Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T10:26:32.904Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

20 - Expertise and Transportation

from PART V.A - PROFESSIONAL DOMAINS

Francis T. Durso
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University
Andrew R. Dattel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University
K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Paul J. Feltovich
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Get access

Summary

There are more expert drivers in the United States than any other type of expert. A 35-year-old Los Angelino who commutes an hour to work and travels only minimally on weekends will have spent over 10,000 hours behind the wheel, a number sometimes held up as a threshold for expertise (Chase & Simon, 1973). If, however, instead of a time-based definition, one takes as the criterion for expertise performing a task better than someone with less experience, then our opening assertion about expert drivers in the United States is less apparent.

In the transportation domain, defining expertise in any absolute sense is nontrivial. Self-evaluations, as is often the case, place most drivers as above average (Waylen, Horswill, Alexander, & McKenna, 2004) and are at best weakly correlated with evalua tions of a driving instructor (Groeger & Grande, 1996). If we searched the literature for “highly experienced” operators back to the turn of the last century, we would find a plethora of transportation studies, but they would not inform modern notions of expertise.

Instead, we chose to look at relative differences in experience. Table 20.1 details the participant characteristics for a number of the studies reviewed here, along with one modern-day classification scheme (Hoffman, 1996). Expertise is usually defined by number of years operating the vehicle, or miles driven, or hours flown by the operator. Table 20.2 shows a variety of comparisons in relative experience.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allgaier, E. (1939). Experience counts: It takes more than physical ability to make a good driver. Public Safety, 15, 21.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Bellenkes, A. H., Wickens, C. D., & Kramer, A. F. (1997). Visual scanning and pilot expertise: The role of attentional flexibility and mental model development. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 68, 569–579.Google ScholarPubMed
Bisseret, A. (1971). Analysis of mental processes involved in air traffic control. Ergonomics, 14, 565–570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cellier, J. M., Eyrolle, H., & Marine, C. (1997). Expertise in dynamic environments. Ergonomics, 40, 28–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, P. R., & Underwood, G. (1998). Visual search of driving situations: Danger and experience. Perception, 27, 951–964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crundall, D., & Underwood, G. (1998). The effects of experience and processing demands on visual information acquisition in drivers. Ergonomics, 41, 448–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dattel, A. R., & Sohn, Y. W. (2003). Novice-expert differences in flight situation awareness and action planning as a function of complexity. Proceedings of the 15th Triennial International Ergonomics Association Meeting. Seoul, Korea: The Ergonomics Society of Korea.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, V., & Piette, A. (1970). Analyse de l'activite des operateurs au tableau synoptique d'une chaine d'agglomeration. Le Travail Humain, 33, 341–352.Google Scholar
Doane, S. M., & Sohn, Y. W. (2004). Pilot ability to anticipate the consequences of flight actions as a function of expertise. Human Factors, 46, 92–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doane, S. M., & Sohn, Y. W. (2000). ADAPT: A predictive cognitive model of user visual attention and action planning. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 10, 1–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., Williams, P., & Brown, I. (1991). Components of driving skill: Experience does not mean expertise. Ergonomics, 34, 919–937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, J., Williams, P., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Brown, I. D. (1992). The control of skilled behavior: Learning, intelligence and distraction. In Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S. (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV (pp. 323–341). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Durso, F. T., & Gronlund, S. D. (1999). Situation Awareness. In Nickerson, F. T., Durso, R., Schvaneveldt, R., Dumais, S., Lindsay, S., & Chi, M. (Eds.), The handbook of applied cognition, (pp. 283–314). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Elander, J., West, R., & French, D. (1993). Behavioral correlates of individual differences in road-traffic crash risk: An examination of methods and findings. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 279–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endsley, M. R. (1995). Measurement of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human Factors, 37, 65–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2003). The search for general abilities and basic capacities: Theoretical implications from the modifiability and complexity of mechanisms mediating expert performance. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertice (pp. 93–125). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Toward a general theory of expertise. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, J. A., & Galer, I. A. R. (1984). The effects of decreasing the radius of curvature of convex external rear view mirrors upon drivers' judgments of vehicles approaching in the rearward visual field. Ergonomics, 27, 1209–1224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forsyth, E., Maycock, G., & Sexton, B. (1995). Cohort study of learner and novice drivers: Part 3, Accidents, offences and driving experience in the first three years of driving. Transport and Road Research Laboratory, TRRL Report LR 111, Crowthorne, UK.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Stevens, A. L. (1983). Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Glaser, R. (1987). Thoughts on expertise. In Schooler, C. & Schaie, W. (Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the life course (pp. 81–94). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Groeger, J. A. (2000). Understanding driving: Applying cognitive psychology to a complex everyday task. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Groeger, J. A., & Grande, G. E. (1996). Self-preserving assessments of skills? British Journal of Psychology, 87, 61–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haber, R. N., & Haber, L. (2003). Perception and attention during low-altitude high-speed flight. In Tsang, P. S. & Vidulich, M. A. (Eds.), Principles and practice of aviation psychology (pp. 21–68). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Henmon, V. A. C. (1919). Air service tests of aptitude for flying. Journal of Applied Psychology, 3, 103–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, R. (1996). How can expertise be defined? Implications of research from cognitive psychology. In Williams, R., Faulkner, W., & Fleck, J. (Eds.), Exploring expertise (pp. 81–100). Edinburgh, Scotland: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
