Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 1
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Funke, Matthew E. Warm, Joel S. Matthews, Gerald Funke, Gregory J. Chiu, Peter Y. Shaw, Tyler H. and Greenlee, Eric T. 2017. The Neuroergonomics of Vigilance. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Vol. 59, Issue. 1, p. 62.

    ×
  • Online publication date: July 2015

6 - Applied Perception and Neuroergonomics

from Part I - Background and Methodology
Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Cambridge Handbook of Applied Perception Research
  • Online ISBN: 9780511973017
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511973017
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×

References

Aaslid, R. (1986). Transcranial Doppler examination techniques. In R. Aaslid (Ed.), Transcranial Doppler sonography (pp. 39–59). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Allen L. (2000). The hole in my vision. Iowa City, IA: Penfield Press.
Anderson, S. W. and Rizzo, M. (1994). Hallucinations following occipital lobe damage: The pathological activation of visual representations. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 651–663.
Ball, K., Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L. and Bruni, J. R. (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34, 3110–3123.
Bender, M. B. (1945). Polyopia and monocular diplopia of cerebral origin. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 54, 323–338.
Blake, R. and Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47–73.
Boynton, R. M. (1992). Human color vision. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.
Coeckelbergh, T. R. M. (2002). Effect of compensatory viewing strategies on practical fitness to drive in subjects with visual field defects caused by ocular pathology. Ph.D. dissertation, Groningen, the Netherlands: Rijksuniversiteit.
Cooke, N., Pringle, H. L. Pedersen, H. K. and Connor, O. (Eds.) (2006). Human factors of remotely operated vehicles. Advances in human performance and cognitive engineering, Vol. 7. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Cosman, J. D., Lees, M. N., Lee, J. D., Rizzo, M. and Vecera, S. P. (2011). Impaired attentional disengagement in older adults with useful field of view decline. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 10.1093/geronb/gbr116
Cosman, J. D., Lees, M. N., Lee, J. D., Rizzo, M. and Vecera, S. P. (2012). Visual search for features and conjunctions following declines in the useful field of view. Experimental Aging Research, 38(4), 411–421. PMCID PMC3823497.
Cosman, J. and Rizzo, M. (2012). Attention. In D. B. Arciniegas, C. A. Anderson and C. Filley (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neuropsychiatry. Cambridge University Press.
Critchley, M. Types of visual perseveration: “Paliopsia” and “illusory visual spread.” Brain, 74: 267–299.
Dominy, N. J. and Lucas, P. W. (2001). Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates. Nature, 410, 363–366.
Farah, M. J., Wong, A. B., Monheit, M. A. and Morrow, L. A. (1989). Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? Neuropsychologia, 27, 461–470.
Ferguson, D. A., Major, G. and Keldoulis, T. (1974). Vision at work: Visual defect and the visual demand of tasks. Applied Ergonomics, 5, 84–93.
Ferris, F. L., Kassoff, A. and Bresnick, G. H. (1982). New visual acuity charts for clinical research. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 94, 91–96.
Ffytche, D. H. (2008). The hodology of hallucinations. Cortex, 44, 1067–1083.
Ffytche, D. H. (2009). Visual hallucinations in eye disease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22, 28–35.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Hillyard, S. A., Simpson, G. V., Woods, D. L., Van Voorhis, S. and Münte, T. F. (1984). Event-related brain potentials and selective attention to different modalities. In F. Reinoso-Suárez and C. Ajmone-Marsan (Eds.), Cortical Integration. (pp. 395–414). New York: Raven Press.
Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N., Shear, P. K., Tripp, L. D., Mayleben, D., Rosa, R. R. and Parasuraman, R. (2003). Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 4, 89–112.
Hoffman, R. R., Detweiler, M.. Conway, R. and Lipton, K. (1993). Some considerations in using color in meteorological displays. Weather and Forecasting, 8, 505–518.
Holliday, J. T., Trujillo, J. T. C, Ruiz, R. S. (1987). Brightness acuity test and outdoor visual acuity in cataract patients. Journal of Cataract and Refractory Surgery, 13, 67–69.
Horrey, W. J. and Wickens, C. D. (2006). Examining the impact of cell phone conversations on driving using meta-analytic techniques. Human Factors, 48, 195–205.
Horton, J. C. and Trobe, J. D. (1999). Akinetopsia from nefazodone toxicity. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 128, 530–531.
Jiang, Y., Greenwood, P. and Parasuraman, R. (1999). Age-related reduction in 3-D motion priming. Psychology and Aging, 14, 619–626.
Jones, L. A. (1943). Historical background and evolution of the colorimetry report. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 33, 534–543.
Just, M., Carpenter, P. A. and Miyake, A. (2003). Neuroindices of cognitive workload: Neuroimaging, pupillometric, and event-related potential studies of brain work. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 4(1–2), 56–88.
Just, M., Keller, T. A. and Cynkar, J. (2008). A decrease in brain activation associated with driving when listening to someone speak. Brain Research, 1205, 70–80.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kramer, A. F., Cassavaugh, N. D., Irwin, D. E. and Peterson, M. S. (2001). Influence of single and multiple onset distractors on visual search for singleton targets. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 952–968.
