Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T02:51:07.947Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Object and color agnosia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2009

Georg Goldenberg
Affiliation:
Krankenhaus München-Bogenhausen, Germany
Olivier Godefroy
Affiliation:
Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens
Julien Bogousslavsky
Affiliation:
Université de Lausanne, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

The visual agnosias

For seeing persons vision is the major source of object recognition. Although agnosia can affect recognition by other sensory channels (see Chapter 18) the majority of research and clinical interest in object agnosia concerns visual agnosia, which is also the topic of this chapter.

The term agnosia characterizes absent recognition in spite of preserved perception. Clinical diagnosis of visual agnosia is based on a model of visual recognition which was developed at the turning from the nineteenth to the twentieth century (Liepmann, 1908; Lissauer, 1890) but still serves as the starting point for cognitive analyses of visual recognition (Humphreys and Riddoch, 1987; Marr, 1982). This model distinguishes two stages leading from visual perception to recognition. At the “apperceptive” stage, elements of primary visual perceptions, like contours, brightness, texture, and color, are integrated into a coherent structural representation of the object. At the following “associative” stage, the structural representation gains access to semantic memory where knowledge about non-visual properties of the object is stored. Activation of knowledge about the object is equivalent to recognition and a necessary prerequisite for retrieving its name. Agnosia is classified as apperceptive or associative according to which of these stages is affected. Although modern neurophysiology casts doubts on the clean distinction between subsequent stages of visual recognition, the syndromes of “apperceptive agnosia” and “associative agnosia” have proven to correspond to salient and distinguishable clinical syndromes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adler, A. (1944). Disintegration and restoration of optic recognition in visual agnosia. Analysis of a case. Arch. Neurol., 51, 243–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, A. (1950). Course and outcome of visual agnosia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 111, 41–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson, D. F. and Greenberg, J. P. (1969). Visual form agnosia – a specific defect in visual discrimination. Arch. Neurol., 20, 82–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cals, N., Devuyst, G., Afsar, N., Karapanayiotides, T., and Bogousslavsky, J. (2002). Pure superficial posterior cerebral artery territory infarction in The Lausanne Stroke Registry. J. Neurol., 249, 855–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowey, A. and Stoerig, P. (1991). The neurobiology of blindsight. Trends Neurosci., 14, 140–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R., Yamada, T., Damasio, H., Corbett, J. and McKee, J. (1980). Central achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic aspects. Neurology, 30, 1064–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renzi, E. and Saetti, M. C. (1997). Associative agnosia and optic aphasia: Qualitative or quantitative difference?Cortex, 33, 115–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Done, D. J. and Hajilou, B. B. (2004). Loss of high-level perceptual knowledge of object structure in DAT. Neuropsychologia, 43, 60–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freund, C. S. (1889). Ueber optische Aphasie und Seelenblindheit. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 20, 276–97, 371–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gainotti, G. (2004). A metanalysis of impaired and spared naming for different categories of knowledge in patients with a visuo-verbal disconnection. Neuropsychologia, 42, 299–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Silveri, M. C., Villa, G. and Caltagirone, C. (1983). Drawing objects from memory in aphasia. Brain, 106, 613–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gloning, K., Hoff, H. and Tschabitscher, H. (1962). Die Rückbildung der kortikalen Blindheit. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 74, 406–7.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G. (1992). Loss of visual imagery and loss of visual knowledge – a case study, Neuropsychologia, 30, 1081–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (2002a). Goldstein and Gelb's case Schn. – a classic case in neuropsychology? In Classic Cases in Neuropsychology, Vol. 2, ed. Code, C.et al. Hove: Psychology Press, pp. 281–99.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G. (2002b). Neuropsychologie - Grundlagen, Klinik, Rehabilitation, 3. Auflage Urban and Fischer, München.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K. and Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia?Neuropsychologia, 41, 1565–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. and Karlbauer, F. (1998). The more you know the less you can tell: Inhibitory effects of visuo-semantic activation on modality specific visual misnaming, Cortex, 34, 471–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, K. and Gelb, A. (1918). Psychologische Analysen hirnpathologischer Fälle auf Grund von Untersuchungen Hirnverletzter – I. Abhandlung. Zur Psychologie des optischen Wahrnehmungs- und Erkennungsvorganges. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 41, 1–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., Milner, A. D., et al. (1994). The nature and limits of orientation and pattern processing supporting visuomotor control in a visual form agnosic. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 6, 46–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. and Riddoch, M. J. (1987). The fractionation of visual agnosia. In Visual Object Processing: A Cognitive Neuropsychological Approach, ed. Humphreys, G. W. and Riddoch, M. J.. Hove London Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 281–306.Google Scholar
Jellinger, K. A. (2002). Alzheimer disease and cerebrovascular pathology: An update. J. Neural Transm., 109, 813–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landis, T., Graves, R., Benson, D. F. and Hebben, N. (1982). Visual recognition through kinaesthetic mediation. Psychol. Med., 12, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lhermitte, F. and Beauvois, M. F. (1973). A visual-speech disconnection syndrome. Report of a case with optic aphasia, agnosic alexia and colour agnosia. Brain, 96, 695–714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liepmann, H. (1908). Ueber die agnostischen Stoerungen, Neurologisches Centralblatt, 27, 609–17, 664–75.Google Scholar
Lissauer, H. (1890). Ein Fall von Seelenblindheit nebst einem Beitrag zur Theorie derselben. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 21, 222–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information, San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Co.Google Scholar
Meadows, J. C. (1974). Disturbed perception of colours associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain, 97, 615–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, A. D., Perrett, D. I., Johnston, R. S., et al. (1991). Perception and action in "visual form agnosia'. Brain, 114, 405–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plaut, D. C. and Shallice, T. (1993). Perseverative and semantic influences on visual object naming errors in optic aphasia: A connectionist account. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 5, 89–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poeck, K. (1984). Neuropsychological demonstration of splenial interhemispheric disconnection in a case of "optic anomia'. Neuropsychologia, 22, 707–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riddoch, M. J. and Humphreys, G. W. (1987). Visual object processing in optic aphasia: A case of semantic access agnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 4, 131–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, O. (1995). The case of the colorblind painter. In An Anthropologist on Mars. New York: Vintage Books, pp. 3–41.Google Scholar
Schnider, A., Benson, D. F. and Scharre, D. W. (1994). Visual agnosia and optic aphasia: Are they anatomically distinct?Cortex, 30, 445–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sitton, M., Mozer, M. C. and Farah, M. J. (2001). Superadditive effects of multiple lesions in a connectionist architecture: Implications for the neuropsychology of optic aphasia. Psychol. Rev., 107, 709–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparr, S. A., Jay, M., Drislane, F. W. and Venna, N. (1991). A historical case of visual agnosia revisited after 40 years. Brain, 114, 789–800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steeves, J. K. E., Humphrey, G. K., Culham, J. C., et al. (2004). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for a contribution of color and texture information to scene classification in a patient with visual form agnosia. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 16, 955–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teixeira-Ferreira, C., Giusiano, B., Ceccaldi, M. and Poncet, M. (1997). Optic aphasia: Evidence of the contribution of different neural systems to object and action naming. Cortex, 33, 499–514.Google Scholar
Turnbull, O. H., Driver, J. and McCarthy, R. (2004). 2D but not 3D: Pictorial-depth deficits in a case of visual agnosia. Cortex, 40, 723–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B. A. and Davidoff, J. (1993). Partial recovery from visual object agnosia: A 10 year follow-up study. Cortex, 29, 529–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeki, S. (1990). A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain, 113, 1721–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×