Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T00:53:34.728Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - A History of Primates Studying Primates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2022

Bennett L. Schwartz
Affiliation:
Florida International University
Michael J. Beran
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
Get access

Summary

When George John Romanes published his book Animal Intelligence in 1882, marking for many the beginning of the study of comparative cognition, he devoted chapter 17 to the mental life of “Monkeys, Apes, and Baboons.” His subsequent experiments on the mental powers of zoo chimpanzees would be published in Science (Romanes, 1889) and Nature (Romanes, 1890). Around the turn of the last century, Edward Thorndike of Columbia University and A. J. Kinnaman of Clark University conducted laboratory studies on the intelligence of capuchin and rhesus monkeys, respectively. Thus, from psychology’s earliest days as an experimental science, nonhuman primate cognition was a focus of interest and investigation. Many of the best-known and most influential psychologists in history – names like Watson, Yerkes, Köhler, Harlow, Lashley, Hebb, Premack, Rumbaugh, and countless others – made significant contributions to our understanding of learning and cognition through studies of monkeys and apes. Although the history of cognitive research with nonhuman primates is too long and too rich to be reviewed exhaustively in one chapter, the present introduction will highlight significant research themes and important milestones across the history of primate cognition research. The goal of this review is to show how contemporary comparative cognition research is intricately connected to its history, and how our understanding of primate cognition shows cumulative progress across the last 140 years of inquiry.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2015). Mirror self-recognition: A review and critique of attempts to promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates, 56, 317326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastian, H. C. (1896). The brain as an organ of mind. D. Appleton & Company.Google Scholar
Butler, R. A. & Harlow, H. F. (1956) The effects of auditory distraction on the performance of monkeys, The Journal of General Psychology, 54, 1520.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and Selection in relation to sex, Vol 1. John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. John Murray.Google Scholar
Davis, H., & Perusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 561615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Waal, F. B. (2005). A century of getting to know the chimpanzee. Nature, 437, 5659.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (1984). Comparative psychology in the twentieth century. Hutchinson Ross Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (2006). Monkey farm: A history of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology, Orange Park, Florida, 1930–1965. Bucknell University Press.Google Scholar
Dewsbury, D. A. (2009). Samuel Fernberger’s rejected doctoral dissertation: A neglected resource for the history of ape research in America. History of Psychology, 12, 16. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014893Google Scholar
Fedigan, L. M., & Strum, S. C. (1999). A brief history of primate studies: National traditions, disciplinary origins, and stages in North American field research. In Dolhinow, P. & Fuentes, A. (Eds.), The nonhuman primates (pp. 258269). Mayfield Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Foley, J. P. Jr., & Warden, C. J. (1934). The effect of practice on the delayed reaction in the rhesus monkey. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 44, 390413.Google Scholar
Franz, S. I. (1913). Observations of the preferential use of the right and left hands by monkeys. Journal of Animal Behavior, 3, 140144.Google Scholar
Fridman, E. P., & Bowden, D. M. (2009). The Russian primate research center – a survivor. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 48, 69.Google Scholar
Furness, W. H. (1916). Observations on the mentality of chimpanzees and orangutans. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 55, 281290.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr., & Anderson, J. R. (2020). Self-recognition in animals: Where do we stand 50 years later? Lessons from cleaner wrasse and other species. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7, 4658.Google Scholar
Garner, R. L. (1892). The speech of monkeys. Charles L. Webster and Company.Google Scholar
Garner, R. L. (1900). Apes and monkeys, their life and language. Ginn & Co.Google Scholar
Grether, W. F., & Maslow, A. H. (1937). An experimental study of insight in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24(1), 127134.Google Scholar
Haggerty, M. E. (1909). Imitation in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 19, 337455.Google Scholar
Haggerty, M. E. (1910). Preliminary experiments on anthropoid apes. Psychological Bulletin, 7, 49.Google Scholar
