Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T22:01:40.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Intuitive Psychology as Mind Designer: Scaffolding Cognitive Novelties in Early Childhood

from Part III - Language and Communicative Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamson, L. B. 1995. Communication development during infancy. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. 2010. Mindreaders. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.) 2013. Navigating the social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. 1976. Language and context. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. 2011. Children’s thinking. Belmont: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. and Pellegrini, A. 2002. The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 1994. Grounds for cognition. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 1997. Interpreting minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2000. Minding minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2001. Developing mental abilities by representing intentionality, Synthese, 129, 233258.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2007. Inside loops, Synthese, 159, 235252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2009. Predicative minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2010. Our own minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. 2013. Mindvaults. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1983. Child’s talk. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. and Gergely, G. 2013. Teleological understanding of actions. In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 3743). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. 1971. Intentional systems. Journal of Philosophy 8, 87106.Google Scholar
Doherty, M. 2009. Theory of mind. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. and Csibra, G. 2003. Teleological reasoning in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Science 7, 287292.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Wellman, H. 1992. Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language 7, 145171.Google Scholar
Grice, P. 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review 66, 377388.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. 1993. Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Howhy, J. 2014. The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond modularity. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. 2013. Origins of social cognition. In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 139144). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. and Sperber, D. 2011. Why do humans reason? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34, 57111.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. 1996. Language in cognitive development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. 2007. Young minds in social worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. 1991. Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Moll, H.. 2013. Why don’t apes understand false belief? In Banaji, M. R. and Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world. (pp. 8187). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. 1993. The self born in intersubjectivity. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), The perceived self. (pp. 121173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. 2011. The generation of human meaning. In Seemann, A. (Ed.), Joint attention. (pp. 73135). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×