual attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 70–74.
Mann, W. C., Helal, S., Davenport, R. D., Justiss, M. D., Tomita, M. R. and Kemp, B. J. (2004). Use of cell phones by elders with impairments: Overall appraisal, satisfaction, and suggestions. Technology and Disability, 16, 49–57.
Marics, M. A. and Engelbeck, G. (1997). Designing voice menu applications for telephone applications. In M. G. Helander, T. K. Landauer, and P. V. Prabhu (Eds.), Handbook of human-computer interaction (pp. 1085–1102). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Mendat, C. C. (2006). Measuring age differences in the effectiveness of tactile cues in cellular phones. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, North Carolina State University, Raleigh.
Mendat, C. C., Bell, J. and Wogalter, M. S. (2004). The effectiveness of tactile cues in cellular phones. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 717–720). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Mendat, C. C. and Mayhorn, C. B. (2007). Oops, wrong number: Exploring age and cellular telephone dialing performance. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 132–135). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Meyer, F. M., Trissell, T. L., Aleva, D. L., Longo, S. J. and Hopper, D. G. (2006). Readability evaluation of an active matrix electrophoretic ink display. In Proceedings of SPIE – the International Society for Optical Engineering. DOI:10.1117/12.668900.
Mitzner, T. L., Boron, J. B., Fausset, C. B., Adams, A. E., Charness, N., Czaja, S. J. et al. (2010). Older adults talk technology: Technology usage and attitudes. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1710–1721.
Mitzner, T. L. and Rogers, W. A. (2006). Reading in the dark: Effects of age and contrast on reading speed and comprehension. Human Factors, 48, 229–240.
Mitzner, T. L., Touron, D., Rogers, W. and Hertzog, C. (2010). Checking it twice: Age-related differences in double checking during visual search. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 1326–1330). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Mooney, A. M., Nussbaum, M. A. and Smith-Jackson, T. L. (2002). Universal access in practice: Usability evaluation of cellular telephones for users with disabilities. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 695–699). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Morrison, H. B. and Casali, J. G. (1994). Intelligibility of synthesized voice messages in commercial truck cab noise for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
National Telecommunications and Information Administration (2000). Falling through the net: Toward digital inclusion. Downloaded from http://search.ntia.doc.gov/pdf/fttn00.pdf, 29 June 2011.
Norman, J. F., Crabtree, C. E., Norman, H. F., Moncrief, B. K., Herrmann, M. and Kapley, N. (2006). Aging and the visual, haptic and cross-modal perception of natural object shape. Perception, 35, 1383–1395.
Oakley, I., Brewster, S. A. and Gray, P. D. (2001). Solving multi-target haptic problems in menu interaction. In Proceedings of Computer Human Interaction Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI) (pp. 357–358). DOI:10.1145/634067.634278.
O’Brien, M. A., Rogers, W. A., Fisk, A. D. and Richman, M. (2008). Assessing design features of virtual keyboards for text entry. Human Factors, 50, 680–698.
Olson, K. E., O’Brien, M. A., Rogers, W. A. and Charness, N. (2010). Diffusion of technology: Frequency of use for younger and older adults. Ageing International, 36, 123–145.
Osterhaus, W. K. E. (2005). Discomfort glare assessment and prevention for daylight applications in office environments. Solar Energy, 79(2), 140–158.
Oyama, H. and Shiramatsu, N. (2002). Smaller and bigger displays. Displays, 23, 31–39.
Pak, R. and McLaughlin, A. C. (2010). Designing displays for older adults. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Pearson, J. D., Morrell, C. H., Gordon-Salant, S., Brant, L. J., Metter, E. J., Klein, L. L. and Fozard, J. L. (1995). Gender differences in a longitudinal study of age-associated hearing loss. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 1196–1205.
Perry, A. R. and Wingfield, A. (1994). Contextual encoding by young and elderly adults as revealed by cued and free recall. Aging and Cognition, 1, 120–139.
Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Johnson, C. E. and Roodenburg, K. E. J. (1998). The discrepancy between hearing impairment and handicap in the elderly: Balancing transaction and interaction in conversation. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 26, 99–119.
Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Schneider, B. A., Benson, N. J., Hamstra, S. J. and Storzer, E. (2006). Effect of age on detection of gaps in speech and nonspeech markers varying in duration and spectral symmetry. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 1143–1155.
Pierno, A. C., Caria, A., Glover, S. and Castiello, U. (2005). Effects of increasing visual load on aurally and visually guided target acquisition in a virtual environment. Applied Ergonomics, 36, 335–343.
Pineau, J., Montemerlo, M., Pollack, M., Roy, N., and Thrun, S. (2003). Towards robotic assistants in nursing homes: Challenges and results. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42, 271–281.