Horswill, M. S., & McKenna, F. P. (2004). Drivers' hazard perception ability: Situation awareness on the road. In Banbury, S. & Tremblay, S. (Eds.), A cognitive approach to situation awareness: Theory, measurement, and application. (pp. 155–175) Hampshire, UK: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Klein, G. (1997). An overview of naturalistic decision making applications. In Zsambok, C. E. & Klein, G. (Eds), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 49–60). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McKenna, F. P., & Crick, J. L. (1991). Hazard perception in drivers: A methodology for testing and training. Final Report. Behavioural Studies Unit. Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, UK.Google Scholar
McKenna, F. P., & Farrand, P. (1999). The role of automaticity in driving. In Grayson, G. B. (Ed.), Behavioural research in road safety IX. Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, UK.Google Scholar
McKinney, E. H., & Davis, K. J. (2003). Effects of deliberate practice on crisis decision performance. Human Factors, 45, 436–444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mourant, R. R., & Rockwell, T. H. (1972). Strategies of visual search by novice and experienced drivers. Human Factors, 14, 325–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NTSB (2004). Retrieved April 30, 2004 from the National Transportation Safety Board web cite: www.ntsb.gov.
Randel, J. M., Pugh, H. L., & Reed, S. K. (1996). Differences in expert and novice situation awareness in naturalistic decision making. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 45, 579–597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Recarte, M. A., & Nunes, L. M. (1996). Perception of speed in automobile: Estimation and production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 291–304.Google Scholar
Schvaneveldt, R. W., Beringer, D. B., & Lamonica, J. A. (2001). Priority and organization of information accessed by pilots in various phases of flight. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 11, 253–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schvaneveldt, R. W., Durso, F. T., Goldsmith, T. E., Breen, T. J., Cooke, N. M., Tucker, R. G., & DeMaio, J. C. (1985). Measuring the structure of expertise. The International Journal of Man-Machine Studes, 23, 699–728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seamster, T. L., Redding, R. E., Cannon, J. R., Ryder, J. M., & Purcell, J. A. (1993). Cognitive task analysis of expertise in air traffic control. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3, 257–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanteau, J. (1992). Competence in experts: The role of task characteristics. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 53, 252–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shinar, D., Meir, M., & Ben-Shoham, I. (1998). How automatic is manual gear shifting? Human Factors, 40, 647–654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, A. J. (1926). Tests for transportation pilots. Journal of Applied Psychology, 10, 37–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sohn, Y. W., & Doane, S. M. (2003). Roles of working memory capacity and long-term working memory skill in complex task performance. Memory & Cognition, 31, 458–466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stokes, A. F., Kemper, K., & Kite, K. (1997). Aeronautical decision making, cue recognition, and expertise under time pressure. In Zsambok, C. E. & Klein, G. (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 183–196). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tsang, P. S., & Voss, D. T. (1996). Boundaries of cognitive performance as a function of age and piloting experience. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 6, 359–377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underwood, G., Crundall, D., & Chapman, P. (in press). Cognition and driving. In. Durso, F. T., Nickerson, R., Dumais, S., Lewandowsky, S., & Perfect, T. (Eds.), Handbook of applied cognition (2nd) Wiley: Chicester.Google Scholar
Wasielewski, P. (1984). Speed as a measure of driver risk: Observed speeds versus driver and vehicle characteristics. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 16, 89–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waylen, A. E., Horswill, M. S., Alexander, J. L., & McKenna, F. P. (2004). Do expert drivers have a reduced illusion of superiority? Transportation Research, F, 323–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiegmann, D. A., Goh, J., & O'Hare, D (2002). The role of situation assessment and flight experience in pilots' decisions to continue visual flight rules flight into adverse weather. Human Factors, 44, 187–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiggins, M., & O'Hare, D. (1995). Expertise in aeronautical weather-related decision making: A cross-sectional analysis of general aviation pilots. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1, 305–320.Google Scholar
Wikman, A. S., Nieminen, T., & Summala, H. (1998). Driving experience and time-sharing during in-car tasks on roads of different width. Ergonomics, 41, 358–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, A. F., & O'Neill, B. (1974). On-the-road driving records of licensed race drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 6, 263–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zsambok, C. E., & Klein, G. (1997). Naturalistic decision making. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×