Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T18:08:45.519Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Appendix A - Some basic grammatical terminology and relations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Elissa D. Asp
Affiliation:
Saint Mary's University, Nova Scotia
Jessica de Villiers
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Vancouver
Get access

Summary

GRAMMATICAL TERMINOLOGY

CLAUSES AND UTTERANCES

In Chapter 3 we defined the terms Clause and Utterance (pp. 29–32). Here we include brief definitions for convenient reference. The reader is referred to Chapter 3 (pp. 29–32) for broader definitions and examples.

English Clauses can be defined syntactically as consisting of a verb, its arguments and adjuncts as in Cosmo bit Piper yesterday. Independent clauses can also be used alone to ask a question, make a statement or exclamation, or give a command. A simple Clause is also typically spoken as a single tone group.

Meaningful speech phenomena which do not meet the criteria for clause such as incomplete utterances, minimal responses, idiosyncratic vocalizations, and isolated hesitation fillers are labelled as utterances. Utterance refers to any unit which can be assigned a speech function, and/or has a distinct tone group, and/or is a linguistic signal of ideational, interactional, or organizational information about a speaker's message.

MORPHEMES

Morphemes are smallest contrastive units in the grammar. Rabbit, dog, -s, in dogs, -ed in called, -ity in fatality, un- in unhappy, brush in toothbrush are all morphemes. None of these items can be further analysed. For instance, rabbit refers to a small furry long-eared animal that hops, but no part of rabbit is associated with one of these meanings. Morphemes may or may not be words: Whereas rabbit is a word, -s in dogs signifies plural but does not occur as a word.

Words may be simple (e.g. rabbit/dog), compound (toothbrush) or complex (fatality/unhappy).

Type
Chapter
Information
When Language Breaks Down
Analysing Discourse in Clinical Contexts
, pp. 215 - 221
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×