Pollard, T. and Cooper, J. (1984). Quantitative analysis of the effect of Acanthamoeba profiling on actin filament nucleation and elongation. Biochemistry, 23, 6631–6641.
Pulli, P., Hyry, J., Pouke, M. and Yamamoto, G. (2010). User interaction in smart ambient environment targeted for senior citizen. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Symposium on Applied Sciences in Biomedical and Communication Technologies (ISABEL).
Rau, P.-L. P. and Hsu, J.-W. (2005). Interaction devices and web design for novice older users. Educational Gerontology, 31, 19–40.
Rizzo, M., Sparks, J., McEvoy, S., Viamonte, S., Kellison, I. and Vecera, S. (2009). Change blindness, aging, and cognition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 245–256.
Rogers, W. A., Mayhorn, C. B. and Fisk, A. D. (2004). Technology in everyday life for older adults. In S. Kwon and D. C. Burdick (Eds.) Gerotechnology: Research and practice in technology and aging (pp. 3–18). New York: Springer.
Rogers, W. A., Meyer, B., Walker, N. and Fisk, A. D. (1998). Functional limitations to daily living tasks in the aged: A focus group analysis. Human Factors, 40, 111–125.
Ronzhin, A., Karpov, A., Kipyatkova, I. and Zelezny, M. (2010). Client and speech detection system for intelligent infokiosk. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 6231, 560–567.
Roring, R. W., Hines, F. G. and Charness, N. (2007). Age differences in identifying words in synthetic speech. Human Factors, 49, 25–31.
Rubin, G., West, S., Munoz, B., Bandeen, Roche, K., Zeger, S., Schein, O., Fried, L. P. (1997). A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans – The SEE study. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38, 557–568.
Russell-Minda, E., Jutai, J. W., Strong, J. G., Campbell, K. A., Gold, D., Pretty, L. and Wilmot, L. (2007). The legibility of typefaces for readers with low vision: A research review. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 101, 402–415.
Sagawa, K. and Takahashi, T. (2001). Spectral luminous efficiency as a function of age. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics, Image Science and Vision, 18, 2659–2667.
Schieber, F. (1994). Age and glare recovery time for low contrast stimuli. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 496–499). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Schneider, B. A. and Hamstra, S. J. (1999). Gap detection thresholds as a function of tonal duration for younger and older listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 371–380.
Scialfa, C. T., Garvey, P. M., Tyrrell, R. A. and Leibowitz, H. W. (1992). Age differences in dynamic contrast thresholds. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47, 172–175.
Scialfa, C. T., Kline, D. W. and Lyman, B. J. (1987). Age differences in target identification as a function of retinal location and noise level: Examination of the useful field of view. Psychology and Aging, 2, 14–19.
Scialfa, C., Thomas, D. and Joffe, K. (1994). Age differences in the useful field of view: An eye movement analysis. Optometry and Vision Science, 71, 736–742.
Sharit, J., Czaja, S. J., Nair, S. and Lee, C. C. (2003). Effects of age, speech rate and environmental support in using telephone voice menu systems. Human Factors, 45, 234–251.
Sheldon, S., Pichora-Fuller, M. K. and Schneider, B. A. (2008). Priming and sentence context support listening to noise-vocoded speech by younger and older adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123, 489–499.
Sherrick, C. E. and Cholewiak, R. W. (1986). Cutaneous sensitivity. In K. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance, Vol. 1 (pp. 1–70). New York: Wiley.
Sloane, M. E., Owsley, C. and Alvarez, S. L. (1988). Aging, senile miosis and spatial contrast sensitivity at low luminance. Vision Research, 28, 1235–1246.
Speranza, F., Daneman, M. and Schneider, B. (2000). How aging affects the reading of words in noisy backgrounds. Psychology and Aging, 15, 253–258.
Steen, R., Whitaker, D., Elliot, D. B. and Wild, J. M. (1994). Age-related effects of glare on luminance and color contrast sensitivity. Optometry and Vision Science, 71, 792–796.
Stevens, J. C. and Choo K. K. (1996). Spatial acuity of the body surface over the life span. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 13, 153–166.
Stevens, J. C. and Patterson, M. O. (1995). Dimensions of spatial acuity in the touch sense: Changes over the life span. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 12, 29–47.
Sturr, J. F., Kline, G. E. and Taub, H. A. (1990). Performance of young and older drivers on a static acuity test under photopic and mesopic luminance conditions. Human Factors, 32, 1–8.
Tanke, E. and Leirer, V. (1993). Use of automated telephone reminders to increase elderly patients’ adherence to tuberculosis medication appointments. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 193–196). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Thornbury, J. M. and Mistretta, C. M. (1981). Tactile sensitivity as a function of age. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 34–39.
Tun, P. A. (1998). Fast noisy speech: Age differences in processing rapid speech with background noise. Psychology and Aging, 13, 424–434.