Hamilton, G. V. (1911). A study of trial and error reactions in mammals. Journal of Animal Behavior, 1, 3366.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 5165.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Meyer, D. R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulation drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 228234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F., Uehling, H., & Maslow, A. H. (1932). Comparative behavior of primates I: Delayed reaction tests on primates from the lemur to the orang-outan. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 13, 313343.Google Scholar
Heymann, E. W., & Ganzhorn, J. U. (2015). History of primate behavioural and ecological field research at the German Primate Center. Primate Biology, 2, 7380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillix, W. A., & Rumbaugh, D. (2004). Animal bodies, human minds: Ape, dolphin, and parrot language skills. Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1915). Mind in evolution. Macmillan and Company.Google Scholar
Holmberg, E. L. (1893). La facultad de comparacion en los monos [The faculty of comparison in the monkeys]. Revista del Jardin Zoologico de Buenos Aires, 1, 254256.Google Scholar
Holmes, S. J. (1916). Studies in animal behavior. RG Badger.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. (1991). History of the Arashiyama Japanese. In Fedigan, L. M. & Asquith, P. J. (Eds.), The monkeys of Arashiyama: Thirty-five years of research in Japan and the West (pp. 2153). State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1877). On the natural history of the man-like apes. In Huxley, T. H. (Ed.), Evidence a to man’s place in nature (pp. 970). D Appleton & Company.Google Scholar
Imanishi, K. (1957). Identification: A process of enculturation in the subhuman society of Macaca fuscata. Primates, 1, 129.Google Scholar
Itani, J. (1954). The nomadism of the wild Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata fuscata, in Takasaki Yama. Japanese Journal of Ecology, 4, 2228.Google Scholar
Jackson, T. A. (1942). Use of the stick as a tool by young chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 34(2), 223235.Google Scholar
Johnson, H. M. (1914). Visual pattern-discrimination in the vertebrates II: Comparative visual acuity in the dog, the monkey and the chick. Journal of Animal Behavior, 4, 340361.Google Scholar
Kellogg, W. N. & Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The ape and the child. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Kempf, E. J. (1916). Two methods of subjective learning in the monkey Macacus Rhesus. Journal of Animal Behavior, 6, 256265.Google Scholar
Kinnaman, A. J. (1902a). Mental life of two Macacus rhesus monkeys in captivity. I. The American Journal of Psychology, 13, 98148.Google Scholar
Kinnaman, A. J. (1902b). Mental life of two Macacus rhesus monkeys in captivity. II. The American Journal of Psychology, 13, 173218.Google Scholar
Klüver, H. (1933). Behavior mechanisms in monkeys. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1957). The mentality of apes (Winter, E., Trans.). Penguin Paperback. Original work published, 1925.Google Scholar
Krause, M. A., & Beran, M. J. (2020). Words matter: Reflections on language projects with chimpanzees and their implications. American Journal of Primatology, 82, e23187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuroda, A. (1926). Mental life of a Macacus monkey. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 6, 148174.Google Scholar
Kuroda, A. (1931). On the counting ability of a monkey (Macacus cynomologus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 171180.Google Scholar
Ladygina-Kohts, N. N. (1935/2002). Infant chimpanzee and human child: A classic 1935 comparative study of ape emotions and intelligence. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lück, H. E., & Jaeger, S. (1988). Wolfgang Köhler and the Anthropoid Research Station on Tenerife island. Revista de Historia de la Psicología, 9, 295308.Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D. E. (2003). Primate psychology. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, C. L. (1903). Introduction to comparative psychology. Scribner.Google Scholar
Müller, F. M. (1887). The science of thought. Charles Scribner’s Sons.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (2002). Wolfgang Köhler. Biographical Memoirs, 81, 81.Google Scholar
Papini, M. R. (1988). Influence of evolutionary biology in the early development of experimental psychology in Argentina (1891–1930). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2.Google Scholar
Rawlins, R. G., & Kessler, M. J. (Eds.). (1986). The Cayo Santiago macaques: History, behavior, and biology. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Révész, G. (1925). Experimental study in abstraction in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 5, 293343.Google Scholar
