Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T12:40:47.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Learning to Read Hebrew

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2017

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Ahmad, H. (2015). Learning to read Arabic syllabically versus alphabetically: psycholinguistics, grammatological and pedagogical aspects. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Aram, D. (2005). Continuity in children’s literacy achievements: A longitudinal perspective from kindergarten to school. First Language, 3, 259289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aram, D. & Levin, I. (2004). The role of maternal mediation of writing to kindergartners in promoting literacy achievements in second grade: A longitudinal perspective. Reading and Writing, 17, 387409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Kochva, I. (2013). What are the underlying skills of silent reading acquisition? A developmental study from kindergarten to the 2nd grade. Reading and Writing, 26, 14171436.Google Scholar
Bar-Kochva, I. & Breznitz, Z. (2014). Reading scripts that differ in orthographic transparency: A within-participant-and-language investigation of underlying skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 1227.Google Scholar
Bar-On, A. (forthcoming). kriya betzel hahomografia: haba’aya hi gam hapitaron [Reading in the shadow of homography: The problem is the solution]. Safa Ve’Oriyanut. [In Hebrew]Google Scholar
Bar-On, A. & Ravid, D. (2011). Morphological analysis in learning to read psseudowords in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 553581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S. & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonologic, and morphologic skills in reading disabled and normal children: Evidence from perception and production of spoken Hebrew. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 876893.Google Scholar
Bentin, S., Deutsch, A. & Liberman, I. Y. (1990). Syntactic competence and reading ability in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 147172.Google Scholar
Bentin, S. & Leshem, H. (1993). On the interaction of phonologic awareness and reading acquisition: It’s a two-way street. Psychological Science, 2, 271274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benuck, M. B. & Peverly, S. T. (2004). The effect of orthographic depth on reliance upon semantic context for oral reading in English and Hebrew. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 281299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. (1985). Acquisition of Hebrew. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. I (pp. 255371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (1997). Modern Hebrew. In Hetzron, R. (Ed.), The semitic languages (pp. 312333). Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (2003). Children’s lexical innovations: Developmental perspectives on Hebrew verb-structure. In Shimron, J. (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Semitic root-based morphology (pp. 243291). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Beveridge, M. E. L. & Bak, T. H. (2012). Beyond one-way streets: The interaction of phonology, morphology, and culture with orthography. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 1819.Google Scholar
Breznitz, Z. (1997). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 289297.Google Scholar
Chomsky, W. (1941). The history of our vowel-system in Hebrew. The Jewish Quarterly Review, 32, 2749.Google Scholar
Cohen-Mimran, R. (2009). The contribution of language skills to reading fluency: A comparison of two orthographies for Hebrew. Journal of Child Language, 36, 657672.Google Scholar
Daniels, P. T. (1990). Fundamentals of grammatology. Journal of the American Oriental Society, 110, 727731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, P. T. & Bright, W. (Eds.) (1996). The world’s writing systems. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Diringer, D. (1948). The Alphabet: A Key to the History of Mankind. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Evans, N. & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Science 32, 429492.Google Scholar
Feitelson, D. (1988). Facts and fads in beginning reading. New York: Ablex.Google Scholar
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263279.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Siegelman, N., Narkiss, A. & Afek, L. (2013). What predicts successful literacy acquisition in a second language? Psychological Science, 24, 12431252.Google Scholar
Geva, E. (2008). Facets of metalinguistic awareness related to reading development in Hebrew: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children. In Koda, K. & Zehler, A. (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 154187). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gillis, S. & Ravid, D. (2006). Typological effects on spelling development: A crosslinguistic study of Hebrew and Dutch. Journal of Child Language, 33, 621659.Google Scholar
Glinert, L. H. (1994). Israeli Hebrew. In Asher, R. E. & Simpson, J. M. Y. (Eds.), The encyclopedia of language and linguistics (pp. 15381541). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Gnanadesikan, A. E. (2009). The writing revolution: Cuneiform to the internet. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Goerwitz, R. L. (1996). The Jewish scripts. In Daniels, P. T. & Bright, W. (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 489494). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gur, T. (2005). Reading Hebrew vowel diacritics: A longitudinal investigation from Grade 1 to Grade 3. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 33, 61135.Google Scholar
Hetzron, R. (1987). Hebrew. In Comrie, B. (Ed.), The world’s major languages (pp. 578593). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Hoberman, R. D. & Aronoff, M. (2003). The verb morphology of Maltese: From Semitic to Romance. In Shimron, J. (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Sematic, root-based, morphology (pp. 6178). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Katzir, T., Schiff, R. & Kim, Y.-S. (2012). The effects of orthographic consistency on reading development: A within and between cross-linguistic study of fluency and accuracy among fourth grade English- and Hebrew-speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 673679.Google Scholar
Korat, O. (2005). Contextual and non-contextual knowledge in emergent literacy development: A comparison between children from low SES and middle SES communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 220238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamm, O. & Epstein, R. (1994). Dichotic listening performance under high and low lexical work load in subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 32, 757785.Google Scholar
Lapidot, M., Tubul, G. & Wohl, A. (1995–1996). Mivchan eranut fonologit kekli nibui lerechishat hakri’a [A test of phonological awareness as a predictor of reading acquisition]. Chelkat Lashon, 1920, 169188. [In Hebrew]Google Scholar