Tun, P. A. and Wingfield, A. (1997). Language and communication: Fundamentals of speech communication and language processing in old age. In A. D. Fisk and W. A. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 125–149). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Tun, P. A. and Wingfield, A. (1999). One voice too many: Adult age differences in language processing with different types of distracting sounds. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54B, P317–P327.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2011). International data base. World population by age and sex. Downloaded from http://www.census.gov/population/international/data/idb/worldpop.php, 30 June 2011.
United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (2004). World population to 2300 (ST/ESA/SER.A/236). New York: United Nations.
Vanderplas, J. M. and Vanderplas, J. H. (1980). Some factors affecting legibility of printed materials for older adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 923–932.
Verrillo, R., Bolanowski, S. and Gescheider, G. (2002). Effect of aging on the subjective magnitude of vibration. Somatosensory and Motor Research, 19, 238–244.
Vitale, S., Cotch, M. F. and Sperduto, R. D. (2006). Prevalence of visual impairment in the United States. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 295, 2158–2163.
Wagner, D. L. (1997). Long-distance caregiving for older adults: Healthcare and aging. Washington, DC: National Council on the Aging.
Wagner, N., Hassanein, K. and Head, M. (2010). Computer use by older adults: A multi-disciplinary review. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 870–882.
Wijk, H., Berg, S., Sivik, L. and Steen, B. (1999). Color discrimination, color naming and color preferences in 80-year olds. Aging (Milano), 11, 176–185.
Wingfield, A., Poon, L. W., Lombardi, L. and Lowe, D. (1985). Speed of processing in normal aging: Effects of speech rate, linguistic structure, and processing time. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 579–585.
Wright, P., Bartram, C., Rogers, N., Emslie, H., Evans, J., Wilson, B. and Belt, S. (2000). Text entry on handheld computers by older users. Ergonomics, 43, 702–716.
Zwislocki, J. J. and Goodman, D. A. (1980). Absolute scaling of sensory magnitudes: A validation. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 28–38.

References

Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.
Ashby, F. G., Ennis, J. M. and Spiering, B. J. (2007). A neurobiological theory of automaticity in perceptual categorization. Psychological Review, 114, 632–656
Bajic, D. and Rickard, T. C. (2009). The temporal dynamics of strategy execution in cognitive skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 35, 113–121.
Banks, E., Reeves, G., Beral, V. et al. (2004). Influence of personal characteristics of individual women on sensitivity and specificity of mammography in the Million Women Study: Cohort study. British Medical Journal, 329, 477
Bourne, L. E., Healy, A. F., Kole, J. A. and Graham, S. M. (2006). Strategy shifts in classification skill acquisition: Does memory retrieval dominate rule use? Memory and Cognition, 34, 903–913
Brammer, J. and Thomma, S. (May 7, 2011). Obama thanks special forces for daring bin Laden raid. The Seattle Times. Downloaded from http://seattletimes.com/html/nationworld/2014986679_obama07.html.
Brooks, S. E., Oi, F. M. and Koehler, P. G. (2003). Ability of canine termite detectors to locate live termites and discriminate them for non-termite material. Journal of Economic Entomology, 96, 1259–1266.
Dosher, B. A. and Lu, Z. (2007). The functional form of performance improvements in perceptual learning: Learning rates and transfer. Psychological Science, 18, 531–539.
Engeman, R. M., Vice, D. S., York, D. and Gruver, K. S. (2002). Sustained evaluation of the effectiveness of detector dogs for locating brown tree snakes in cargo outbound from Guam. International Biodeterioration and Biodegradation, 49, 101–106.
Ericsson, K.A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J. and Hoffman, R. R., (Eds.) (2006). Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. and Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Fjellanger, R., Andersen, E. K. and McLean, I. (2000). A training program for filter-search mine detection dogs. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, 277–286.
Furton, K. G. and Myers, L. J. (2001). The scientific foundation and efficacy of use of canines as chemical detectors of explosives. Talanta, 54, 487–500.
Gazit, I., Goldblatt, A. and Terkel, J. (2005a). Formation of olfactory search image for explosives odours in sniffer dogs. Ethology, 111, 669–680.
Gazit, I., Goldblatt, A. and Terkel, J. (2005b). The role of context specificity in learning: The effects of training context on explosives detection in dogs. Animal Cognition, 8, 143–150
Gazit, I. and Terkel, J. (2003). Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous physical activity. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 81, 149–161.
Gazit, I. and Terkel, J. (2003). Domination of olfaction over vision in explosives detection by dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 82, 65–73.
Gigerenzer, G. and Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–669.
Helton, W. S. (2007a). Skill in expert dogs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 171–178.
Helton, W. S. (2007b). Deliberate practice in dogs: A canine model of expertise. Journal of General Psychology, 134, 247–257.