Kramer, A. F. and McCarley, J. (2007). Eye movements as a window on perception. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.). Neuroeorgonomics: The brain at work. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kramer, A. and Parasuraman, R. (2007). Neuroergonomics – application of neuroscience to human factors. In J. Caccioppo, L. Tassinary, L. and G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology. (pp. 704–722). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Land, E., Hubel, D., Livingstone, M., Perry, S. and Burns, M. (1983). Colour-generating interactions across the corpus callosum. Nature, 303, 616–618.
Land, E. H. (1986). Recent advances in Retinex theory. Vision Research, 26, 7–21.
Land, M. F. and Lee, D. N. (1994). Where we look when we steer. Nature, 369, 742–744.
Lees, M. N., Cosman, J. D., Fricke, N., Lee, J. D. and Rizzo, M. (2010). Translating cognitive neuroscience to the driver’s operational environment: A neuroergonomics approach. American Journal of Psychology. 123, 391–411.
Lees, M. N., Lee, J. D. and Rizzo, M. (in press). Cross-modal warnings for orienting attention in older drivers with and without attention impairments. Applied Ergonomics.43, 768–776. PMCID PMC3302974.
Lyons, T. J. and Simpson C. G. (1989) The giant hand phenomenon. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, 60, 64–66.
McGowan, G., Taha, T. Y., Heron, G. and Diaper, C. (2011). The Pulfrich phenomenon; clumsiness and collisions which can be ameliorated. Practical Neurology, 11, 173–176.
Mourant, R. R. and Rockwell, T. H. (1972). Strategies of visual search by novice and experienced drivers. Human Factors, 14, 325–335.
Nakayama, K. and Joseph, J. S. (1998). Attention, pattern recognition and pop-out in visual search. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain. (pp. 279–298). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
National Research Council. (2011). The effects of commuting on pilot fatigue. Board on Human-Systems Integration, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Navon, D. (1984). Resources: A theoretical soup stone. Psychological Review, 91, 216–234.
Nawrot, M. (2001). Depth perception in driving: Alcohol intoxication, eye movement changes, and the disruption of motion parallax. In Proceedings of the First International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driving Assessment, Training, and Vehicle Design.
Nawrot, M. and Rizzo, M. (1998). Chronic motion perception deficits from midline cerebellar lesions in human. Vision Research, 38, 2219–2224.
O’Regan, J. K., Rensink, R. A. and Clark, J. J. (1999). Change-blindness as a result of “mudsplashes.” Nature, 398, 34.
Palmer, S. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Parasuraman, R. (2003). Neuroergonomics: Research and practice. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 4, 5–20.
Parasuraman, R. (2011). Neuroergonomics: Brain, cognition, and performance at work. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 181–186.
Parasuraman, R., Cosenzo, K. and de Visser, E. (2009). Adaptive automation for human supervision of multiple uninhabited vehicles: Effects on change detection, situation awareness, and mental workload. Military Psychology, 21, 270–297.
Parasuraman, R., De Visser, E., Clarke, E., McGarry, W. R., Hussey, E., Shaw, T. and Thompson, J. (2009). Detecting threat-related intentional actions of others: Effects of image quality, response mode, and target cueing on vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 275–290.
Parasuraman, R. and Rizzo, M. (2007). Neuroergonomics: The brain at work. New York: Oxford University Press.
Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. S. and See, J. W. (1998). Brain systems of vigilance. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.) The attentive brain (pp. 221–256). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pelli, D. G, Robson, J. G and Wilkins, A. J. (1988). The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clinical Vision Sciences, 2, 187–199.
Pietras, T., Shi, Q. and Rizzo, M. (2006). Traffic entry behavior in older drivers with selective attention impairment. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102, 632–644.
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. and Ogden, W., (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial locations, in H. Pick and L. Saltzmann (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information. (pp. 137–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pradhan, A. K, Pollatsek, A, Knodler, M. and Fisher, D. L. (2009). Can younger drivers be trained to scan for information that will reduce their risk in roadway traffic scenarios that are hard to identify as hazardous? Ergonomics, 52, 657–673.
Previc, H. (1998). The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 123–164.
Recarte, M. A. and Nunes, L. M. (2000). Effects of verbal and spatial-imagery tasks on eye fixations. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Applied, 6, 31-43.
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K. and Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373.
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K. and Clark, J. J. (2000). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions. Visual Cognition, 7, 127–145.
Rizzo, M. (2011). A 70-year-old man trying to decide if he should continue driving. Journal of the American Medical Association, 305, 1018–1026.
