We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Witness testimony provides the first draft of history and requires a kind of reading that connects descriptions of events from many perspectives and sources. This chapter examines one critical step in that connective process, namely, how to assess a speaker's certainty about the events they describe. By surveying a group of approximately 300 readers and their approximately 28,000 decisions about speaker certainty, this chapter explores how readers may think about factual and counterfactual statements, and how they interpret the certainty with which a witness makes their statements. Ultimately, this chapter argues that readers of collections of witness testimony were more likely to agree about event descriptions when those providing the description were certain, and that readers' abilities to accept gradations of certainty were better when a witness described factual, rather than counterfactual or negated events. These findings lead to a suggestion for how researchers in natural language processing could better model the question of speaker certainty, at least when dealing with the kind of narrative nonfiction one finds in witness testimony.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.