Rilling, M. (1993). Invisible counting animals: A history of contributions from comparative psychology, ethology, and learning theory. In Boysen, S. T. & Capaldi, E. J. (Eds.), Comparative cognition and neuroscience. The development of numerical competence: Animal and human models (pp. 337). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Roginsky, G. S. (1939). On the comparative psychology of monkeys. Trudy Gosudarstvennogo Instituta Po Izucheniëiìu Mozga Imeni V. M. Bekhtereva, 11, 99105.Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1882). Animal intelligence. D. Appleton.Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1883). Mental evolution in animals: With a posthumous essay on instinct by Charles Darwin. Kegan Paul, Trench.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1888). Mental evolution in man: Origin of human faculty. Kegan Paul, Trench.Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1889). The mental powers of the chimpanzee. Science, 14, 215.Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1890). Intelligence of chimpanzees. Nature, 42, 245246.Google Scholar
Ruch, T. C., & Fulton, J. F. (1942). Growth of primate literature since 1800. Science, 95, 4748.Google Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M., & Rice, C. P. (1962). Learning-set formation in young great apes. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 866868.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M., Call, J., & Boesch, C. (Eds.). (2013). Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shepherd, W. (1910). Some mental processes of the rhesus monkey. The Psychological Monographs, 12, i-65.Google Scholar
Shepherd, W. T. (1915). Some observations on the intelligence of the chimpanzee. Journal of Animal Behavior, 5, 391396.Google Scholar
Sidman, M. (1958). By-products of aversive control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 265280. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1958.1-265Google Scholar
Sidman, M., Herrnstein, R. J., & Conrad, D. G. (1957). Maintenance of avoidance behavior by unavoidable shocks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 553557.Google Scholar
Small, W. S. (1901). Experimental study of the mental processes of the rat. II. The American Journal of Psychology,12, 206239.Google Scholar
Teuber, M. L. (1994). The founding of the primate station, Tenerife, Canary Islands. The American Journal of Psychology, 107, 551581.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 2, i-109.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1901). The mental life of the monkeys. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 3(5), i-51.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Warden, C. J., Fjeld, H. A., & Koch, A. M. (1940). Imitative behavior in cebus and rhesus monkeys. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 56, 311322. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1940.10534505Google Scholar
Warden, C. J., & Jackson, T. A. (1935). Imitative behavior in the rhesus monkey. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 46, 103125.Google Scholar
Washburn, M. F. (1908, 1936). The animal mind: A text-book of comparative psychology. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1909). Some experiments bearing upon color vision in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 19, 128.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1910). Reviewed work: Imitation in Monkeys by M. E. Haggerty. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 7, 357359.Google Scholar
Weir, J. (1899). The dawn of reason: Or, mental traits in the lower animals. Macmillan.Google Scholar
White, L. A. (1942). On the use of tools by primates. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 34, 369374.Google Scholar
Witmer, L. (1910). Intelligent imitation and curiosity in a monkey. The Psychological Clinic, 3, 224227.Google Scholar
Witmer, L. (1909). A monkey with a mind. The Psychological Clinic, 3, 179205.Google Scholar
Woodrow, H. (1929). Discrimination by the monkey of temporal sequences of varying number of stimuli. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 9, 123157.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. M. (1896). Lectures on human and animal psychology. Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
Yamagiwa, J. (2010). Research history of Japanese macaques in Japan. In Nakagawa, N., Nakamichi, M., & Sugiura, H., H. (Eds.), The Japanese macaques (pp. 325). Springer.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R.M. (1916a). The mental life of monkeys and apes: A study of ideational behavior. Behavior Monographs, 3, 1145.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1916b). Provision for the study of monkeys and apes. Science, 43, 231234.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1927). A program of anthropoid research. The American Journal of Psychology, 39, 181199.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1932). Robert Mearns Yerkes. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), International university series in psychology: A history of psychology in autobiography, Vol. 2 (pp. 381407). Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1935). Yale laboratories of primate biology, incorporated. Science, 82, 618620.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×