Rizzo, M., Akutsu, H. and Dawson, J. (2001). Increased attentional blink after focal cerebral lesions. Neurology, 57, 795–800.
Rizzo, M. and Barton, J. J. S. (2005). Central disorders if visual function. In N. R. Miller, J. Nancy Newman, V. Biousse and J. B. Kerrison, (Eds.), Walsh and Hoyt’s clinical neuro-ophthalmology, Vol. 1 (6th ed., pp. 575–645). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.
Rizzo, M. and Kellison, I. L. (2004). Eyes, brains, and autos. Archives of Ophthalmology, 122, 641–647.
Rizzo, M., McGehee, D., Dawson, J. and Anderson, S. W. (2001). Simulated car crashes at intersections in drivers with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 15, 10–20.
Rizzo, M., Nawrot, M., Sparks, J. D. and Dawson, J. (2009). First and second order motion perception after focal human brain lesions. Vision Research, 48, 2682–2688.
Rizzo, M., Nawrot, M. and Zihl, J. (1995). Perception of structure from motion and non-motion cues in “central akinetopsia.” Investigative Ophthalmology and Vision Science, 34, 231.
Rizzo, M., Robinson, S. and Neale, V. (2007). The brain in the wild. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.), Neuroergonomics: The brain at work. (pp. 113–128). New York: Oxford University Press.
Rizzo, M., Sparks, J. D., McEvoy, S., Viamonte, S., Kellison, I. and Vecera, S. (2009). Change blindness, aging and cognition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 245–256.
Rizzo, M. and Vecera, S. P. (2002). Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint’s syndrome. Review series: Nosological entities. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 72, 162–178.
Safford, A. S., Hussey, E. A., Parasuraman, R. and Thompson, J. C. (2010). Object-based attentional modulation of biological motion processing: Spatiotemporal dynamics using fMRI and EEG. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 9064–9073.
Salvucci, D. D., Boer, E. R. and Liu, A. (2001). Toward an integrated model of driver behavior in a cognitive architecture. Transportation Research Record, 1779, 9–16.
Scotto Di Cesare, C., Bringoux, L., Bourdin, C., Sarlegna, F. R. and Mestre, D. R. (2011). Spatial localization investigated by continuous pointing during visual and gravitoinertial changes. Exp. Brain Res. 215:173–82. Epub 2011 Oct 11.
Sekuler, A. B., Bennett, P. J. and Mamelak, M. (2000). Effects of aging on the useful field of view. Experimental Aging Research, 26, 103–120.
Shaw, T. H., Warm, J. S., Finomore, V., Tripp, L., Matthews, G., Weiler, E. and Parasuraman, R. (2009). Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance. Neuroscience Letters, 461(3), 207–211.
Simion, F., Regolin, L. and Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 105(2), 809–813.
Spence, C. and Driver, J. (1996). Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1005–1030.
Spence, C. and Driver, J. (1997). Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 1–22.
Strayer, D., and Drews, F. (2007). Cell phone induced driver distraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 128–131.
Tan, H. Z., Gray, R., Young, J. J. and Traylor, R. (2003). A haptic back display for attentional and directional cueing. Haptics-e, 3(1). Downloaded from http://www.haptics-e.org.
Thompson, J. and Parasuraman, R. (2012). Attention, biological motion, and action recognition. NeuroImage, 59, 4–13
Thornton, I. M., Rensink, R. A. and Shiffrar, M. (2002). Active versus passive processing of biological motion, Perception, 31, 837–853.
Ullman, S. (1997). Higher-level vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ungerleider, L. and Mishkin, M. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neurosciences, 6, 414–417.
Vaux, L. M., Ni, R., Rizzo, M., Uc, E. Y. and Anderson, G. J. (2010). Detection of imminent collisions by drivers with Alzheimer’s disease, and Parkinson’s disease: A preliminary study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42, 852–858.
Warm, J. S. and Parasuraman, R. (2007). Cerebral hemodynamics and vigilance. In R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Eds.) Neuroergonomics: The brain at work. (pp. 146–158). New York: Oxford University Press.
Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R. and Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50, 433–451.
Warren, W. H., Jr., Blackwell, A. W. and Morris, M. W. (1989). Age differences in perceiving the direction of self motion from optical flow. Journal of Gerontology, 44, P147–P153.
Weinstein L. F., Previc F. H., Simpson C. G., Lyons T. J. and Gillingham K. K. (1991), A test of thumb and index finger control in overcoming a visual analogue of the giant hand illusion. Aviat Space Environ Med. 62:336–41.
Westmoreland, D., Krell, R. W. and Self, B. P. (2007) Physiological responses to the Coriolis illusion: Effects of head position and vision. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine. 78: 985–9
Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman and R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–101). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159–177.
Wickens, C. D. and McCarley, J. S. (2007). Applied attention theory. New York: CRC Press, Taylor and Francis.
Wiener, E. L. and Attwood, D. A. (1968). Training for vigilance: Combined cueing and KR. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 474–479.
Wood, J., Owsley C. and Rubin, G. (Special issue eds.) (2011). The role of vision in everyday activities. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 31, 201.
Wyszecki, G. and Stiles, W. S. (1982). Color science: concepts and methods, quantitative data and formulae (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.