Helton, W. S. (2008). Expertise acquisition as sustained learning in humans and other animals: Commonalities across species. Animal Cognition, 11, 99–107.
Helton, W. S. (2009). Canine ergonomics: The science of working dogs. New York: CRC.
Helton, W. S. (2011). Animal expertise: Evidence of phase transitions by utilizing running estimates of performance variability. Ecological Psychology, 23, 59–75.
Helton, W. S., Begoske, S., Pastel, R. and Tan, J. (2007). A case study in canine-human factors: A remote scent sampler for landmine detection. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 51, 582–586.
Hoffman, R. R. (1998). How can expertise be defined?: Implications of research from cognitive psychology. In R. Williams, W. Faulkner and J. Fleck (Eds.), Exploring expertise (pp. 81–100). New York: Macmillan.
Hogarth, R. L. and Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1–55.
HSNW (2010). Pentagon: Dogs better than technology at bomb detection. Downloaded from http://homelandsecuritynewswire.com/pentagon-dogs-better-technology-bomb-detection.
Johnston, J. M. (1999). Canine detection capabilities: Operational implications of recent R and D findings. Auburn, AL: Institute for Biological Detection Systems, Auburn University.
Kauhanen, E., Harri, M., Nevalainen, A. and Nevalainen, T. (2002). Validity of detection of microbial growth in buildings by trained dogs. Environment International, 28, 153–157.
Kiddy, C. A., Mitchell, D. S., Bolt, D. J., Hawk, H. W. (1978). Detection of estrus-related odors in cows by trained dogs. Biology of Reproduction, 19, 389–395.
Krechevsky, L. (1932). Hypotheses in rats. Psychological Review, 39, 516–532.
Macmillan, N. A. and Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McCulloch, M., Jezierski, T., Broffman, M., Hubbard, A., Turner, K. and Janecki, T. (2006). Diagnostic accuracy of canine scent detection in early- and late-stage lung and breast cancers. Integrative Cancer Therapies, 5, 30–39.
Morgan, C. L. (1894). An introduction to comparative psychology. New York: Scribners.
Pickel, D., Manucy, G. P., Walker, D. B., Hall, S. B. and Walker, J. C. (2004). Evidence of canine olfactory detection of melanoma. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 89, 107–116.
Rickard, T. C. (1999). A CMPL alternative account of practice effects in numerosity judgement tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 532–542.
Sanquist, T. F., Mahy, H. A., Posse, C. and Morris, F. (2006) Psychometric survey methods for measuring attitudes toward homeland security systems and personal privacy. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50, 1808–1811.
Slabbert, J. M. and Rasa, O. A. E. (1997). Observational learning of an acquired maternal behaviour pattern by working dog pups: An alternative training method? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 53, 309–316.
Slotta-Bachmayr, L. (2005). How burial time of avalanche victims is influenced by rescue method: an analysis of search reports from the Alps. Natural Hazards, 34, 341–352.
Smith, D. A., Ralls, K., Hurt, A., Adams, B., Parker, M., Davenport, B., Smith, M. C. and Maldonado (2003). Detection and accuracy rates of dogs trained to find scats of San Joaquin kit foxes (Vulpes macrotis mutica). Animal Conservation, 6, 339–346.
Tolman, E. C. (1927). A behaviorist’s definition of consciousness. Psychological Review, 34, 433–439.
Tolman, E. C. and Minium, E. (1942). VTE in rats: Overlearning and difficulty of discrimination. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 34, 301–306.
Touron, D. R. and Hertzog, C. (2004). Strategy shift affordance and strategy choice in young and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 32, 298–310.
Vetter, G., Stadler, M. and Haynes, J. D. (1997). Phase transitions in learning. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 18, 335–350.
Walker, D. B., Walker, J. C., Cavnar, P. J., Taylor, J. L., Pickel, D. H., Hall, S. B. and Suarez, J. C. (2006). Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 97, 241–254.
Weissburg, M. J. (2000). The fluid dynamical context of chemosensory behavior. Biological Bulletin, 198, 188–202.
Wickens, C. D., Goh, J., Helleberg, J., Horrey, W. J. and Talleur, D. A. (2003). Attentional models of multitask pilot performance using advanced display technology. Human Factors, 45, 360–380.
Williams, M., Johnston, J. M., Waggoner, L. P., Jackson, J., Jones, M., Boussom, T., Halalowell, S. F. and Petrousky, J. A. (1997). Determination of the canine odor detection signature for selected nitroglycerin based smokeless powder. Proceedings of the 13th Annual Security Technology Symposium and Exhibition, Virginia Beach, VA.
Zanone, P. G. and Kelso, J. A. S. (1992). Evolution of behavioural attractors with learning: Nonequilibrium phase transitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 403–421.