Leikin, M., Schwartz, M. & Tobin, Y. (2011). Current issues in bilingualism: Cognitive and socio-linguistic perspectives. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Levin, I. (1997). Hitpatxut haktiva hamukdemet be’Ivrit [The early development of writing in Hebrew]. In Shimron, J. (Ed.), Mexkarim bepsixologia shel halashon beyisrael: rexishat lashon, kriya vektiva [Studies in the psychology of language in Israel: Language acquisition, reading and writing]. Jerusalem: Magnus. [In Hebrew]Google Scholar
Levin, I., Aram, D., Tolchinsky, L. & McBride, C. (2013). Maternal mediation of writing and children’s early spelling and reading: The Semitic abjad versus the European alphabet. Writing Systems Research, 5, 134155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, I., Korat, O. & Amsterdamer, P. (1996). Emergent writing among kindergartners: Cross-linguistic commonalities and Hebrew-specific issues. In Rijlaarsdam, G., van der Bergh, H. & Couzijn, M. (Eds.), Current trends in writing research: Theories, models and methodology (pp. 398419). Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. J. (1981). A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 373418.Google Scholar
Miller, P. (2009). Learning in a world of silence: Insight from 30 years of work and research with deaf and hard of hearing children. In Aram, D. & Korat, O. (Eds.), Literacy and language: Interactions, bilingualism, and difficulties (pp. 471491). Jerusalem: Maganes Press.Google Scholar
Naveh, J. (1982). Early history of the alphabet: An introduction to West Semitic epigraphy and palaeography. Jerusalem: Magnes Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In Verhoeven, L., Elbro, C. & Reitsma, P. (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189213). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D. (1990). Internal structure constraints on new-word formation devices in Modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica, 24, 289346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D. (1996). Accessing the mental lexicon: Evidence from incompatibility between representation of spoken and written morphology. Linguistics, 34, 12191246.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (2001). Learning to spell in Hebrew: Phonological and morphological factors. Reading and Writing, 14, 459485.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (2012). Spelling morphology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Bar-On, A. (2005). Manipulating written Hebrew roots across development: The interface of semantic, phonological, and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing, 18, 231256.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Kubi, E. (2003). What is a spelling error? The discrepancy between perception and reality. Faits de Langues, 22, 8798.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Mashraki, Y. E. (2007). Prosodic reading, reading comprehension and morphological skills in Hebrew-speaking fourth graders. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 140150.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Schiff, R. (2006). Roots and patterns in Hebrew language development: Evidence from written morphological analogies. Reading and Writing, 19, 789818.Google Scholar
Ricketts, J., Nation, K. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 235257.Google Scholar
Rogers, H. (1995). Optimal orthographies. In Taylor, I. & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Scripts and Literacy (pp. 3143). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007). Epilinguistic and metalinguistic phonological awareness may be subject to different constraints: Evidence from Hebrew. First Language, 27, 385405.Google Scholar
Schiff, R., Katzir, T. & Shoshan, N. (2013). Reading accuracy and speed of vowelized and unvowelized scripts among dyslexic readers of Hebrew: The road not taken. Annals of Dyslexia, 63, 171185.Google Scholar
Schiff, R. & Lotem, M. (2011). Effects of phonological and morphological awareness on children’s word reading development from two socioeconomic backgrounds. First Language, 31, 139163.Google Scholar
Schiff, R., Schwartz-Nahshon, S. & Nagar, R. (2011). Effect of phonological and morphological awareness on reading comprehension in Hebrew-speaking adolescents with reading disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 4463.Google Scholar
Schwarzwald, O. R. (2002). Modern Hebrew morphology. Tel Aviv: Open University. [In Hebrew]Google Scholar
Shany, M., Bar-On, A. & Katzir, T. (2012). Reading different orthographic structures in the shallow-pointed Hebrew script: A cross-grade study in elementary school. Reading and Writing, 25, 12171238.Google Scholar
Shany, M. & Breznitz, Z. (2011). Rate- and accuracy-disabled subtype profiles among adults with dyslexia in the Hebrew orthography. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37, 889913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shany, M. & Share, D. L. (2011). Subtypes of reading disability in a shallow orthography: A double dissociation between accuracy-disabled and rate-disabled reading in Hebrew. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 6484.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2003). Dyslexia in Hebrew. In Goulandris, N. (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 208234). London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of over-reliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584616.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. (2012). Frost and fogs, or sunny skies? Orthography, reading, and misplaced optimalism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 4546.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting among literate and pre-literate Hebrew-speakers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182202.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. & Gur, T. (in preparation). When less is more: Declining code knowledge signals progress in reading in a Semitic abjad.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In Harris, M. & Hatano, G. (Eds.), Learning to read and write (pp. 89111). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Share, D. L. & Shalev, C. (2004). Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Reading and Writing, 17, 131.Google Scholar
Shatil, E. & Share, D. L. (2003). Cognitive antecedents of early reading ability: A test of the modularity hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatil, E., Share, D. L. & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten writing to Grade 1 literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 125.Google Scholar
Shimron, J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 5267.Google Scholar
Shimron, J. & Sivan, T. (1994). Reading proficiency and orthography: Evidence from Hebrew and English. Language Learning, 44, 527.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360406.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Tolchinsky, L., Levin, I., Aram, D. & McBride-Chang, C. (2012). Building literacy in alphabetic, abjad and morphosyllabic systems. Reading and Writing, 25, 15731598.Google Scholar
Tolchinsky, L. & Teberosky, A. (1998). The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development, 13, 114.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E. & Chapman, J. W. (2012). Does the set for variability mediate the influence of vocabulary knowledged on the development of word recognition skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 122140.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×